首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   109篇
  免费   4篇
综合类   17篇
化学工业   2篇
金属工艺   1篇
机械仪表   3篇
建筑科学   17篇
轻工业   3篇
水利工程   1篇
无线电   5篇
一般工业技术   9篇
冶金工业   28篇
自动化技术   27篇
  2021年   1篇
  2019年   1篇
  2017年   2篇
  2016年   1篇
  2015年   1篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   3篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   4篇
  2008年   12篇
  2007年   13篇
  2006年   13篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   9篇
  2002年   2篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   1篇
  1992年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   1篇
排序方式: 共有113条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
大规模定制在房地产开发中的应用分析   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
随着技术的发展和消费者对个性化要求的提高,大规模定制生产逐渐成为当今世界最流行的生产方式之一.文章探讨了大规模定制在房地产开发中应用的背景、基础和优势,并从模块化设计、业务流程再造、供应链、信息系统等方面提出大规模定制开发的实施策略和思路.  相似文献   
2.
在借鉴IM3、TOP-ERP、EMAN、用友等公司成功经验的基础上,针对中国模具企业在实施ERP管理中存在的问题,提出要提高ERP管理的个性化和软件的深度,进一步把模具企业管理的成功经验融入ERP,使模具企业在数字化管理中真正受益。  相似文献   
3.
李芃 《中国包装》2000,20(3):82-83
在包装设计的视觉表现中 ,色彩既要有一定的共性典型性 ,又要有独特的个性化色彩 ,这种包装才能在市场竞争中立于不败之地  相似文献   
4.
熊建新  祝丽莉 《包装工程》2007,28(8):68-69,163
运用创新设计原理,对室内空间环境的设计创意、空间设计、室内陈设和个性化设计4个方面进行了论述,使室内设计不仅能满足人们在功能上的需求,并更具人性化.  相似文献   
5.
针对目前分布式教育资源搜索缺乏智能化的现状,提出了根据用户的兴趣进行个性化搜索的概念,并设计了相应的个性化分析模型和个性化资源搜索模型,给出了采用自学习算法和信息检索、过滤方法。通过研究,可以解决传统教育资源配送系统效率低和智能化不高的问题,实现教育资源配送中心的个性化管理。  相似文献   
6.
Noting that there has been an increasing sharing of ideas between psychoanalysts and analytical psychologists within the past decade, the authors, all Jungian analysts, survey Jung's clinical theory in an effort to identify areas where psychoanalytic clinicians might find Jung's concepts helpful. They discuss Jung's approach to the dream, his understanding of psychotherapy as a dialectical procedure, and the theory of complexes and archetypes. They also review how Jung's notions of the Self and individuation are used in contemporary Jungian practice. A dream of a man in his mid-30s is presented to illustrate how these formulations can facilitate understanding of the patient and the analytic process. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
7.
To examine whether powerful people fail to individuate the less powerful, the authors assigned participants to either a high-power or low-power role for a computer E-mail role play. In 3 studies, participants in the high-power role made decisions and determined the outcomes of interactions; low-power role players had no power and relied on high-power targets for outcome decisions. Studies 1 and 2 found that high-power perceivers better individuated low-power targets. Study 3 demonstrated that high-power role players' superior judgment can be impaired by including a task that directs their responsibility toward organizational rather than interpersonal concerns. In all, results suggest that the effect of power on social judgment may be more complex and multifaceted than has previously been acknowledged. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
8.
Three experiments examined the relationship between prejudice and processing of stereotypic information. Higher levels of prejudice were associated with greater attention to and more thorough encoding of stereotype-inconsistent than stereotype-consistent behaviors but only when processing capacity was plentiful (Experiments 1 and 3). High-prejudice participants attributed consistent behaviors to internal factors and inconsistent behaviors to external forces (Experiment 2). Together, these results suggest that high-prejudice people attend carefully to inconsistent behaviors to explain them away but only if they have sufficient resources to do so. Results also showed that low-prejudice but not high-prejudice participants formed individuated impressions by integrating the implications of the target's behaviors (i.e., individuating). High levels of prejudice appear to be associated with biased encoding and judgment processes that may serve to maintain stereotypes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
9.
介绍了传统的全空间空调方式,指出全空间空调方式不能同时为所有人提供可接受的热感觉和室内空气品质,而置换通风和个体化控制的联合将是一种理想的空调方式,在更大程度上满足人们的不同需要。  相似文献   
10.
Recent research indicates that by 4.5 months, infants use shape and size information as the basis for individuating objects but that it is not until 11.5 months that they use color information for this purpose. The present experiments investigated the extent to which infants' sensitivity to color information could be increased through select experiences. Five experiments were conducted with 10.5- and 9.5-month-olds. The results revealed that multimodal (visual and tactile), but not unimodal (visual only), exploration of the objects prior to the individuation task increased 10.5-month-olds' sensitivity to color differences. These results suggest that multisensory experience with objects facilitates infants' use of color information when individuating objects. In contrast, 9.5-month-olds did not benefit from the multisensory procedure; possible explanations for this finding are explored. Together, these results reveal how an everyday experience--combined visual and tactile exploration of objects--can promote infants' use of color information as the basis for individuating objects. More broadly, these results shed light on the nature of infants' object representations and the cognitive mechanisms that support infants' changing sensitivity to color differences. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号