共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Raúl Parada‐Castellano 《Color research and application》2016,41(2):175-187
The purpose of this research is to analyze the factors that create a sensation of balance or unbalance when a color image is observed depending on the light transmitted, reflected, and/or emitted by the objects that appear in it. This study is carried out by calculating the center of gravity (CG) of each image. The weight and the CG are obtained by calculating the force resulting from the different visual weights that make up the visual image and its position on this. When the visual CG and the geometric center coincide, it is recognized that the image is balanced. The action that acts on the balance of the lightness of an image is labeled as the visual weight. The visual weight of a figure is defined from the amount of visible light coming from it and the space occupied by this in the visual image. The colors show us the amount of light coming from a figure, while the shape provides us with information about the space that it occupies. The study explores how the qualities of appearance of three‐dimensional objects affect visual weight and how it can vary due to changes in light, motion of objects or the perspective of the image. Since these calculations can be very complex, we have developed the software called “PesoVisual” that automatically performs these operations on digital color images. Similarly, this software enables the analysis of the variability of the visual image. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 41, 175–187, 2016 相似文献
2.
William A. Thornton 《Color research and application》2000,25(2):148-150
Suggested optimal “primaries” and concomitant optimal gamut for any form of device or product whose output serves as input to the normal human visual system: TV‐like images, or images reflected from illuminated hardcopy. For those devices and products using additive coloration, three spectral primaries 450–530–610 nm, and their associated gamut, are suggested as goals. For those using subtractive coloration, three reflective components, of width perhaps 50–60 nm at half‐height and peaking at the same wavelengths, are suggested. Thus, in either case, the light from each pixel of the generated color image entering the pupil of the normal human observer is composed of a mixture of that observer's prime colors. “Prime colors” are defined as usual as (a) the wavelengths marking the peaks of the three spectral sensitivities of the (trichromatic) normal human visual system, and, thus, (b) those spectral lights to which the normal human visual system responds most strongly per watt of power content input to the pupil. Spectral sensitivities of the normal human visual system are carefully distinguished from those of the CIE Standard Observers. © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 25, 148–150, 2000 相似文献
3.
This research extends the previous RIT-DuPont research on suprathreshold color-difference tolerances in which CIELAB was sampled in a balanced factorial design to quantify global lack of visual uniformity. The current experiments sampled hue, specifically. Three complete hue circles at two lightnesses (L* = 40 and 60) and two chroma levels ( = 20 and 40) plus three of the five CIE recommended colors (red, green, blue) were scaled, visually, for hue discrimination, resulting in 39 color centers. Forty-five observers participated in a forced-choice perceptibility experiment, where the total color difference of 393 sample pairs were compared with a near-neutral anchor-pair stimulus of 1.03 A supplemental experiment was performed by 30 additional observers in order to validate four of the 39 color centers. A total of 34,626 visual observations were made under the recently established CIE recommended reference conditions defined for the CIE94 color-difference equation. The statistical method logit analysis with three-dimensional normit function was used to determine the hue discrimination for each color center. A three-dimensional analysis was required due to precision limitations of a digital printer used to produce the majority of colored samples. There was unwanted variance in lightness and chroma in addition to the required variance in hue. This statistical technique enabled estimates of only hue discrimination. The three-dimensional analysis was validated in the supplemental experiment, where automotive coatings produced with a minimum of unwanted variance yielded the same visual tolerances when analyzed using one-dimensional probit analysis. The results indicated that the hue discrimination suprathresholds of the pooled observers varied with CIELAB hue angle position. The suprathreshold also increased with the chroma position of a given color center, consistent with previous visual results. The results were compared with current color-difference formulas: CMC, BFD, and CIE94. All three formulas had statistically equivalent performance when used to predict the visual data. Given the lack of a hue-angle dependent function embedded in CIE94, it is clear from these results that neither CMC nor BFD adequately predict the visual data. Thus, these and other hue-suprathreshold data can be used to develop a new color-difference formula with superior performance to current equations. © 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 23, 302–313, 1998 相似文献
4.
The estimation of chromatic diversity of natural images is commonly quantified through the computation of the number of discernible colors and has received much attention because of the different implications it has. However, the relationship between that number and the number of colors that really attracts the attention from an observer is still not clear and has been given little attention. New concepts about salient discernible colors‐the salient chromatic diversity of images‐ and remarkable salient colors‐connected colors in the same salient image area‐are introduced as opposed to the classical number of discernible object colors, which is usually evaluated for the global image without differentiating between probable attended and non‐attended image regions. We have used different well‐known saliency models to locate the salient regions in the scenes and have heuristically studied the extent to which those models preserve the chromatic diversity of natural images. Based on a bottom‐up approach, a reduction of around 40%‐55% in the number of discernible colors were obtained, and not all saliency algorithms preserved a uniform sampling of the original color gamut. Thus, our results suggests that particularly the graph‐based visual saliency model got good low dissimilarity values in comparison with other approaches that put emphasis solely on color as the main low‐level feature. Furthermore, we have introduced a quantification scheme of the average number of remarkable salient colors appearing in the images, and have proved how the heuristic‐based analysis of salient image areas can be used to create segmented images automatically according to their salient chromatic diversity. 相似文献
5.
Erika Kumakura Katja Schmid Kazuhiko Yokosawa Annette Werner 《Color research and application》2019,44(6):886-893
In previous studies of image evaluation using wide color gamut displays, it is unclear to what degree people prefer the chroma‐varying images, especially the images that are more saturated than the most preferred image. Here, we present a study using a wide color gamut display, which measured viewers' most preferred level of image chroma (experiment 1) and their subjective evaluation of images that were less or more saturated than the previously most preferred one (experiment 2). The results showed that (a) the subjects' preferences for the original or for more saturated images depended on the images' original chroma and (b) the subjective evaluation of the images, that is, Valence and Arousal, did not deteriorate when the chroma of images increased more than the most preferred level. Such results indicate that the increase of the image chroma in wider color gamut displays can have a substantial influence on the impression images have on viewers. 相似文献
6.
Arjan Gijsenij Marjan Vazirian Peter Spiers Stephen Westland Pim Koeckhoven 《Color research and application》2023,48(1):69-87
Designing a mood board is a creative tool designers often employ at the start of a new (interior) design project. The colors of a mood board represent important information about the envisioned interior designs. In this article, we focus on digital mood boards and we propose a novel method to determine the mood boards' key colors from a design perspective using dE-means color clustering. This proposed algorithm includes a fixed initialization to overcome the non-deterministic nature of traditional k-means, and a merging step to ensure all colors in the calculated palette are at least a dE-threshold apart. Quantitative and qualitative results demonstrate that the proposed algorithm outperforms existing methods in determining the key colors from a design perspective. Additionally, we introduce a novel method to visualize the calculated color palettes that takes into account the contribution of individual pixels to the construction of the calculated color palettes. Finally, we demonstrate how the proposed algorithm can be used to characterize color palettes for various design styles like modern, industrial and art deco. 相似文献
7.
Wen‐Guey Kuo Yuh‐Chang Wei Alex Liu Shang‐Ming Lin Yi‐Ting Shu 《Color research and application》2009,34(2):115-127
Although the CIE1931 and 1964 color matching functions have been used in color specification for decades, many researchers, from Allen in 1970 to Hu and Houser in 2006, have found that there still exists a great visual mismatch on the discrimination of color difference as in terms of the CIE color matching functions. Hence, some significant error would be made on color specification due to employing the CIE1931 and 1964 color matching functions. Therefore, six color difference formulae developed from different experimental methods are used to derive various deviate visual functions (DVFs) respectively, and to investigate the effect of these DVFs on the performance of the color difference formulae tested in estimating visual color difference. The results indicate that the performance of the color difference formulae in estimating color difference is significantly improved by the deviate visual functions derived in this study. The CIE94 color difference formula has the best performance in predicting the total visual color difference (ΔVT) using the DVFs and DVFIIs having the mean values 29 and 27 in PF/4 unit, respectively, while the CMC(l:c) the worst the ones 37 and 38. © 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 34, 115–127, 2009 相似文献
8.
OSA uniform color space was used to study the relationship between visual acuity and OSA color contrast. Visual acuity is characterized by 50% minimal separable visual angle using Landolt-C. The OSA color contrast is characterized by the distance between colors in OSA color space. Twenty subjects with normal color vision were tested on 342 test sheets printed with colored Landolt-Cs and background. These results demonstrated that MSVA is approximately inverse log-linearly related to OSA color contrast (R2 = 80.4%). Although luminance contrast (R2 = 54.2%) is more salient than chromatic contrast (R2 = 16.4%), both contrasts can induce very high visual acuity provided that they are sufficiently high. There is also evidence of an additive interaction between chromatic contrast and luminance contrast. Based on these findings, the OSA uniform color space and its color difference formula can be used as a scale for quantifying color contrast to accurately predict the size of colored text or symbols. © 1996 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 相似文献
9.
Gunilla Derefeldt Tiina Swartling Ulf Berggrund Peter Bodrogi 《Color research and application》2004,29(1):7-19
This report surveys cognitive aspects of color in terms of behavioral, neuropsychological, and neurophysiological data. Color is usually defined as a color stimulus or as perceived color. In this article, a definition of the concept of cognitive color is formulated. To elucidate this concept, those visual tasks are described where it is relevant: in color categorization, color coding, color naming, the Stroop effect, spatial organization of colored visual objects, visual search, and color memory. © 2003 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 29, 7–19, 2004; Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/col.10209 相似文献
10.
Li‐Chen Ou Patrick Chong M. Ronnier Luo Carl Minchew 《Color research and application》2011,36(5):355-372
An additive approach to predict harmony for three‐colour combinations is proposed in this article. It is hypothesised that a three‐colour combination can be seen as a combination of three colour pairs, each generating a harmonious/disharmonious feeling that can be quantified by a two‐colour harmony model the authors previously derived; the average of these three harmony values can then determine the overall harmony. To establish whether this hypothesis was valid, two psychophysical experiments were conducted in the United Kingdom and the United States. Experiment 1 used 6545 three‐colour wheels as the stimuli, presented individually on a calibrated cathode ray tube display. Under the same viewing conditions, Experiment 2 used 111 interior images as the stimuli. In each experiment, 20 British and 31 American participated as the observers. An additional test was undertaken, with 64 observers taking part, to address the issue of large sample size as encountered in Experiment 1, using 90 colour wheels selected randomly from those used in Experiment 1. The experimental results show close agreement between the observers' response and the harmony value predicted by the proposed method, with a correlation coefficient of 0.71 for the 6545 colour wheels, 0.93 for the 111 interior images and 0.88 for the additional 90 colour wheels. The results support the additive approach as a simple but robust method for predicting harmony in any three‐colour combinations, which may also apply to combinations generated by any number of colours. © 2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2011 相似文献
11.
The visual clarity of a lighting environment is significantly affected by changing the general color rendering index (Ra) of its illumination. This effect has been studied by a number of researchers, but the cause of this effect has not been thoroughly studied. In order to clarify this, the mutual relations between visual clarity, lightness perception, and feeling of contrast are analyzed by using object colors under illuminations with various Ra values. the results obtained are as follows. (1) the visual clarity of a lighting environment is different from the lightness perception of the object colors in the environment. (2) the change in visual clarity of a lighting environment is highly correlated to a feeling of contrast between object colors under the illumination. (3) the effect of visual clarity is estimated effectively by assessing the feeling of contrast using a specially selected four-color combination. (4) the illuminance for equal visual clarity for any illumination is predicted by the equality of feeling of contrast under the same illumination specified by the gamut area made by the component colors of the four-color combination in a brightness and colorfulness space. (5) the effect of visual clarity under various illuminations cannot be predicted by using their Ra values. 相似文献
12.
Hyojin Jung Hyeon‐Jeong Suk Saori Kitaguchi Tetsuya Sato Kanji Kajiwara 《Color research and application》2012,37(4):272-280
Consumers' tolerance of the color of recycled paper was evaluated by the visual assessment of such paper by 30 Japanese university students. The assessment was performed to measure color tolerance using 266 color samples in eight conditions specifying the situation of paper as (1) “either paper is recycled or not recycled,” (2) “whether you will buy/use,” and (3) “use for office paper or for workbook paper.” The responses of the subjects were sorted out quantitatively in terms of the colorimetric values of color samples and then correlated with International Organization for Standardization (ISO) brightness and CIE whiteness. Both the ISO brightness and CIE whiteness were found to be dependent on hue factors and also to be restricted for use in direct representation of the consumers' responses to paper quality. Since the consumers' evaluation of paper quality is due to visual whiteness, we propose a new equation to predict the consumers' tolerance of paper color, in which the equation contains two factors concerning the color distance from the white point and the hue impact. The new tolerance equation was confirmed to predict the consumers' tolerance successfully, particularly when the consumers are aware that the paper is recycled. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2011 相似文献
13.
Zena O'Connor 《Color research and application》2015,40(1):85-92
Visual communications design, which generally relates to design projects with specific communication objectives, includes most forms of graphic and digital design that have a commercial or an educational purpose. The effectiveness of such design projects rests on how well the embedded communications objectives are met from the perspective of the target audience. In advertising as in education, various post‐campaign evaluation techniques are used to measure effectiveness. The challenge for designers is to create designs that are aesthetically appropriate and visually engaging for the target audience, but which are also functionally‐legible and support the encoding of communication messages so that these are effectively decoded as intended. This is particularly important given that the effectiveness of visual communications is not guaranteed in the visually‐cluttered environment of the 21st century. Of the design elements available to the designer of visual communications, colour and contrast play key roles in visual perception, and the strategic use of these can contribute to the effectiveness of visual communications design. Examining the roles that colour and contrast within the context of Gestalt theories of perception provides additional insight into the ways in which these design elements can be harnessed to improve the effectiveness of visual communications design. © 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 40, 85–92, 2015 相似文献
14.
In this article, the color appearance of a large (85°) homogeneous self‐luminous visual stimulus was studied in a psychophysical experiment. Large stimuli were displayed on a plasma display panal (PDP) monitor. The large stimuli were viewed with a fixed viewing time (2 s). They were compared with 2° and 10° stimuli presented on a grey background on a CRT monitor. The so‐called “color size effect” was found to be significant. The color stimulus was perceived to be lighter when it was large compared with the 2° and 10° situation. But we did not find the general increase of chroma claimed in previous literature. We found only small hue changes. A model of the color appearance of large‐field stimuli is presented in terms of the CIELAB L*, a*, and b* values of the corresponding 2° and 10° stimuli. © 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 33, 45–54, 2008 相似文献
15.
Many applications in textile require color measurement of single strands of yarns due to unavailability of a mass of consecutive yarns. While multispectral imaging systems can capture images of single strands of yarns, an unavoidable problem accompanied is how to specify their colors from images, in which pixel values vary with positions owing to three‐dimensional shape of yarn. Based on the response of multispectral imaging systems to single strands of yarns, this article formulates the color specification problem as an optimization question and four methods are proposed: average of all pixels (AA), average of pixels in central area (AC), maxima of all pixels (MA), and lightness weighting method (LW). The first experiment analyzed color distribution of pixels on a single strand of yarn. Experimental results show pixels in central area have smallest color variation and largest intensity. The second experiment compared the proposed methods. Results show that CIEXYZ, L*, and C* values specified by the AA and MA methods are lowest and highest. Finally, colors of single strands of yarns specified by the proposed methods were compared with spectrophotometric colors of yarn windings using 12 pairs of single strands of yarns and yarn windings. Experimental results show the MA method yields the smallest lightness and chroma difference compared with spectrophotometric colors of yarn windings. The average color difference between spectrophotometric colors of yarn windings and multispectral imaging colors of single strands of yarns specified by the AA, AC, MA, and LW methods is 3.45, 2.72, 2.37, and 3.33 CMC(2:1) units. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 41, 500–512, 2016 相似文献
16.
Ken Sagawa 《Color research and application》2000,25(3):193-199
Visual comfort of a colored image of an ordinary scene was investigated in terms of the number of categorical colors contained in the image. The categorical colors were extracted by presenting the image for 5 s and asking the observer to report all the colors perceived in the image by using categorical color naming. It was found that the number of categorical colors obtained had a high negative correlation with the observer's direct estimation of visual comfort for the image. This indicates that the more colors an image contains, the less comfort for the image is felt, and suggests that visual comfort can be evaluated as a function of the number of categorical colors. The number of categorical colors was also determined colorimetrically and had a high negative correlation to the observer's direct estimation of visual comfort as well. © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 25, 193–199, 2000 相似文献
17.
Nowadays, many factors are involved in lighting design to achieve targeted lighting in addition to meeting the user's requirements and security to avoid energy loss. As building indexation at night is one of the main reasons for building lighting, it is necessary to make a decision to reduce energy consumption and increase observer's attention. The present study investigated the relationship between the correlated color temperature of the background and building based on the amount of visual attention and the amount of visual desirability. In this article, using a factor analysis approach, a range that attracts visual attention and visual desirability at the same time was obtained, and it is called viewerphilic perspective. The results presented in the form of a diagram show a relationship between the variables that directly and indirectly influence the formation of a viewerphilic night perspective. The results showed that the elements with a high correlated color temperature in the background and a low correlated color temperature and neutral color were in the viewerphilic range, while the elements with a low color temperature and a background with a high color temperature was in the range of viewer phobic. 相似文献
18.
Although a number of methods have been developed for image adjustment in various applications, very little work has been done in the context of visual design. In this regard, this article introduces a novel and practical context of image color adjustment and develops a method to adjust an image for harmony with a target color. The experiment with designers revealed that designers made significant changes in hue dimension, and preferred to promote color similarity between the image and the target color. Based on insights from designers, we proposed a method to achieve a harmonious combination of an image and a color element by increasing the hue similarity between them. The result of a user test revealed that our method is particularly useful for images with nonliving objects but less effective for images involving human skin, foods, and so on. It is expected that the practical context investigated in this study can promote a variety of related studies that satisfy the tangible needs of industries and academia. 相似文献
19.
Patrick Monnier 《Color research and application》2011,36(2):101-110
Distractor color heterogeneity refers to a condition in which a target is presented among distractors of different chromaticities. In the present study, the amount distractor color heterogeneity was varied systematically to determine how efficiently the visual system processes displays composed of search elements of multiple colors. Distractor color heterogeneity was expressed by selecting distractor chromaticities from sectors of various angles in a cone‐based normalized color space. The generalizability of the measurements was tested using two different visual search paradigms. An accuracy search task was used in which the search displays were presented as brief flashes and the dependent variable was search accuracy. A latency search task was used in which the display presentation time was under the participant's control and the dependent variable was reaction time. Compared to a homogeneous condition with distractors of a single color, distractor color heterogeneity had a deleterious effect on search performance in both paradigms. In normalized units, the measurements were similar across participants, target chromaticities, and tasks, but the same measurements expressed in non‐normalized units showed clear and systematic individual differences. © 2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2011 相似文献
20.
Evolving multimodal mobility needs influences established human–product relationships and requires a deeper insight into color preferences for car interiors. Hence, a study was conducted in which 204 members of a web contest created 1,265 designs. After a peer‐evaluation process, 53 most‐appreciated and 34 least‐preferred interior color compositions were identified and compared to identify patterns in color choices. Besides, visual lightweight design by layering of large interior components such as the seat, a modest use of color and patterns accompanied by repetition and the framing of the entire interior to create a feeling of spaciousness were found. Additionally, differences in the type of color between most and least favored color designs were found. Brown and beige occur more frequently among the top‐ than the worst‐rated designs. Larger surfaces are favored in lighter hues and smaller components in darker hues. 相似文献