首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Deficits in smooth pursuit eye movements are well documented in schizophrenia and schizotypic psychopathology. The status of eye tracking dysfunction (ETD) as an endophenotype for schizophrenia liability is relatively robust. However, the relation of ETD to schizophrenia-related deviance in the general population has not been confirmed. This study examined smooth pursuit eye tracking and schizotypal personality features in the general population. Smooth pursuit eye movement and schizotypal features were measured in 300 adult community subjects. The sample included both sexes, subjects with a wide age and educational range, and subjects with no prior history of psychosis. Primary outcome measures were peak gain (eye velocity/target velocity), catch-up saccade rate, and schizotypal feature scores. Total schizotypal features were significantly associated with decreased peak gain and were associated at the trend level with increased catch-up saccade rate. These associations were essentially unchanged after controlling for age, sex, and intellectual level effects. These data confirm a hypothesized association between schizotypal features and poorer eye tracking performance (principally, peak gain) in the general population as well as support the conceptualization of ETD as an endophenotype for schizophrenia liability. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
When 2 targets for pursuit eye movements move in different directions, the eye velocity follows the vector average (S. G. Lisberger & V. P. Ferrera, 1997). The present study investigates the mechanisms of target selection when observers are instructed to follow a predefined horizontal target and to ignore a moving distractor stimulus. Results show that at 140 ms after distractor onset, horizontal eye velocity is decreased by about 25%. Vertical eye velocity increases or decreases by 1°/s in the direction opposite from the distractor. This deviation varies in size with distractor direction, velocity, and contrast. The effect was present during the initiation and steady-state tracking phase of pursuit but only when the observer had prior information about target motion. Neither vector averaging nor winner-take-all models could predict the response to a moving to-be-ignored distractor during steady-state tracking of a predefined target. The contributions of perceptual mislocalization and spatial attention to the vertical deviation in pursuit are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Investigated temporal factors in vision in relation to the delay of the retinal feedback of ocular movements in eye tracking. A hybrid real-time computer system and dynamic programing methods were used to calibrate photoelectric eye-movement transducers in viewing visual targets, to yoke these targets to eye motion, to introduce feedback delays in eye-movement-retinal interaction, and to measure error in eye tracking. Results indicate that feedback delay affected the accuracy of both the compensatory and the pursuit tracking in a significant way, with a somewhat greater effect being found for pursuit movements. Since delay reduced smooth pursuit motions to saccadic reactions that varied in size with the delay interval, it is suggested that ocular dynamics and guidance in space perception are governed by time-specific neuron mechanisms of the central visual system. Findings negate classical theory of ocular dynamics and perception of direction by proving that directional guidance of the eyes is determined by directional specificity and temporal specificity of the feedback processes of pursuit and saccadic movements of the eyes and is not caused primarily by learned temporal association between visual and tactual sensory processes. It is concluded that major disabilities and distortions in vision, which are not reducible to traditionally defined optometric and ophthalmologic factors, may be produced by built-in developmental perturbations of ocular feedback timing. Findings emphasize dynamic optometric measurements in understanding common and elusive distortions of visual perception. (19 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Eye movement impairment and schizotypal psychopathology   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
OBJECTIVE: Eye movement dysfunction in relation to a smooth pursuit task has been documented in schizophrenic patients and in patients with the related personality disorder, schizotypal personality disorder. To investigate which quantitative measures are associated with the eye movement dysfunction and whether the dysfunction is more related to the psychotic-like or the deficit-like symptoms of schizotypal personality disorder, ratings of eye movements in several groups of subjects were compared. METHOD: The study groups consisted of 26 patients with schizotypal personality disorder, 42 patients with other personality disorders (22 who also had two or more schizotypal personality traits and 20 who had fewer than two), and 37 normal comparison subjects. Smooth pursuit eye tracking of sinusoidal and constant velocity targets was recorded by an infrared eye tracking system. Two raters evaluated pursuit gain and large and small saccades in the direction of the target and in the direction opposite to that of the target (quantitative ratings) and constant velocity (qualitative rating). RESULTS: Patients with schizotypal personality disorder and patients with other personality disorders and two or more schizotypal traits, but not those with fewer than two schizotypal traits, had significantly poorer qualitative ratings of tracking than the normal comparison subjects. Neither gain nor any of the saccadic measures significantly differed between groups. The number of large saccades in the direction of the target was the only quantitative variable that predicted low qualitative ratings. Qualitatively poor tracking was associated with the deficit-like, but not the psychotic-like, symptoms of schizotypal personality disorder. CONCLUSIONS: Patients with schizotypal personality disorder demonstrate qualitatively poorer tracking than comparison groups, and the impaired tracking is associated with deficit-like symptoms.  相似文献   

5.
1. Our goal was to assess whether visual motion signals related to changes in image velocity contribute to pursuit eye movements. We recorded the smooth eye movements evoked by ramp target motion at constant speed. In two different kinds of stimuli, the onset of target motion provided either an abrupt, step change in target velocity or a smooth target acceleration that lasted 125 ms followed by prolonged target motion at constant velocity. We measured the eye acceleration in the first 100 ms of pursuit. Because of the 100-ms latency from the onset of visual stimuli to the onset of smooth eye movement, the eye acceleration in this 100-ms interval provides an estimate of the open-loop response of the visuomotor pathways that drive pursuit. 2. For steps of target velocity, eye acceleration in the first 100 ms of pursuit depended on the "motion onset delay," defined as the interval between the appearance of the target and the onset of motion. If the motion onset delay was > 100 ms, then the initial eye movement consisted of separable early and late phases of eye acceleration. The early phase dominated eye acceleration in the interval from 0 to 40 ms after pursuit onset and was relatively insensitive to image speed. The late phase dominated eye acceleration in the interval 40-100 ms after the onset of pursuit and had an amplitude that was proportional to image speed. If there was no delay between the appearance of the target and the onset of its motion, then the early component was not seen, and eye acceleration was related to target speed throughout the first 100 ms of pursuit. 3. For step changes of target velocity, the relationship between eye acceleration in the first 40 ms of pursuit and target velocity saturated at target speeds > 10 degrees /s. In contrast, the relationship was nearly linear when eye acceleration was measured in the interval 40-100 ms after the onset of pursuit. We suggest that the first 40 ms of pursuit are driven by a transient visual motion input that is related to the onset of target motion (motion onset transient component) and that the next 60 ms are driven by a sustained visual motion input (image velocity component). 4. When the target accelerated smoothly for 125 ms before moving at constant speed, the initiation of pursuit resembled that evoked by steps of target velocity. However, the latency of pursuit was consistently longer for smooth target accelerations than for steps of target velocity.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

6.
The effect of attention on smooth pursuit and saccadic tracking was studied in infants at 8, 14, 20, and 26 weeks of age. A small rectangle was presented moving in a sinusoidal pattern in either the horizontal or vertical direction. Attention level was distinguished with a recording of heart rate. There was an increase across age in overall tracking, the gain of the smooth pursuit eye movements, and an increase in the amplitude of compensatory saccades at faster tracking speeds. One age change was an increase in the preservation of smooth pursuit tracking ability as stimulus speed increased. A second change was the increasing tendency during attentive tracking to shift from smooth pursuit to saccadic tracking when the stimulus speed increased to the highest velocities. This study shows that the development of smooth pursuit and targeted saccadic eye movements is closely related to the development of sustained attention in this age range. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
In real-life situations, such as during locomotion, or while driving a vehicle, it is necessary to maintain visual fixation and tracking in the presence of the visual flow of the surroundings, which represents a potentially adequate stimulus for the elicitation of optokinetic nystagmus. The present study is concerned with the influence of vestibular disorders, whether pathological or experimentally induced, on those cortically controlled fixation mechanisms, predominantly in the smooth pursuit system, which are involved in suppressing optokinetic information. The study examines the possibility of obtaining an objective measure to assist in counselling patients with unilateral vestibular loss on their vehicle driving ability. To this end, the influence of optokinetic and vestibular stimulation on the execution of smooth pursuit target tracking was measured by recording eye movements during a combination of standard pursuit tasks (0.25, 0.5 and 1 Hz sinusoidal) against standard optokinetic striped backgrounds (0, 30 and 60 degrees/sec). The influence of vestibular imbalance, induced in healthy subjects (n = 35) by unilateral caloric irrigation, and caused by unilateral vestibular loss (in five patients), was also examined under these conditions. During induced vestibular imbalance in normal subjects, and to a greater extent in the tested patients, significant deficits in smooth pursuit gain and increases in saccade frequency were observed during target pursuit against an optokinetic background. Moreover, the findings indicate that the most sensitive parameter for the influence of vestibular optokinetic stimuli on smooth pursuit is frequency of saccades, rather than the gain factor. The tests described here are appropriate for clinical and medico-legal assessment of the influence of vestibular disorder on vehicle driving.  相似文献   

8.
The floccular lobe of the monkey is critical for the generation of visually-guided smooth eye movements. The present experiments reveal physiological correlates of the directional organization in the primate floccular lobe by examining the selectivity for direction of eye motion and visual stimulation in the firing of individual Purkinje cells (PCs) and mossy fibers. During tracking of sinusoidal target motion along different axes in the frontoparallel plane, PCs fell into two classes based on the axis that caused the largest modulation of simple-spike firing rate. For "horizontal" PCs, the response was maximal during horizontal eye movements, with increases in firing rate during pursuit toward the side of recording (ipsiversive). For "vertical" PCs, the response was maximal during eye movement along an axis just off pure vertical, with increases in firing rate during pursuit directed downward and slightly contraversive. During pursuit of target motion at constant velocity, PCs again fell into horizontal and vertical classes that matched the results from sinusoidal tracking. In addition, the directional tuning of the sustained "eye velocity" and transient "visual" components of the neural responses obtained during constant velocity tracking were very similar. PCs displayed very broad tuning approximating a cosine tuning curve; the mean half-maximum bandwidth of their tuning curves was 170-180 degrees. Other cerebellar elements, related purely to eye movement and presumed to be mossy fibers, exhibited tuning approximately 40 degrees narrower than PCs and had best directions that clustered around the four cardinal directions. Our data indicate that the motion signals encoded by PCs in the monkey floccular lobe are segregated into channels that are consistent with a coordinate system defined by the vestibular apparatus and eye muscles. The differences between the tuning properties exhibited by PCs compared with mossy fibers indicate that a spatial transformation occurs within the floccular lobe.  相似文献   

9.
Delays in the transmission of sensory and motor information prevent errors from being instantaneously available to the central nervous system (CNS) and can reduce the stability of a closed-loop control strategy. On the other hand, the use of a pure feedforward control (inverse dynamics) requires a perfect knowledge of the dynamic behavior of the body and of manipulated objects. Sensory feedback is essential both to accommodate unexpected errors and events and to compensate for uncertainties about the dynamics of the body. Experimental observations concerning the control of posture, gaze and limbs have shown that the CNS certainly uses a combination of closed-loop and open-loop control. Feedforward components of movement, such as eye saccades, occur intermittently and present a stereotyped kinematic profile. In visuo-manual tracking tasks, hand movements exhibit velocity peaks that occur intermittently. When a delay or a slow dynamics are inserted in the visuo-manual control loop, intermittent step-and-hold movements appear clearly in the hand trajectory. In this study, we investigated strategies used by human subjects involved in the control of a particular dynamic system. We found strong evidence for substantial nonlinearities in the commands produced. The presence of step-and-hold movements seemed to be the major source of nonlinearities in the control loop. Furthermore, the stereotyped ballistic-like kinematics of these rapid and corrective movements suggests that they were produced in an open-loop way by the CNS. We analyzed the generation of ballistic movements in the light of sliding control theory assuming that they occurred when a sliding variable exceeded a constant threshold. In this framework, a sliding variable is defined as a composite variable (a combination of the instantaneous tracking error and its temporal derivatives) that fulfills a specific stability criterion. Based on this hypothesis and on the assumption of a constant reaction time, the tracking error and its derivatives should be correlated at a particular time lag before movement onset. A peak of correlation was found for a physiologically plausible reaction time, corresponding to a stable composite variable. The direction and amplitude of the ongoing stereotyped movements seemed also be adjusted in order to minimize this variable. These findings suggest that, during visually guided movements, human subjects attempt to minimize such a composite variable and not the instantaneous error. This minimization seems to be obtained by the execution of stereotyped corrective movements.  相似文献   

10.
Previous studies have shown that accurate saccades can be generated, in the dark, that compensate for movements of the visual axis that result from movements of either the eyes alone or the head alone that intervene between target presentation and saccade onset. We have carried out experiments with human subjects to test whether gaze saccades (gaze = eye-in-space = eye-in-head + head-in-space) can be generated that compensate for smooth pursuit movements of gaze that intervene between target onset and gaze-saccade onset. In both head-unrestrained (head-free) and -restrained (head-fixed) conditions, subjects were asked to make gaze shifts, in the dark, to the remembered location of a briefly flashed target. On most trials, during the memory period, the subjects carried out intervening head-free gaze pursuit or head-fixed ocular pursuit along the horizontal meridian. On the remaining (control) trials, subjects did not carry out intervening pursuit movements during the memory period; this was the classical memory-guided saccade task. We found that the subjects accurately compensated for intervening movements of the visual axis in both the head-free and head-fixed conditions. We conclude that the human gaze-motor system is able to monitor on-line changes in gaze position and add them to initial retinal error, to program spatially accurate gaze saccades.  相似文献   

11.
To characterize oculomotor components and diagnostic specificity of eye tracking abnormalities in schizophrenia, we examined a large consecutively admitted series of psychotic patients and matched controls. The most common abnormality in schizophrenic patients was low gain (slow) pursuit eye movements (47% of cases). Pursuit and saccadic eye movement abnormalities were no more severe in schizophrenic Ss than in those with affective psychoses, except that high rates of catch-up saccades were unique to schizophrenic Ss (17% of cases). These findings indicate that impaired pursuit eye movements are a major cause of eye tracking impairments in schizophrenia, that tracking dysfunctions commonly occur in affective psychoses, and that markedly high rates of catch-up saccades during eye tracking may be specific to schizophrenia. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
PURPOSE: The authors evaluated the reliability of the coefficients of the (1) amplitude/duration and (2) amplitude/peak velocity relationships of the mean precision values and the mean latency values (saccadic eye movements) and the coefficients of the target velocity/gain relationship (smooth pursuit eye movements). They computed test-retest maximum variability limits for these parameters. METHODS: After a 1-week interval, saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements were recorded twice from 20 healthy subjects; 12 of these subjects underwent a third recording session. The estimate of the intraclass coefficient of reliability, R, was adopted to evaluate the reliability of eye movement quantitative analysis. RESULTS: The data demonstrated that the reliability was fairly good for the amplitude/peak velocity relationship, was good for the precision, and was excellent for the amplitude/duration, the target velocity/gain relationships, and the latency. CONCLUSIONS: Quantitative analysis of both saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements is reliable. One statistic used to estimate reliability, ie, the within-subjects mean square value, also enables the determination of test-retest normal variability values for both the variances and the differences of measurements.  相似文献   

13.
Used real-time computer methods of controlling feedback factors in eye tracking to compare accuracy in tracking environmentally-generated and hand-generated visual targets in steering behavior of 5 undergraduates. Feedback delays of .1 and .2 sec. between hand and target movement produced a time lag of eye motion with respect to the hand-produced target action. Results confirm the assumption that steering and stimulus tracking represent different modes of response and are subject to different conditions of delay and displacement of action feedback of body movements. The main effect of feedback delays on eye tracking in steering was to restrict the normal capability of the eye to predict the course of self-generated stimulus movements by reducing the interval of time between hand action and eye response beyond the magnitude of the actual delay interval. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
When human subjects are presented with visual displays consisting of random dots moving sideways at different velocities, they perceive transparent surfaces, moving in the same direction but located at different distances from themselves. They perceive depth from motion parallax, without any additional cues to depth, such as relative size, occlusion or binocular disparity. Simultaneously, large-field visual motion triggers compensatory eye movements which tend to offset such motion, in order to stabilize the visual image of the environment. In a series of experiments, we investigated how such reflexive eye movements are controlled by motion parallax displays, that is, in a situation where a complete stabilization of the visual image is never possible. Results show that optokinetic nystagmus, and not merely active visual pursuit of singular elements, is triggered by such displays. Prior to the detection of depth from motion parallax, eye tracking velocity is equal to the average velocity of the visual image. After detection, eye tracking velocity spontaneously matches the slowest velocity in the visual field, but can be controlled by attentional factors. Finally, for a visual stimulation containing more than three velocities, subjects are no longer able to perceptually dissociate between different surfaces in depth, and eye tracking velocity remains equal to the average velocity of the visual image. These data suggest that, in the presence of flow fields containing motion parallax, optokinetic eye movements are modulated by perceptual and attentional factors.  相似文献   

15.
"Records of error from 27 Ss are analyzed to find the relation between types of error in pursuit tracking and two main determinants of tracking accuracy, target speed, and aided-tracking time constant. Three categories of error are distinguished in the analysis. The short wavelength errors are thought to represent positioning errors or quick adjustive movements to get back on course. The long wavelength errors probably represent errors in rate adjustment. The main finding of this study is that the psychological effects of an aiding device are complex. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
PURPOSE: This study in human subjects investigated whether or not the saccade system can monitor smooth changes of the eye position in total darkness. METHODS: The authors studied the properties of memory-guided saccades toward targets flashed during pursuit eye movements (target velocities of 15 degrees/s, 30 degrees/s, and 45 degrees/s) in four normal human subjects. Subjects were instructed to execute memory-guided saccades toward the position of the flashed target in total darkness when the pursuit target was extinguished. RESULTS: The vector of the saccade was more highly correlated with the vector of "spatial error" (the vector from the position of the eye at the time of the saccade to the position of the flashed target in space) than with the vector of "retinal error" (the vector from the position of the eye at the time of the presentation of the flashed target to the position of the flashed target). The amplitude and direction errors of memory-guided saccades were correlated with the amplitude of the retinal error but not with amplitude of eye deviation after the presentation of the flashed target. Pursuit velocity did not affect the error of the saccade. CONCLUSIONS: These findings suggest that the saccade system can monitor smooth changes of the eye position in total darkness, regardless of the velocity of pursuit, and that the accuracy of memory-guided saccades is dependent only on the amplitude of the retinal error.  相似文献   

17.
We proposed a velocity control strategy for dynamic posture determination that underlay an optimization-based differential inverse kinematics (ODIK) approach for modeling three-dimensional (3-D) seated reaching movements. In this modeling approach, a four-segment seven-DOF linkage is employed to represent the torso and right arm. Kinematic redundancy is resolved efficiently in the velocity domain via a weighted pseudoinverse. Weights assigned to individual DOF describe their relative movement contribution in response to an instantaneous postural change. Different schemes of posing constraints on the weighting parameters, by which various motion apportionment strategies are modeled, can be hypothesized and evaluated against empirical measurements. A numerical optimization procedure based on simulated annealing estimate the weighting parameter values such that the predicted movement best fits the measurement. We applied this approach to modeling 72 seated reaching movements of three distinctive types performed by six subjects. Results indicated that most of the movements were accurately modeled (time-averaged RMSE < 5 degrees) with a simple time-invariant four-weight scheme which represents a time-constant, inter-joint motion apportionment strategy. Modeling error could be further reduced by using less constrained schemes, but notably only for the ones that were relatively poorly modeled with a time-invariant four-weight scheme. The fact that the current modeling approach was able to closely reproduce measured movements and do so in a computationally advantageous way lends support to the proposed velocity control strategy.  相似文献   

18.
Subjects used a position control system to perform compensatory tracking of a repeated input pattern. Tracking error was roughly proportional to the velocity of the input signal. Error magnitude decreased with practice and increased with the addition of a concurrent memory task. These effects can be modeled as progressive and regressive changes in how well subjects used control movement velocity and displayed error velocity to anticipate the input pattern and thereby reduce their effective time delay. The weighting of velocity cues in this model progressed with practice and regressed with the secondary task, even though the secondary task required no concurrent visual scanning or simultaneous motor response. This regression effect appears to indicate cognitive interference with the anticipation process. Stationary linear models provide a good approximation to the movement patterns; however, these models do not account for episodes of rapid pulse-like movements that were revealed in the ensemble-averaged trajectories. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Experiments conducted in the laboratory concerned with the various aspects of pursuit tracking have their counterpart in everyday skilled movements. This article discusses prediction in a single activity, pursuit or 2-pointer tracking in one dimension, and then shows how similar principles are involved in many everyday skills. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
White (1976) reported that presentation of a masking stimulus during a pursuit eye movement interfered with the perception of a target stimulus that shared the same spatial, rather than retinal, coordinates as the mask. This finding has been interpreted as evidence for the existence of spatiotopic visual persistence. We doubted White's results because they implied a high degree of position constancy during pursuit eye movements, contrary to previous research, and because White did not monitor Ss' eye position during pursuit; if White's Ss did not make continuous pursuit eye movements, it might appear that masking was spatial when in fact it was retinal. We attempted to replicate White's results and found that when eye position was monitored to ensure that subjects made continuous pursuit movements, masking was retinal rather than spatial. Ss' phenomenal impressions also indicated that retinal, rather than spatial, factors underlay performance in this task. The implications of these and other results regarding the existence of spatiotopic visual persistence are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号