首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Recent data from developing rats suggest that structures downstream from the amygdala are involved in the acquisition of conditioned fear-potentiated startle (FPS). The authors tested this idea in adult rats by temporarily inactivating the structure critical for FPS, the caudal pontine reticular nucleus (PnC), during fear conditioning. When the conditioned stimulus (CS) was an odor, rats displayed freezing, but not FPS, at test. This effect was not due to a decrease in footshock sensitivity. Further, no savings were evident on retraining. When the CS was a light, inactivation of the PnC had no effect on the acquisition of FPS. Thus, the PnC may be crucial for the acquisition of conditioned FPS to an odor, but not a light. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
3.
In the taste-potentiated odor aversion (TPOA) paradigm, animals acquire a strong aversion to an odor that is followed by delayed intoxication only if a gustatory stimulus is presented with the odor during conditioning. Although previous work has shown that N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) play a role in the acquisition of TPOA, the present study aimed at.describing the process in which NMDA receptors in the BLA are involved during acquisition of TPOA. Male Long-Evans rats received intra-BLA infusions of the competitive NMDA receptor antagonist {d},{l}-2-2-amino-5-phosphonovalerate ({d}-APV; 0.05 and 0.50 μg) immediately before or after the odor–taste conditioned stimulus (CS) presentation, or immediately before the test. Results showed that {d}-APV impaired acquisition of TPOA when infused before, but not after, the CS presentation, but did not affect retrieval. These results suggest that NMDA receptors of the BLA are involved in the formation of potentiation—by taste—of the olfactory memory trace, but not in the maintenance of this process. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Multi-unit and field potential responses in the anterior (AC) and posterior cingulate cortices (PC), dentate gyrus (DG), and anterior ventral (AV) and medial dorsal (MD) thalamic nuclei of rabbits were recorded during acquisition and performance of a locomotor conditioned response (CR). The CR, stepping in an activity wheel in response to a tone (conditioned stimulus [CS+]), prevented the occurrence of a shock unconditioned stimulus (UCS) scheduled 5 sec after CS+ onset. Ss also learned to ignore a different tone (CS–), not predictive of the UCS. Training was given daily until behavioral discrimination reached criterion. After criterion, asymmetric probability (AP) sessions were given that were the same as the conditioning session except for probability manipulation. A significant discriminative response developed in all regions during behavioral acquisition. The unit response in the AP session was enhanced in all areas by rare presentation of the CS–, compared with the equal and frequent CS– conditions. Rare presentation of the CS+ enhanced the unit response in the AC, PC, and DG, but it suppressed the firing of AV and MD neurons. Rare CS+ presentations did not alter AV and PC neuronal activity in Ss with subicular lesions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
The expression of learned fear emerges in a response-specific sequence where freezing occurs before fear potentiated startle (FPS) to an odor conditioned stimulus (CS; Postnatal Day [PN] 16 vs. PN 23; e.g., Hunt, 1997; Richardson, Paxinos, & Lee, 2000). Studies have shown that learned fear is expressed in a manner appropriate to the animal's age at training and not its age at test (Richardson & Fan, 2002; Richardson et al., 2000). Specifically, animals trained with an odor CS at PN 16 exhibit avoidance but not FPS when tested at PN 23. The present study shows that subsequent training with a different CS can "update" an early memory, allowing it to be expressed in a manner appropriate to the animal's age at test. This updating effect appears to be modality specific, whereby the subsequent training must involve a CS of the same sensory modality as the original training. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
The nictitating membrane/eyelid responses of 18 rabbits were classically conditioned using cerebellar mossy-fiber stimulation as a conditioned stimulus (CS) and air puff as an unconditioned stimulus (US). The dorsolateral, lateral, and medial pontine nuclei and the middle cerebellar penduncle were effective stimulation-CS sites for training. In one group of rabbits, robust conditioned eyelid responses were produced with paired trials and subsequently extinguished with CS-alone and explicitly unpaired presentation of the CS and US. In a second group of rabbits, no conditioned responses were evident for 4 days of unpaired CS and US presentations. Conditioned responses did develop, however, after paired training was begun. Lesions of the interpositus nucleus of the cerebellum completely abolished the conditioned responses of a third group of rabbits overtrained with the mossy-fiber CS and air-puff US. Results support studies that have demonstrated that the cerebellum is critically involved in acquisition and retention of simple learned responses and theories of cerebellar function which have proposed that mossy fibers supply critical "learning" input to the cerebellum for acquisition and retention of motor skills. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
The medial division of the medial geniculate body (MGm) projects to the lateral amygdala and the upper layer of the auditory cortex and develops physiological plasticity rapidly during classical conditioning. The effects of learning on frequency receptive fields (RFs) in the MGm of the guinea pig have been determined. Classical conditioning (tone–footshock), as indexed by rapid development of conditioned bradycardia, produced conditioned stimulus (CS)–frequency specific RF plasticity: increased response at the CS frequency with decreased responses at other frequencies, both immediately and after a 1-hr retention period. Sensitization training produced only general changes in RFs. These findings are considered with reference to both the elicitation of amygdala-mediated, fear-conditioned responses and the mechanism of retrieval of information stored in the auditory cortex during acquisition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Retention of the classically conditioned eyeblink response in rats was tested with a conditioned stimulus (CS)-alone extinction test and 2 sessions of reacquisition training. Retention of the eyeblink conditioned response (CR) during both tests was highest 24 hrs and 1 mo after initial acquisition. Three months after initial acquisition, responding during the CS-alone test was at baseline, but there was significant savings during reacquisition. By 6 mo after initial acquisition, the memory for the eyeblink CR was not expressed in either test. The group differences in retention, despite initial acquisition of the eyeblink CR to equal levels, suggest that rat eyeblink conditioning may provide a useful behavioral model for studying the neural processes underlying memory retention and loss. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Evaluative conditioning (EC) effects are often assumed to be based on a learned mental link between the CS (conditioned stimulus) and the US (unconditioned stimulus). We demonstrate that this link is not the only one that can underlie EC effects, but that if evaluative responses are actually given during the learning phase also a direct link between the CS and an evaluative response—a CS-ER link—can be learned and lead to EC effects. In Experiment 1, CSs were paired with USs and participants were asked to evaluate the pairs during the conditioning phase. Resulting EC effects were unaffected by a later revaluation of the USs, suggesting that these EC effects can be attributed to CS-ER learning rather than to CS-US learning. Experiment 2 replicated Experiment 1 with the difference that no evaluative responses were given during the learning phase. EC effects in this study were influenced by US revaluation, suggesting that these EC effects are mainly based on CS-US learning. In Experiment 3, it was shown that EC effects can be found even if the USs are entirely removed from the procedure and the CSs are only paired with enforced evaluative responses. Together the experiments show that the valence of a stimulus can change because of a contingency with an evaluative response. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Enhanced conditionability has been proposed as a crucial factor in the etiology and maintenance of panic disorder (PD). To test this assumption, the authors of the current study examined the acquisition and extinction of conditioned responses to aversive stimuli in PD. Thirty-nine PD patients and 33 healthy control participants took part in a differential aversive conditioning experiment. A highly annoying but not painful electrical stimulus served as the unconditioned stimulus (US), and two neutral pictures were used as either the paired conditioned stimulus (CS+) or the unpaired conditioned stimulus (CS-). Results indicate that PD patients do not show larger conditioned responses during acquisition than control participants. However, in contrast to control participants, PD patients exhibited larger skin conductance responses to CS+ stimuli during extinction and maintained a more negative evaluation of them, as indicated by valence ratings obtained several times throughout the experiment. This suggests that PD patients show enhanced conditionability with respect to extinction. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
The effects of the temporal sequence of tone (conditioned stimulus [CS]) and footshock (unconditioned stimulus [US]) during training (acquisition) on tone-dependent retention were studied in mice. Freezing increased significantly as an associative behavioral response in mice subjected to CS paired with US or after unpaired by 30 s in the memory test performed 24 hr after training. In the home cage of freely moving mice implanted with an electrocardiogram transmitter, CS triggered a strong tachycardiac response in the memory test. Heart rate (HR) increased from about 580 bpm to more than 750 bpm, and HR variability decreased significantly. The inhibition of the HR increase by the nonspecific β-adrenergic antagonist sotalol indicated the strong sympathetic contribution to the tachycardiac response. CS evoked a significant but minor HR increase in mice subjected to either CS or US only or CS and US unpaired by 60 s. Thus, HR and HR variability reflected associative learning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
The effectiveness of odor cues to support nutrient-conditioned flavor preferences in rats was studied. When the rats drank fluid, the CS+ odor was paired with intragastric (IG) infusions of Polycose, and the CS– odor with IG water. In Experiment 1, rats trained with almond and anise odors presented with plain drinking water failed to acquire a CS+ odor preference. In contrast, rats in Experiment 2 formed a strong aversion to anise (or almond) paired with lithium chloride, which indicated that the odors were distinguishable to the rats. Experiment 3 showed that providing unique tastes (bitter or sour) in combination with the odors during training potentiated odor conditioning. The rats displayed a strong preference for the odor?+?taste CS+ and for the odor component alone. Experiment 4 showed that with another pair of odors (peppermint and vanilla), CS+ preferences could be conditioned in the absence of taste cues during training. These results demonstrate that rats can acquire strong nutrient-conditioned odor preferences. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Examined the relative contributions of the amygdaloid basolateral complex (ABL) and central nucleus (CN) to taste-potentiated odor aversion (TPOA) learning, an associative learning task that is dependent on information processing in 2 sensory modalities. In Exp 1, rats with neurotoxic lesions of these systems were trained on the TPOA task by presenting a compound taste–odor conditioned stimulus (CS), which was followed by LiCl administration. Results showed that ABL damage caused an impairment in potentiated odor aversion learning but no deficit in the conditioned taste aversion. In contrast, rats with CN damage learned both tasks. Exp 2 examined the effects of ABL damage on TPOA and odor discrimination learning. The odor discrimination procedure used a place preference task to demonstrate normal processing of olfactory information. Results indicated that although ABL-lesioned animals were impaired on TPOA, there was no deficit in odor discrimination learning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
The authors examined how a conditioned stimulus (CS) that included species-typical cues affected the acquisition and extinction of conditioned sexual responses in male quail (Coturnix japonica). Some subjects were conditioned with a CS that supported sexual responses and included a taxidermic head of a female quail. Others were conditioned with a similar CS that lacked species-typical cues. Pairing the CSs with access to live females increased CS-directed behavior, with the head CS eliciting significantly more responding than the no-head CS. Responding to the head CS persisted during the 42-day, 126-trial extinction phase; responses to the no-head CS extinguished. Responding declined when the cues were removed or the subjects were sexually satiated. Possible functions and mechanisms of these effects are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
In rats, conditioned odor aversion (COA) occurs only if the time interval separating the odor from the subsequent intoxication is very short suggesting that the memory trace of the odor is subject to rapid decay. Recent results from our laboratory have found that lesion of the entorhinal cortex (EC), and activation of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) rendered COA tolerant to long interstimulus interval. The present study examined whether the odor memory trace depends on the interaction between the EC and the BLA. Rats lesioned in the EC received infusions of muscimol (a GABAA receptor agonist) into the BLA immediately after the odor presentation during acquisition of COA. Injection of muscimol into BLA prevented tolerance of COA to long interstimulus interval induced by EC lesions. This suggests that EC modulates the short-term memory trace of the odor by controlling the GABAergic activity of the BLA during acquisition of COA. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
Nonalcoholic men with high-density family histories of alcohol dependence (high risk) were compared with men with negative family histories (low risk) on a differential classical conditioning protocol that examined the acquisition of conditioned skin conductance responses (SCRs) to a tone (CS+) signaling the occurrence of an electric shock. High-risk Ss had significantly smaller SCRs to the CS+ tone probes during the acquisition phase and poor response discrimination between CS+ probes and CS– tones that were not paired with shock. The low-risk Ss showed a consistent pattern of discrimination between the CS+ and CS– tones. Unresponsivity to the CS+ probes was significantly related to more alcohol-related problems. The results suggest a relationship between risk for alcohol abuse and poor conditioning to signals for punishment, possibly reflecting weak behavioral inhibition system processes (D. C. Fowles; 1987). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
The hippocampus is generally thought to play a modulating role in the timing of conditioned responses in classical trace conditioning. One hypothesis is that the hippocampus stores a memory trace of the conditioned stimulus (CS) during the stimulus-free period. Cellular recordings, however, do not show any obvious CS storage. This article examines this issue by using a biologically plausible model of the CA3 region of the hippocampus. Simulations of the model reproduce both behavioral and physiological experimental data. On the basis of neural codes that develop in the model, the authors hypothesize that the hippocampus functions as a time-indexed encoding device for the CS and not as a CS storage buffer. Specifically, the CS initiates a sequence of neural activity during the trace interval that only indirectly represents the CS. The model yields 2 predictions: Some cells will increase in activity only during the trace interval, and some unconditioned stimulus (US)-coding cells will shift in time and fire before US onset. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
Evaluated backward conditioning of appetitive jaw movement and aversive nictitating membrane responses in rabbits, using 3 procedures: test trials, forward acquisition of the same response, and crossmotivational acquisition of the other response system. In both experiments, backward conditioning consisted of 3 conditioned stimulus/stimuli (CS) alone and 22 CS and unconditioned stimulus/stimuli (UCS) pairings. Contrasting the backward conditioning group with explicitly unpaired and no-treatment controls revealed that backward pairings produced no associative effects. The forward acquisition and crossmotivational acquisition tests suggest that excitatory backward acquisition of jaw movement was obtained. The forward acquisition test identified weak inhibitory conditioning of nictitating membrane responses, but the crossmotivational test implicated excitatory conditioning. (French abstract) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
The nucleus accumbens core (AcbC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) are required for normal acquisition of tasks based on stimulus-reward associations. However, it is not known whether they are involved purely in the learning process or are required for behavioral expression of a learned response. Rats were trained preoperatively on a Pavlovian autoshaping task in which pairing a visual conditioned stimulus (CS+) with food causes subjects to approach the CS+ while not approaching an impaired stimulus (CS-). Subjects then received lesions of the AcbC, ACC, or CeA before being retested. AcbC lesions severely impaired performance; lesioned subjects approached the CS + significantly less often than controls, failing to discriminate between the CS + and CS-. ACC lesions also impaired performance but did not abolish discrimination entirely. CeA lesions had no effect on performance. Thus, the CeA is required for learning, but not expression, of a conditioned approach response, implying that it makes a specific contribution to the learning of stimulus-reward associations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Five experiments examined the effects of iv naloxone treatment on aversive Pavlovian conditioning of eye-blink and heart-rate responses, and related unconditioned behaviors, in 143 male albino rabbits. Naloxone (NX) treatment before testing attenuated bradycardiac orienting responses to tones used as CSs. NX also attenuated conditioned bradycardia when administered either before or after training sessions, but it potentiated conditioned bradycardia during extinction of discriminative conditioning. NX did not influence acquisition or extinction of discriminative eye-blink conditioning or somatic or cardiac responses to shocks used as UCSs, but it did decrease locomotor activity. NX immediately after training sessions facilitated acquisition of eye-blink responses. It is concluded that NX influences aversive Pavlovian conditioning in more than one way: (a) During training, it appears to alter reception and processing of signals but does not affect subsequent development of somatic responses to the Pavlovian conditioning contingency. (b) After training, NX apparently affects consolidation of both somatic and autonomic conditioning. (c) NX also appears to delay extinction of Pavlovian conditioning; this effect may similarly involve changes in a stimulus-processing mechanism or in memory functions, but it apparently does not involve changes in somatomotor responsivity. (44 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号