首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1373篇
  免费   19篇
  国内免费   1篇
电工技术   14篇
综合类   4篇
化学工业   305篇
金属工艺   15篇
机械仪表   35篇
建筑科学   34篇
矿业工程   10篇
能源动力   37篇
轻工业   101篇
水利工程   15篇
石油天然气   6篇
无线电   86篇
一般工业技术   197篇
冶金工业   363篇
原子能技术   16篇
自动化技术   155篇
  2022年   12篇
  2021年   7篇
  2018年   15篇
  2017年   16篇
  2016年   6篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   22篇
  2013年   55篇
  2012年   37篇
  2011年   51篇
  2010年   40篇
  2009年   46篇
  2008年   51篇
  2007年   46篇
  2006年   44篇
  2005年   37篇
  2004年   32篇
  2003年   30篇
  2002年   40篇
  2001年   32篇
  2000年   21篇
  1999年   27篇
  1998年   62篇
  1997年   45篇
  1996年   43篇
  1995年   28篇
  1994年   30篇
  1993年   31篇
  1992年   24篇
  1991年   21篇
  1990年   34篇
  1989年   34篇
  1988年   35篇
  1987年   21篇
  1986年   20篇
  1985年   24篇
  1984年   20篇
  1983年   16篇
  1982年   22篇
  1981年   26篇
  1980年   15篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   18篇
  1977年   14篇
  1976年   22篇
  1975年   13篇
  1974年   13篇
  1973年   16篇
  1972年   7篇
  1971年   8篇
排序方式: 共有1393条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
We model the capability of a small (6-optode) time-resolved diffuse optical tomography (DOT) system to infer baseline absorption and reduced scattering coefficients of the tissues of the human head (scalp, skull, and brain). Our heterogeneous three-dimensional diffusion forward model uses tissue geometry from segmented magnetic resonance (MR) data. Handling the inverse problem by use of Bayesian inference and introducing a realistic noise model, we predict coefficient error bars in terms of detected photon number and assumed model error. We demonstrate the large improvement that a MR-segmented model can provide: 2-10% error in brain coefficients (for 2 x 10(6) photons, 5% model error). We sample from the exact posterior and show robustness to numerical model error. This opens up the possibility of simultaneous DOT and MR for quantitative cortically constrained functional neuroimaging.  相似文献   
992.
Weakly basic drugs, such as verapamil hydrochloride, that are poorly soluble in neutral/alkaline medium may have poor oral bioavailability due to reduced solubility in the small intestine and colon. Film coated pellets were prepared using two strategies to enhance drug release at high pH values. Firstly, pellets were coated with Eudragit RS/hydroxypropyl methylcellulose acetate succinate (HMAS) mixtures in proportions of 10:1 and 10:3, respectively. The enteric polymer, HMAS, would dissolve in medium at pH > 6 creating pores through the insoluble Eudragit RS membrane to increase drug release. Secondly, an acidic environment was created within the core by the inclusion of fumaric acid at concentrations of 5 and 10% in order to increase drug solubility. Both strategies enhanced drug release into neutral medium in dissolution studies using the pH change method to simulate GIT transit. Dissolution profiles of samples tested in pH 1.2 for 12 hr were compared with those using the pH change method (pH 1.2 for first 1.5 hr, pH raised to 6.8 for remaining 10.5 hr) using the area under the dissolution curve (AUC), the dissolution half-life (t50%), and the amount of drug released in 3 hr (A3hr) values. Both strategies enhanced drug release into neutral medium although the strategy using HMAS in the film was more effective. The formulation least affected by pH change was a combination of the two strategies, i.e., pellets containing 5% fumaric acid coated with Eudragit RS 12% w/w and HMAS 1.2% w/w.  相似文献   
993.
Results from 5 studies (e.g., determining whether 32 US presidents died on critical days) show that biorhythm status did not appear to be useful for predicting behavior. (8 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
994.
Pluralistic ignorance occurs when individuals infer that the identical actions of the self and others reflect different internal states. We propose that pluralistic ignorance arises most commonly in contexts where individuals believe that fear of embarrassment is a sufficient cause for their own behavior but not for the behavior of others. Three predictions derived from the proposed analysis were tested. In Experiment I, we examined the hypothesis that people believe that they possess more of those traits that lead to social inhibition than do others. Ratings of the self and the average other on a series of trait dimensions supported this hypothesis. In Experiment 2, we pursued the hypothesis that people believe that fear of embarrassment is a more potent determinant of their own behavior than of the behavior of others. Subjects first were given an opportunity to engage in or refrain from engaging in an action that potentially had both beneficial and embarrassing consequences. They then were asked to estimate the percentage of other subjects whom they believed would act similarly. Consistent with the predictions, subjects both avoided the embarrassing course of action and overestimated the percentage of others who took it. In Experiment 3, groups of subjects were placed in the same context that confronted the individual subjects in Experiment 2. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
995.
Solvay clusters are sets of intermediate chemical reactions that execute a desired net reaction. We show that May-Rudd polygons give access to all Solvay clusters that involve two and three intermediate reactions regardless of the number of chemical species involved, and to all clusters that involve fewer than nine chemical species regardless of the number of chemical reactions. More elaborate Solvay clusters are accessible providing they exhibit a planar topological structure.  相似文献   
996.
Reports follow-up data for 20 male alcoholics who received either a multifaceted inpatient alcoholism treatment program alone (controls) or emetic or shock aversion therapy in addition to that program. Results indicate that emetic treatment exerted a modest beneficial effect at the 6-mo mark, but control and emetic Ss did not differ at the 12-mo mark. Both emetic and control Ss compiled more days of abstinence than shock Ss at both follow-up intervals. Analysis of the relationship between inpatient measures of the strength of alcohol aversions and posttreatment drinking revealed an inverse relationship between heart rate response to alcohol and number of days of drinking. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
997.
Administered to 270 4th–6th graders the Culture Fair Intelligence Test and the Comprehension subtest of the Gates-MacGinitie Reading Tests. Matched pairs of skilled and less skilled readers then read aloud a variety of material in cloze procedure format and printed in geometric transformations. The extent to which the syntactic and semantic constraints of the text guided their performance was assessed. Both on the cloze procedure and the transformed texts, skilled readers made greater use of grammatical and contextual information. Furthermore, there was no significant improvement across grade levels in the proportions of syntactically and semantically appropriate responses. The stability of differences between skilled and less skilled readers suggests the possibility that differential utilization of syntactic and semantic cues by beginning readers might contribute to differences among them in the development of reading skill. (25 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
998.
In a response to G. W. Bradley's (see record 1979-26025-001) article, the value of his "broadened self-serving bias formulation" is examined, and a case is made for clearly delineating different types of self-serving attributions. It is contended that there is still little support for the hypothesis that individuals alter their perception of causality so as to protect or enhance their self-esteem. (2 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
999.
[Correction Notice: An erratum for this article was reported in Vol 37(2) of Journal of Counseling Psychology (see record 2009-22584-001). It was incorrectly reported that a principal-components analysis with varimax rotation was performed. In fact, principal-axis factoring with varimax rotation was performed, and it is the results of this analysis that are reported in Table 2 of the article. In principal-axis factoring communalities are used on the main diagonal of the correlation matrix. Initially, these communalities are equal to the squared multiple correlation of the variable with the other variables in the set. An iteration procedure is used to improve these initial estimates of communalities. Another point of clarification is that in the principal-axis factoring reported in Table 2, we used listwise deletion of missing data, whereas we calculated the correlations reported in Table 1 by using pairwise deletion of missing data. The effect of these different procedures is that the factor analysis (N = 98) was based on a slightly different correlation matrix that the one reported in Table 1, and consequently, Table 2 cannot be exactly reproduced by factoring the correlations reported in Table 1. Also, the sign of the correlation of the Career Decision Profile Choice-Work Importance (row 13) and Decidedness (column 7) subscales (.37) is given as positive in Table 1, whereas it was in fact negative.] We examined 13 career subscales contained in 4 measures of career indecision to determine how different measures relate to one another and how many dimensions may exist across these different instruments. Ss completed career instruments and measures of state and trait anxiety, which were included to aid in the interpretation of factors. Correlations provide some evidence of the convergent validity of the subscales. A factor analysis yielded a three-factor solution accounting for 55.9% of the variance. The first and most general factor was interpreted as a component that represented information about occupations and self. The second factor was interpreted as a more indecisive component and was positively correlated with anxiety. The third factor seemed to be an affective comfort component but failed to relate to anxiety as was expected. Theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
1000.
In this article we argue that people are motivated to evaluate both the universalistic and particularistic standing of their abilities. One's universalistic standing is assessed by comparing with others who are similar to oneself on attributes related to the ability being assessed. One's particularistic standing is assessed by comparing with reference others, those with whom one shares an identity or bond. In five studies we attempted to distinguish between these two types of evaluation. We manipulated reference closeness by varying the distinctiveness of a shared attribute. In Studies 1 and 2 we gave subjects the choice of comparing a test score with that of either a distinctively similar (reference) other or a nondistinctively similar (nonreference) other. Although the two choices provided equivalent universalistic information, subjects overwhelmingly preferred to compare with the distinctively similar other. Studies 3–5 provided evidence that subjects actually did identify more closely with distinctively similar others than with nondistinctively similar others. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号