首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
Event-related potentials were recorded from 7- to 18-year-old children of alcoholics (COAs, n = 50) and age- and sex-matched control children (n = 50) while they performed a visual selective attention task. The task was to attend selectively to stimuli with a specified color (red or blue) in an attempt to detect the occurrence of target stimuli. COAs manifested a smaller P3b amplitude to attended-target stimuli over the parietal and occipital scalp than did the controls. A more specific analysis indicated that both the attentional relevance and the target properties of the eliciting stimulus determined the observed P3b amplitude differences between COAs and controls. In contrast, no significant group differences were observed in attention-related earlier occurring event-related potential components, referred to as frontal selection positivity, selection negativity, and N2b. These results represent neurophysiological evidence that COAs suffer from deficits at a late (semantic) level of visual selective information processing that are unlikely a consequence of deficits at earlier (sensory) levels of selective processing. The findings support the notion that a reduced visual P3b amplitude in COAs represents a high-level processing dysfunction indicating their increased vulnerability to alcoholism.  相似文献   

2.
Event-related potentials were recorded from 80 participants ranging in age from 7 to 24 years while they attended selectively to stimuli with a specified color (red or blue) in an attempt to detect the occurrence of target stimuli. Color attention effects were identified as frontal selection positivity (FSP; 140-275 ms), selection negativity (SN; 150-300 ms), and N2b (200-450 ms), whereas target detection was reflected in P3b (300-700 ms). There were age-related decreases in the latencies of FSP, N2b, and P3b that paralleled decreases in reaction time and error rates. Also, the SN amplitude increased with advancing age, whereas both N2b and P3b showed changes in scalp topography. These results represent neurophysiological evidence that the efficiency of visual selective processes increases during childhood and adolescence. Developmental growth may take place at both relatively early and late levels of visual selective information processing.  相似文献   

3.
Previous research on the visual evoked brain response (VEBR) recorded from the occipital scalp indicates that this response shows a high degree of stimulus control. An evoked brain response can also be elicited simultaneously from the vertex response is affected to the same degree by several different aspects of visual stimuli as is the corresponding occipital response. The VEBR was recorded from the vertex of 3 human subjects as a function of changes in stimulus size and intensity. The results showed that the amplitude of the first major positive component of the VEBR was not reliably related to the stimulus parameters. It was concluded that the VEBR recorded at the vertex probably has its source in a distinct cortical generator which is not specifically related to the parameters of visual stimulation.  相似文献   

4.
OBJECTIVES: First, to differentiate between inattention and impulsivity based on type of errors made in the AX version of the Continuous Performance Task (CPT), and second, to investigate whether differences in performance between children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and normal controls also occur in specific forms of brain activity, namely event-related potentials (ERPs), presumably related to inattention and impulsivity or inhibition. METHOD: Sixteen ADHD and 16 normal control children performed the CPT-AX. ERPs were recorded at occipital (Oz), parietal (Pz), central (Cz), and frontal (Fz) leads. RESULTS: The ADHD children had a higher CPT-Inattention score and showed smaller parietal positive waves at a latency of approximately 300 msec in reaction to target stimuli, target P3s, likewise indicating less attention. In contrast, they showed neither higher CPT-Impulsivity nor a smaller frontocentral negative wave at about 200 msec (N2); the N2 is generally seen as reflecting inhibition. A subgroup of children with ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder (n = 6) had smaller N2 waves than controls, however. CONCLUSIONS: The ADHD group studied showed deficits in attention but not in impulsivity (or inhibition).  相似文献   

5.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from healthy subjects in response to unilaterally flashed high and low luminance bar stimuli presented randomly to left and right field locations. Their task was to covertly and selectively attend to either the left or right stimulus locations (separate blocks) in order to detect infrequent shorter target bars of either luminance. Independent of attention, higher stimulus luminance resulted in higher ERP amplitudes for the posterior N95 (80-110 ms), occipital P1 (110-140 ms), and parietal N1 (130-180 ms). Brighter stimuli also resulted in shorter peak latency for the occipital N1 component (135-220 ms); this effect was not observed for the N1 components over parietal, central or frontal regions. Significant attention-related amplitude modulations were obtained for the occipital P1, occipital, parietal and central N1, the occipital and parietal P2, and the parietal N2 components; these components were larger to stimuli at the attended location. In contrast to the relatively short latencies of both spatial attention and luminance effects, the first interaction between luminance and spatial attention effects was observed for the P3 component to the target stimuli (350-750 ms). This suggests that interactions of spatial attention and stimulus luminance previously reported for reaction time measures may not reflect the earliest stages of sensory/perceptual processing. Differences in the way in which luminance and attention affected the occipital P1, occipital N1 and parietal N1 components suggest dissociations among these ERPs in the mechanisms of visual and attentional processing they reflect.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

6.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) evoked by light flashes and auditory tones in a standard odd-ball procedure were recorded from Fz, Cz and Pz scalp sites. Tonic pain was evoked by immersion of the hand in cold water (5 degrees C). Significant effects of pain were found in responses to target stimuli but not in responses to non-target stimuli. P300 wave was affected more than the earlier P200 component. The reduction of P300 amplitude was the strongest effect, both in auditory and visual tests. P300 latency was not significantly affected. Difference curves (target minus non-target ERPs) showed the additional effects: latency of P200 component was elongated and its amplitude enlarged but only in auditory experiments. In control experiments with warm water stimulation no significant alterations of P300 or P200 components were found. The results show that the effect of tonic pain is specific: it predominantly affects the processes that manifest themselves as amplitude changes of P300 components in responses to target stimuli.  相似文献   

7.
The P3(00) event-related brain potential (ERP) was elicited with visual stimuli using an oddball task in which the subject indicated with a finger tap response the occurrence of a target stimulus that occurred randomly on 20% of the trials and refrained from responding to a standard stimulus. A total of six trial blocks were collected, with an equal number of artifact-free epochs averaged for both stimulus types. P3 amplitude from the target stimuli did not decrease across trial blocks; P3 amplitude from the standard stimuli did decrease across trial blocks. P3 latency from both the target and standard stimuli increased across trial blocks. No changes in amplitude or latency independent of the P3 effects were obtained for the other ERP components with trial block. The results suggest that P3 components elicited by visual stimuli do not readily habituate for actively discriminated target stimuli. The theoretical implications are discussed in the context of previous findings.  相似文献   

8.
A previous study by H. Grunebaum et al (see record 1975-00816-001) found that 5-yr-old children of schizophrenic mothers were impaired on visual attention tasks as compared with controls. No difference was observed in 6-yr-olds, however. This study reexamined the attentive behavior of 12 of these children 3 yrs later. In addition, evoked potentials to various visual stimuli were obtained. Group differences could no longer be found in visual attentive behavior; however, the risk group had evoked potentials of significantly higher amplitude and, under certain conditions, of longer latency than the controls. These findings suggest a maturational lag in visual stimulus processing in these children at risk. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
The relationship of task relevance and stimulus probability to P300 morphology, latency and distribution was assessed. Eight year olds and adults completed visual oddball tasks of recognition memory with frequent non-target (60%), infrequent target (20%), and infrequent novel (20%) stimuli. Stimuli consisted of 2 female faces posing neutral expressions, and 40 trial unique novel photographs depicting scenes, animals, objects or abstract patterns. Event-related potentials were recorded from 17 electrodes over frontal, central and parietal scalp, including lateral temporal sites. All stimuli elicited P300 responses at parietal electrodes, with the largest responses to the target stimuli (relevant and infrequent). The P300 responses of adults and children were morphologically dissimilar, with children showing broader peaks and latency shifts across electrodes. In addition, the eight year olds displayed a frontal negativity to novel stimuli which was absent in the responses of adult participants. Results suggest that different anatomical or functional circuitry may be involved in the processing of novelty for adults as compared to eight year olds.  相似文献   

10.
A male disadvantage has been reported in several outcome studies of children born preterm. Twenty-two healthy premature children (10 girls, 12 boys) born between 25 and 28 weeks of gestation and 20 controls born full-term (10 boys, 10 girls) were matched on socioeconomical status and age. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded by using 14 electrodes in a visual oddball task, with 75% frequent and 25% rare stimuli. This task elicited a larger P3 to the rare than to the frequent stimuli, with a prominent parietocentral localization. However, the amplitude was larger in full-term boys than in full-term girls, a difference that was not observed between preterm boys and preterm girls, especially to targets and on the central electrodes. In addition, the preterm group was characterized by a frontal slow wave larger in boys than in girls. In these prematures, the lack of the sex-related difference may be accounted by differences in the strength of the neuronal generators in males, as they might have been affected by the high level of androgens by the fetal testis under the control of placental gonadotropes during the first two thirds of gestation.  相似文献   

11.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded to trains of rapidly presented auditory and visual stimuli. ERPs in conditions in which subjects attended to different features of visual stimuli were compared with ERPs to the same type of stimuli when subjects attended to different features of auditory stimuli. This design permitted us to study effects of variations in both intramodal and intermodal visual attention on the timing and topography of ERP components in the same experiment. There were no indications that exogenous N110, P140 and N180 components to line gratings of high and low spatial frequencies were modulated by either intra- or intermodal forms of attention. Furthermore, intramodal and intermodal attention effects on ERPs showed similar topographical distributions. These combined findings suggest that the same neural generators in extrastriate occipital areas are involved in both forms of attention. Visual ERPs elicited in the condition in which subjects were engaged in auditory selective attention showed a large positive displacement at the occipital scalp sites relative to ERPs to attended and unattended stimuli in the visual condition. The early onset of this positivity might be associated with a highly confident and early rejection of the irrelevant visual stimuli, when these stimuli are presented among auditory stimuli. In addition, the later onset of selection potentials in the intramodal condition suggests that a more precise stimulus selection is needed when features of visual stimuli are rejected among other features of the same stimulus pattern, than when visual stimuli are rejected among stimuli of another modality.  相似文献   

12.
A number of studies have examined across-trial averaged late component. Event Related Potentials (EPR) and Reaction Times (RT) in response to multiple target stimuli. In this study, within-trial relatively fast and slow sub averages are additionally examined, in 20 patients with schizophrenia and 20 age and sex matched controls. A conventional auditory oddball paradigm. Across-trial ERP average analysis showed smaller N200 amplitude and delayed latency (but larger P200 amplitude) in patients with schizophrenia compared with controls. Within-trial ERP analysis revealed a number of additional findings. Controls showed distinctive differences in fast compared with slow ERP sub averages (smaller P200 amplitude, increased N200/P300 amplitudes and earlier latencies). The schizophrenic group on the other hand, showed relatively similar fast versus slow subaverages (no differences in P200 amplitude and N200 latency). In addition, between-group (within-trial) analyses highlighted significant differences in earlier stages of processing (compared with across-trial averages) in both fast and slow subaverages (increased N100 amplitude in controls). The complementary within-trial (compared with across-trial) data are interpreted with respect to a possible disturbance in inhibitory function in patients with schizophrenia.  相似文献   

13.
Event Related Potentials (ERPs) from thirty children (9-13 years old); fourteen of whom were dyslexic and sixteen normal-reading controls, were recorded during the performance of a memory scanning task. Visually presented stimuli consisted of either digits (lexical) or unfamiliar (non-lexical) characters. The subjects had to indicate whether a probe stimulus was a member of a preceding memorized set, or not. The memorized sets included 1, 2 or 3 items. Performance accuracies were significantly lower and reaction times longer in the dyslexic group. The significant Group x Lexicality interaction demonstrated P3 amplitudes which were smaller to lexical compared to non-lexical stimuli in dyslexics compared to skilled readers. Brain activity in response to the probes was more prominent over the right scalp in dyslexics, in contrast to controls that had left side prominence. The results suggest that dyslexic children relate to the physical features of stimuli as opposed to skilled readers, who rely more on the linguistic features of the stimuli. We suggest that remembering visually presented items in dyslexics involves different cognitive strategies and brain structures compared to skilled readers.  相似文献   

14.
Studied whether the amplitude of the visual EP is reduced during hypnotic hallucination in which the S is instructed to perceive visual stimuli as diminished in brightness or obstructed from view. The visual EP responses of 6 high- and 6 low-hypnotizable Ss (aged 18–33 yrs) and of 6 undergraduate controls were compared in 3 hypnotic conditions: stimulus enhancement, stimulus diminution, and stimulus elimination (obstructive hallucination). ANOVA revealed that high-hypnotizable Ss demonstrated significant suppression of the later components of the EP response (N? and P?) while experiencing obstructive hallucinations, indicating a change in information processing. This effect was significantly greater in the right, as compared to the left, occipital region. (35 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) from 50 children (6-11 years) and 10 adults were elicited by auditory passive, and by rare target and frequent non-target stimuli, and analyzed in the time and frequency domains. The latency of the maximal theta response (or the theta frequency component of the ERP) was evaluated with respect to age and scalp topography effects. The major findings were: (1) The latency of the maximal theta response decreased with increasing age in children, although for each stimulus type and location adults had shorter latencies than the children. (2) The developmental time course of latency reduction depended on the electrode location, with the most prominent reduction occurring at 8 years at Cz, and no differences between children groups obtained for the frontal site. (3) Maximal theta response latency was strongly associated with the latency of the late parietal P400-700 (P3b) component in children. The results suggest that the developmental latency decrease in P300 processes originate from a decrease in the preceding theta-related processes and may reflect a speeding of cognitive stimulus evaluation.  相似文献   

16.
This study investigated the simple reaction time (RT) and event-related potential (ERP) correlates of biasing attention towards a location in the visual field. RTs and ERPs were recorded to stimuli flashed randomly and with equal probability to the left and right visual hemifields in the three blocked, covert attention conditions: (i) attention divided equally to left and right hemifield locations; (ii) attention biased towards the left location; or (iii) attention biased towards the right location. Attention was biased towards left or right by instructions to the subjects, and responses were required to all stimuli. Relative to the divided attention condition, RTs were significantly faster for targets occurring where more attention was allocated (benefits), and slower to targets where less attention was allocated (costs). The early P1 (100-140 msec) component over the lateral occipital scalp regions showed attentional benefits. There were no amplitude modulations of the occipital N1 (125-180 msec) component with attention. Between 200 and 500 msec latency, a late positive deflection (LPD) showed both attentional costs and benefits. The behavioral findings show that when sufficiently induced to bias attention, human observers demonstrate RT benefits as well as costs. The corresponding P1 benefits suggest that the RT benefits of spatial attention may arise as the result of modulations of visual information processing in the extrastriate visual cortex.  相似文献   

17.
Scalp recordings of the P300 component of the event-related potential were made from a group of medication-free, chronic male alcoholics and a control group, participating in a visual Go/No Go reaction time paradigm. Subjects were presented with large and small forms of the letters T and V. The large forms (Go stimuli) required a button press with either the left or right hand, whereas the small forms (NO Go stimuli) required response inhibition. Recordings were made from 31 electrodes that, for statistical analyses, were grouped into five regions: frontal, central, parietal, occipital, and temporal. The results indicated that, in each of the five regions, both Go and NO Go response amplitudes were larger in the controls than in the alcoholics. No group differences in latency were observed in any region. Surface energy (Wang et al., Brain Topogr. 6:193-202, 1994) magnitudes paralleled P300 amplitudes and in the controls, compared with the alcoholics, were larger during both Go and No Go trials. Our findings suggest that abstinent, chronic alcoholics differ electro-physiologically from control individuals. These differences are manifested as widespread reductions in P300 amplitudes during the performance of a simple information processing paradigm. The reduced amplitudes may reflect a deficiency in an inhibitory mechanism proposed to underlie P300 generation.  相似文献   

18.
There is evidence that frontal lobe function may diminish in normal aging. The P3 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP) elicited by target events in an oddball paradigm becomes more frontally oriented in elderly subjects. It was hypothesized that the extent to which the P3 distribution is frontally oriented in old subjects may index less efficient frontal lobe function. In this study, bootstrapping methods were used to establish the reliability of the locations of maxima of surface brain activity obtained with ERP recordings from young and old subjects. The results indicated that brain activity maxima are reliable for a given individual. However, among the elderly only, there were also clear individual differences in the distribution of the P3 component elicited by target stimuli in an oddball paradigm. On the basis of these differences, the old subjects were divided into two groups. In line with predictions, those elderly subjects who showed frontal-maximal P3 scalp distributions had lower performance on standardized neuropsychological tests of frontal lobe function than those elderly subjects who showed posterior-maximal scalp topographies.  相似文献   

19.
Current source generators (dipoles) of the human visual evoked potentials to pattern-onset stimuli were investigated with the dipole tracing method, using a realistic four-layer head model of scalp-skull-fluid-brain, which can equate the surface potential distributions on a scalp to one or two corresponding equivalent dipoles. Three healthy adult human subjects were used, and 29 electrodes were set on a scalp of each subject. Visual stimulus of a checkerboard pattern was presented for 250 ms in each of eight different visual fields (central and peripheral parts of each of four quadrant fields). The visual evoked potentials consisting of initial positive-late negative waves (CI and CII components designated by Jeffreys and Axford) were recorded mainly on the occipital region contralateral to stimulated visual fields. The initial positive wave (CI) of visual evoked potentials were divided into two components: early component of the CI (e-CI--an early small positive deflection with approximate peak latency of 70-90 ms) and late component of the CI (l-CI--a late large positive deflection with approximate peak latency of 100-120 ms). The dipole with a fit exceeding 98% dipolarity with our model at the shortest latencies was defined as an "earliest dipole" of the evoked potentials, produced by the primary responses in the occipital cortex to an afferent volley from the lateral geniculate body. These earliest dipoles, for eight different visual field stimulations, were estimated at the approximate peak of the e-CI. Estimated dipoles were superimposed on a three-dimensional magnetic resonance image of each subject's brain. Earliest dipoles for right upper and right lower quadrant-field stimulations were located at the left calcarine cortices below and above the calcarine fissure, respectively; earliest dipoles for left upper and left lower quadrant-field stimulations were located at the right calcarine cortices below and above the calcarine fissure, respectively. Furthermore, earliest dipoles for central and peripheral quadrant-field stimulations were located posteriorly and anteriorly in the calcarine cortex, respectively. The results from these non-invasive analyses of visual evoked potentials indicated topographic localization of the dipoles around the calcarine fissure based on the loci of the visual fields. This was comparable to the retinotopy of the human occipital lobe based on clinicopathological studies.  相似文献   

20.
In order to address basic mechanisms behind a reduced averaged P300 wave in schizophrenia and depression, 17 unmedicated schizophrenic and 11 unmedicated depressive subjects were tested in an 'oddball paradigm' against healthy controls matched for gender and age. The amplitude distributions of single trials' maximum positive deflections after stimulation (P300) for both target and nontarget stimuli were determined, which served as a basis for calculating the discrimination index d'. This index characterizes differences in the electrophysiological responses to target and nontarget stimuli of a subject being engaged in a discrimination task. As a main result d' was significantly lower for schizophrenics than for their controls. Directly compared to depressive subjects, schizophrenics also depicted a statistically significant decrease of the discrimination index, which could not be explained by differences in age. Although the averaged P300 signals did not show any significant differences between the two diagnostic groups, the approach of calculating d' on the basis of single trial analysis differentiated between schizophrenics and depressives. In conclusion, schizophrenic patients revealed different functional features when generating event-related potentials in an 'oddball paradigm' compared to healthy controls and depressives.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号