全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5417篇 |
免费 | 413篇 |
国内免费 | 259篇 |
专业分类
电工技术 | 126篇 |
综合类 | 668篇 |
化学工业 | 24篇 |
金属工艺 | 9篇 |
机械仪表 | 71篇 |
建筑科学 | 73篇 |
矿业工程 | 12篇 |
能源动力 | 1篇 |
轻工业 | 57篇 |
水利工程 | 7篇 |
石油天然气 | 6篇 |
武器工业 | 7篇 |
无线电 | 1167篇 |
一般工业技术 | 489篇 |
冶金工业 | 1138篇 |
自动化技术 | 2234篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 53篇 |
2024年 | 118篇 |
2023年 | 108篇 |
2022年 | 127篇 |
2021年 | 154篇 |
2020年 | 154篇 |
2019年 | 141篇 |
2018年 | 95篇 |
2017年 | 129篇 |
2016年 | 119篇 |
2015年 | 145篇 |
2014年 | 240篇 |
2013年 | 227篇 |
2012年 | 266篇 |
2011年 | 403篇 |
2010年 | 340篇 |
2009年 | 355篇 |
2008年 | 332篇 |
2007年 | 387篇 |
2006年 | 347篇 |
2005年 | 320篇 |
2004年 | 297篇 |
2003年 | 254篇 |
2002年 | 228篇 |
2001年 | 168篇 |
2000年 | 109篇 |
1999年 | 67篇 |
1998年 | 62篇 |
1997年 | 67篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 29篇 |
1994年 | 28篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
1965年 | 9篇 |
1964年 | 11篇 |
1963年 | 7篇 |
1960年 | 6篇 |
1959年 | 7篇 |
1958年 | 4篇 |
1957年 | 5篇 |
1956年 | 9篇 |
1955年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有6089条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
In long-term memory, negative information is better remembered than neutral information. Differences in processes important to working memory may contribute to this emotional memory enhancement. To examine the effect that the emotional content of stimuli has on working memory performance, the authors asked participants to perform working memory tasks with negative and neutral stimuli. Task accuracy was unaffected by the emotional content of the stimuli. Reaction times also did not differ for negative relative to neutral words, but on an n-back task using faces, participants were slower to respond to fearful faces than to neutral faces. These results suggest that although emotional content does not have a robust effect on working memory, in some instances emotional salience can impede working memory performance. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
2.
提出一种以词性为参考值的文本挖掘算法,能有效挖掘与种子词有关的关联规则。基于Bootstrapping算法思想,既减少了预处理阶段对于词根还原的依赖,能处理日志中出现的中文词汇。增加了对于日志文本上下的理解,提高了关联规则的有效性,并应用与IDS日志挖掘之中,有效改善挖掘效率,为规则库提供关联规则。 相似文献
3.
Mayer John D.; Panter A. T.; Salovey Peter; Caruso David R.; Sitarenios Gill 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2005,5(2):236
Replies to comments made by G. E. Gignac (see record 2005-06671-010) on the current authors' original article (see record 2003-02341-015). Gignac reanalyzed the factor structure of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) and found results that differed from those the authors obtained initially. The authors tracked down the surprising sources of those discrepancies. G. E. Gignac's hierarchical model of emotional intelligence appears promising, and the authors anticipate that further investigations of the MSCEIT factor structure may yield additional information. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
4.
介绍了一个将参考模板存放在智能卡上的与相关的话者确认系统及其工作原理、相关算法和参数设置。 相似文献
5.
van de Schoot Rens; Hoijtink Herbert; Mulder Joris; Van Aken Marcel A. G.; Orobio de Castro Bram; Meeus Wim; Romeijn Jan-Willem 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2011,47(1):203
Researchers often have expectations about the research outcomes in regard to inequality constraints between, e.g., group means. Consider the example of researchers who investigated the effects of inducing a negative emotional state in aggressive boys. It was expected that highly aggressive boys would, on average, score higher on aggressive responses toward other peers than moderately aggressive boys, who would in turn score higher than nonaggressive boys. In most cases, null hypothesis testing is used to evaluate such hypotheses. We show, however, that hypotheses formulated using inequality constraints between the group means are generally not evaluated properly. The wrong hypotheses are tested, i.e.. the null hypothesis that group means are equal. In this article, we propose an innovative solution to these above-mentioned issues using Bayesian model selection, which we illustrate using a case study. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
6.
Reports an error in Age differences in psychosocial predictors of positive and negative affect: A longitudinal investigation of young, midlife, and older adults by Tim D. Windsor and Kaarin J. Anstey (Psychology and Aging, 2010[Sep], Vol 25[3], 641-652). Contains an error in Figure 3, on page 649. The correction discusses where to find the correct data. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2010-18944-009.) Research has consistently shown that despite aging-related losses, older adults have high levels of emotional well-being relative to those in young and midlife adults. We aimed to contribute to knowledge around the factors that predict emotional well-being over the life course by examining age group differences in associations of positive and negative social exchanges and mastery beliefs with positive and negative affect in a sample of 7,472 young, midlife, and older adults assessed on 2 measurement occasions, 4 years apart. Results from structural equation models indicated lower levels of negative affect with advancing age. Mastery was consistently related to higher well-being, with the strongest associations evident for young adults. Older adults reported the most frequent positive and least frequent negative social exchanges; however, associations of social relations with affect tended to be stronger among young and midlife adults relative to older adults. Results are discussed in the context of life course perspectives on goal orientations and self-regulatory processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
7.
van der Schalk Job; Fischer Agneta; Doosje Bertjan; Wigboldus Dani?l; Hawk Skyler; Rotteveel Mark; Hess Ursula 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2011,11(2):286
In the present research, we test the assumption that emotional mimicry and contagion are moderated by group membership. We report two studies using facial electromyography (EMG; Study 1), Facial Action Coding System (FACS; Study 2), and self-reported emotions (Study 2) as dependent measures. As predicted, both studies show that ingroup anger and fear displays were mimicked to a greater extent than outgroup displays of these emotions. The self-report data in Study 2 further showed specific divergent reactions to outgroup anger and fear displays. Outgroup anger evoked fear, and outgroup fear evoked aversion. Interestingly, mimicry increased liking for ingroup models but not for outgroup models. The findings are discussed in terms of the social functions of emotions in group contexts. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
8.
《石油库与加油站》编辑部 《石油库与加油站》2006,15(1):1-3
党组决定今天召开集团公司暨股份公司2006年安全环保工作会议。在新年伊始就召开安全环保工作会议,是我们多年来坚持的好传统、好做法。这次会议的主要任务是,贯彻党的十六届四中、五中全会和中央经济工作会议精神,按照科学发展观和构建和谐社会的要求,落实集团公司暨股份公司2005年度工作会议的部署,把2006年安全环保工作安排好、落实好,为完成全年各项目标任务打下坚实的基础。 相似文献
9.
基于DSP的TETRA话音编码设计与实现 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
陆上集群无线电通信(TETRA)系统是欧洲电信标准协会(ETSI)在专用移动通信(PMR)和公共接入移动通信(PAMR)网络领域惟一支持无线数字集群的开放标准,本文主要介绍了该系统中话音鳊码算法,并讨论了其在DSP上实现的具体流程。 相似文献
10.
基于小波-卡尔曼的语音增强方法研究 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
提出了一种基于小波变换和卡尔曼滤波相结合的语音增强方法,这样既保留了小波变换对自相似过程的去相关作用和多分辨分析的功能,同时又保持了卡尔曼滤波器对未知信号的线性无偏最小方差估计的特点,可以有效地减小非平稳噪声;并引入基于声学模型的感知滤波器,以提高语音信号的可懂度。实验证明该方法对于低信噪比的有色噪声干扰条件下的语音信号的增强效果要优于一般的语音增强系统。 相似文献