首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Neural activity plays an important role in the development and maintenance of sensory pathways. However, while there is considerable experience using cochlear implants in both congenitally deaf adults and children, little is known of the effects of a hearing loss on the development of the auditory cortex. In the present study, cortical evoked potentials, field potentials, and multi- and single-unit activity evoked by electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve were used to study the functional organisation of the auditory cortex in the adult congenitally deaf white cat. The absence of click-evoked auditory brainstem responses during the first weeks of life demonstrated that these animals had no auditory experience. Under barbiturate anaesthesia, cortical potentials could be recorded from the contralateral auditory cortex in response to bipolar electrical stimulation of the cochlea in spite of total auditory deprivation. Threshold, morphology and latency of the evoked potentials varied with the location of the recording electrode, with response latency varying from 10 to 20 ms. There was evidence of threshold shifts with site of the cochlear stimulation in accordance with the known cochleotopic organisation of AI. Thresholds also varied with the configuration of the stimulating electrodes in accordance with changes previously observed in normal hearing animals. Single-unit recordings exhibited properties similar to the evoked potentials. Increasing stimulus intensity resulted in an increase in spike rate and a decrease in latency to a minimum of approximately 8 ms, consistent with latencies recorded in AI of previously normal animals (Raggio and Schreiner, 1994). Single-unit thresholds also varied with the configuration of the stimulating electrodes. Strongly driven responses were followed by a suppression of spontaneous activity. Even at saturation intensities the degree of synchronisation was less than observed when recording from auditory brainstem nuclei. Taken together, in these auditory deprived animals basic response properties of the auditory cortex of the congenitally deaf white cat appear similar to those reported in normal hearing animals in response to electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve. In addition, it seems that the auditory cortex retains at least some rudimentary level of cochleotopic organisation.  相似文献   

2.
If the repeated presentation of a single (standard) auditory stimulus is randomly interspersed with a second acoustically different (deviant) stimulus, the cortical activity evoked by the deviant stimulus can contain a negative component known as the mismatch negativity (MMN). The MMN is derived by subtracting the averaged response evoked by the standard stimulus from that evoked by the deviant stimulus. When the magnitude of the response is small or the signal-to-noise ratio is poor, it is difficult to judge the presence or absence of the MMN simply by visual inspection, and statistical detection techniques become necessary. A method of analysis is proposed to quantify the magnitude and statistically evaluate the presence of the MMN based on time-integrated evoked responses. This paper demonstrates the use of this integrated mismatch negativity (MMNi) analysis to detect the MMN evoked by stimulus contrasts near the perceptual threshold of two subjects. The MMNi, by virtue of being equivalent to a low-pass filtered response, presents an almost noise-free estimate of MMN magnitude. A single measure of the integrated evoked response at a fixed time point is used in a distribution-free statistic that compares the magnitude of the averaged response evoked by the deviant stimulus with a magnitude distribution derived from 200 subaveraged responses to the standard stimulus (with the number of sweeps per average equal to that of the deviant stimulus). This allows a calculation of the exact probability for the null hypothesis that the negative magnitude of the response evoked by the deviant stimulus is drawn from the magnitude distribution of responses evoked by the standard stimulus. Rejection of this hypothesis provides objective evidence of the presence of the MMN.  相似文献   

3.
The effect of stimulus intensity (sound pressure level, SPL) of auditory stimuli on the BOLD response in the auditory cortex was investigated in 14 young and healthy subjects, with no hearing abnormalities, using echo-planar, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a verbal and a non-verbal auditory discrimination task. The stimuli were presented block-wise at three different intensities: 95, 85 and 75 dB (SPL). All subjects showed fMRI signal increases in superior temporal gyrus (STG) covering primary and secondary auditory cortex. Most importantly, the spatial extent of the fMRI response in STG increased with increasing stimulus intensity. It is hypothesized that spreading of excitation is associated with the encoding of increasing stimulus intensity levels. In addition, we found bifrontal activation supposedly evoked by the auditory-articulary loop of working memory. The results presented here should assist in the design of optimal activation strategies for studying the auditory cortex with fMRI paradigms and may help in understanding intensity coding of auditory stimuli.  相似文献   

4.
Recorded the discharges of 22 single neurons in the secondary auditory cortical field (AII) during acquisition of the pupillary dilation conditioned defensive response in 12 chronically prepared cats. All 22 neurons developed discharge plasticity in background activity, and 21 of 22 cells developed plasticity in their responses to the acoustic CS. Decreases in background activity developed at the time that Ss began to display CRs. Increases in background activity developed in Ss that became more tonically aroused during conditioning. However, both increases and decreases in evoked activity developed independently of the rate of pupillary learning, tonic arousal level, or changes in background activity. Findings indicate that changes in background activity are closely related to behavioral processes of learning and arousal, whereas stimulus-evoked discharge plasticity develops solely as a consequence of stimulus pairing. Comparison with data obtained by the 2nd author and colleagues (see record 1985-03305-001) for the primary auditory cortical field (AI) indicates that both regions developed neuronal discharge plasticity early in the conditioning phase and that increases in background activity in primary auditory cortex were also associated with elevated levels of tonic arousal. The incidence of single neurons developing learning-related discharge plasticity was significantly greater in AII than in AI. Findings are discussed in terms of parallel processing in sensory systems and multiple sensory cortical fields. (69 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
To investigate cortical activities related to the visual recognition of characters, we recorded the magnetoencephalography (MEG) in six normal subjects who were encouraged to discriminate capital English letters displayed for a brief period. To reduce the primary responses evoked by the luminance change in the striate cortex (V1), we used a novel stimulus method, random dots blinking (RDB), by means of the temporal changes of patterns using a large number of small random dots. Along with the MEG recording, we also measured the discrimination accuracy rate (%) to know how well the subjects recognized the letters. One clear component, about 300 ms in peak latency, was identified in all six subjects. Its peak amplitude and the discrimination accuracy rate increased similarly as the character display duration became longer. Its signal source was estimated in the extrastriate cortex, around the fusiform gyrus, in the right hemisphere. We suspect that the activity in these cortical areas has strong relation to the conscious perception of characters.  相似文献   

6.
The subject of this case report is an 18-year-old woman with grossly abnormal auditory brain stem response (ABR), normal peripheral hearing, and specific behavioral auditory processing deficits. Auditory middle latency responses (MLRs) and cortical potentials N1, P2, and P300 were intact. The mismatch negativity (MMN) was normal in response to certain synthesized speech stimuli and impaired to others--consistent with her behavioral discrimination of these stimuli. Behavioral tests of auditory processing were consistent with auditory brain stem dysfunction. A neuropsychological evaluation revealed normal intellectual and academic performance. The subject was in her first year of college at the time of the evaluation. This case study is important because: (1) Although there have been several reports of absent/abnormal ABR with preserved peripheral hearing and deficits in auditory processing, little is known about the specific nature of the auditory deficits experienced by these individuals. Such information may be valuable to the clinical management of patients with this constellation of findings. (2) Of interest is the information that the mismatch negativity (MMN) cortical event-related potential can bring to the evaluation of patients with auditory processing deficits. The MMN reflects central auditory processing of small acoustic differences and may provide an objective measure of auditory discrimination. (3) From a theorectical standpoint, a patient with neural deficits affecting specific components of the auditory pathway provides insight into the relationship between evoked potentials and physiological mechanisms of auditory processing. How do various components of the auditory pathway contribute to speech discrimination? How might evoked potentials reflect the processes underlying the neural coding of specific features of speech stimuli such as timing and spectral cues?  相似文献   

7.
The 'Zwicker tone' (ZT) is an auditory after-image that can be evoked most effectively when a band-suppressed noise (relative width of gap 1/3 octave) presented for a certain period of time has been switched off. The sensation of this purely monaural phenomenon is that of a pure tone with a frequency corresponding to the center frequency of the gap and an equivalent level of 10-15 dB above auditory threshold. The sensation decays gradually; it may last as long as 10 s depending on how long the evoking noise was presented. The search for a physiological correlate has been futile so far, probably because the search was confined to more peripheral levels of the auditory system (inferior colliculus). A neuromagnetic study was performed in normal-hearing subjects in order to look for a neurophysiological correlate of the ZT in the auditory cortex. With a stimulation paradigm especially designed for this study, we have been able to isolate poststimulus activity which appears to be related to the ZT and which originates in the supratemporal auditory cortex. It is a sustained neuromagnetic activity that shows a clear-cut dipolar field distribution, and it appears that this activity has certain similarities with the tone-evoked auditory sustained response. The hypothesis is put forward that during the sensation of the ZT a process takes place in the auditory cortex which is similar to that underlying the sustained response, and which gives rise to the sensation of the ZT. In contrast to the sustained response, however, which is due to neural activity evoked by an external acoustic stimulus, the sustained activity associated with the ZT is due to a temporary absolute or relative reduction of neural activity originating from those regions in which the ZT exciting stimulus caused an adaptation. These differences in neural activity cannot be distinguished by the auditory system from a corresponding external acoustic signal. Preliminary studies in patients suffering from tonal tinnitus yielded results which exhibit a certain similarity with those obtained in the ZT experiment.  相似文献   

8.
To clarify the role of primary and second somatosensory cortex (SI and SII) in somatosensory discrimination, we recorded somatosensory evoked magnetic fields during a stimulus strength discrimination task. The temporal pattern of cortical activation was analyzed by dipole source model coregistered with magnetic resonance image. Stimulus intensity was represented in SI as early as 20 ms after the stimulus presentation. The later components of SI response (latency 37.7 and 67.9 ms) were enhanced by rarely presented stimuli (stimulus deviancy) during passive and active attention. This supports an early haptic memory mechanism in human primary sensory cortex. Contra- and ipsilateral SII responses followed the SI responses (latency 124.6 and 138.3 ms, respectively) and were enhanced by attention more prominently than the SI responses. Active attention increased SII but not SI activity. These results are consistent with the concept of ventral somatosensory pathway that SI and SII are hierarchically organized for passive and active detection of discrete stimuli.  相似文献   

9.
Normal 6-mo-old infants (10 male and 10 female) were studied to determine if cortical auditory evoked potentials (EPs) recorded to probe stimuli during verbal and musical stimulus presentation provided an index of cerebral functional organization. Furthermore, EP left–right amplitude asymmetries were examined in relation to gender differences and hand-reaching preference in these infants. Six-mo-old girls exhibited EP amplitude asymmetries associated with the stimulus conditions, whereas boys did not. In addition, both boys and girls showed associations between their patterns of EP asymmetry and their hand-reaching preference. The patterns of electrophysiological responses observed and their relation to hand preference suggest that the rate of maturation of the left and right hemispheres differs and is dependent on the gender of the infant. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
The purpose of the present study was to evaluate evoked potential and single fibre responses to biphasic current pulses in animals with varying degrees of cochlear pathology, and to correlate any differences in the physiological response with status of the auditory nerve. Six cats, whose cochleae ranged from normal to a severe neural loss (< 5% spiral ganglion survival), were used. Morphology of the electrically evoked auditory brainstem response (EABR) was similar across all animals, although electrophonic responses were only observed from the normal animal. In animals with extensive neural pathology, EABR thresholds were elevated and response amplitudes throughout the dynamic range were moderately reduced. Analysis of single VIIIth nerve fibre responses were based on 207 neurons. Spontaneous discharge rates among fibres depended on hearing status, with the majority of fibres recorded from deafened animals exhibiting little or no spontaneous activity. Electrical stimulation produced a monotonic increase in discharge rate, and a systematic reduction in response latency and temporal jitter as a function of stimulus intensity for all fibres examined. Short-duration current pulses elicited a highly synchronous response (latency < 0.7 ms), with a less well synchronized response sometimes present (0.7-1.1 ms). There were, however, a number of significant differences between responses from normal and deafened cochleae. Electrophonic activity was only present in recordings from the normal animal, while mean threshold, dynamic range and latency of the direct electrical response varied with cochlear pathology. Differences in the ability of fibres to follow high stimulation rates were also observed; while neurons from the normal cochlea were capable of 100% entrainment at high rates (600-800 pulses per second (pps)), fibres recorded from deafened animals were often not capable of such entrainment at rates above 400 pps. Finally, a number of fibres in deafened animals showed evidence of 'bursting', in which responses rapidly alternated between high entrainment and periods of complete inactivity. This bursting pattern was presumably associated with degenerating auditory nerve fibres, since it was not recorded from the normal animal. The present study has shown that the pathological response of the cochlea following a sensorineural hearing loss can lead to a number of significant changes in the patterns of neural activity evoked via electrical stimulation. Knowledge of the extent of these changes have important implications for the clinical application of cochlear implants.  相似文献   

11.
Examined the effects of conditioning on the discharges of single neurons in primary auditory cortex during acquisition of the pupillary conditioned response in 8 chronically prepared cats. Acoustic stimuli (1-sec white noise or tone) were presented with electrodermal stimulation unpaired during a sensitization control phase followed by pairing during a subsequent conditioning phase. Stimulus constancy at the periphery was ensured by the use of neuromuscular blockade. Discharge plasticity developed rapidly for both evoked and background activity, the former attaining criterion faster than the latter. The pupillary dilation conditioned response was acquired at the same rate as were changes in evoked activity (i.e., 10–25 trials) and faster than background activity (i.e., 20–25 trials). Increases in background activity were correlated with increasing level of tonic arousal, as indexed by pretrial size of the pupil. A following paper by D. M. Diamond and N. M. Weinberger (see record 1985-03268-001) examined neuronal discharge in the secondary auditory cortical field. (69 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
BACKGROUND: Altered sensory response is a prominent feature of schizophrenia. Inhibitory gatting mechanisms, shown by diminished P50 evoked responses to repeated auditory stimuli, seem to be deficient in schizophrenic persons. These inhibitory mechanisms usually are studied by averaging the electroencephalographic responses to many presentations of pairs of stimuli. Although averaging increases signal-to-noise ratio, it may obscure trial-to-trial differences. We compared differences between schizophrenic and normal persons in single trials and averages of P50 response. METHODS: Recordings from 10 schizophrenic patients and 10 normal subjects were analyzed using conventional averaging and single-trial measurements. A computer simulation of both methods examined their ability to extract evoked responses from background activity. Related single-neuron activity in the hippocampus in an animal model also was studied, because neuronal action potentials can be reliably identified in single trials. RESULTS: Averaged evoked potentials showed significant suppression of the P50 response to the second stimulus of the pair in normal patients, but not in schizophrenic patients. Single-trial analysis did not detect a response above background activity. Computer simulations gave similar results, suggesting that failure to detect suppression in single trials comes from inadequate differentiation of signal from noise. Recordings in animals confirmed almost complete suppression of the response of hippocampal pyramidal neurons to the second stimulus. CONCLUSIONS: The normal inhibition of response to repeated auditory stimuli seems to be compromised in schizophrenia. This loss of inhibitory gating could reflect a physiological deficit of hippocampal interneurons that is consonant with other evidence for interneuron pathologic defects in schizophrenia.  相似文献   

13.
The effects of excitatory classical conditioning on cytochrome oxidase activity in the central auditory system were investigated using quantitative histochemistry. Rats in the conditioned group were trained with consistent pairings of a compound conditional stimulus (a tone and a light) with a mild footshock, to elicit conditioned suppression of drinking. Rats in the pseudorandom group were exposed to pseudorandom presentations of the same tone, light and shock stimuli without consistent pairings. Untrained rats in a naive group did not receive presentations of the experimental stimuli. The findings demonstrated that auditory fear conditioning modifies the metabolic neuronal responses of the auditory system, supporting the hypothesis that sensory neurons are responsive to behavioural stimulus properties acquired by learning. There was a clear distinction between thalamocortical and lower divisions of the auditory system based on the differences in metabolic activity evoked by classical conditioning, which lead to an overt learned behavioural response versus pseudorandom stimulus presentations, which lead to behavioural habituation. Increases in cytochrome oxidase activity indicated that tone processing is enhanced during associative conditioning at upper auditory structures (medial geniculate nucleus and secondary auditory cortices). In contrast, metabolic activation of lower auditory structures (cochlear nuclei and inferior colliculus) in response to the pseudorandom presentation of the experimental stimuli suggest that these areas may be activated during habituation to tone stimuli. Together these findings show that mapping the metabolic activity of cytochrome oxidase with quantitative histochemistry can be successfully used to map regional long-lasting effects of learning on brain systems.  相似文献   

14.
Assigned 43 Long-Evans hooded rats to 7 groups receiving normal, visually restricted, or auditory restricted rearing experience. Ss were then implanted with bipolar pairs of electrodes in the auditory and visual projection areas. Electrical stimulation of the cortex was used as a discriminative stimulus for a lever-pressing response. The ease of using electrical stimulation of visual or auditory cortex as a discriminative stimulus was related to Ss' paranatal sensory experience. Deleterious effects were limited to the restricted cortical projection area, and there were suggestions of facilitated performance in response to stimulation of the nonrestricted cortical areas. These effects were absent when restriction was induced in adulthood. (28 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Developmental dyslexia is generally believed to result from impaired linguistic processing rather than from deficits in low-level sensory function. Challenging this view, we studied the perception of non-verbal acoustic stimuli and low-level auditory evoked potentials in dyslexic adults. Compared with matched controls, dyslexics were selectively impaired in tasks (frequency discrimination and binaural unmasking) which rely on decoding neural discharges phase-locked to the fine structure of the stimulus. Furthermore, this ability to use phase-locking was related to reading ability. In addition, the evoked potential reflecting phase-locked discharges was significantly smaller in dyslexics. These results demonstrate a low-level auditory impairment in dyslexia traceable to the brainstem nuclei.  相似文献   

16.
The discrimination of minimally paired speech sounds by seven retarded children with a mean age of 3 years, 2 months and a mean IQ of 38.4 was compared with the discrimination performance of eight normally developing 7-month-old infants. Children and infants were tested using the Visually Reinforced Infant Speech Discrimination (VRISD) paradigm in which they were taught to respond with a head turn to a change in a repeating background auditory stimulus. Responses were reinforced by activation of an animated toy. All children proved to be conditionable and both groups evidenced discrimination of the speech contrasts tested. The data suggest that the retarded children have moree difficulty processing a contrast cued by rapid spectral changes (often associated with consonant discrimination) than they do a contrast cued by steady-state spectral information (often associated with the perception of slowly articulated vowels). The normally developing infants did not find rapid spectral cues mor difficult than steady-state cues These results parallel those of Tallal (1976) who found that dynamic cues were specifically difficult for dysphasic children (with normal nonverbal intelligence), but not for linguistically-normal elementary school children.  相似文献   

17.
The role of contingency awareness in classical conditioning experiments using human subjects is currently under debate. This study took a novel approach to manipulating contingency awareness in a differential Pavlovian conditioning paradigm. Complex sine wave gratings were used as visual conditional stimuli (CS). By manipulating the fundamental spatial frequency of the displays, we were able to construct pairs of stimuli that varied in discriminability. One group of subjects was given an “easy” discrimination, and another was exposed to a “difficult” CS+ and CS–. A 3rd group was exposed to a stimulus that was paired with the unconditional stimulus (UCS) 50% of the time and served as a control. Skin conductance response (SCR) and continuous UCS expectancy data were measured concurrently throughout the experiment. Differential UCS expectancy was found only in the easy discrimination group. Differential SCRs were found in the easy discrimination group as well as in the difficult discrimination group, but not in the 50% contingency control. The difficult discrimination group did not exhibit differential UCS expectancy but did show clear differential SCR. These observations support a dual process interpretation of classical conditioning whereby conditioning on an implicit level can occur without explicit knowledge about the contingencies. The role of contingency awareness in classical conditioning experiments using human subjects is currently under debate. This study took a novel approach to manipulating contingency awareness in a differential Pavlovian conditioning paradigm. Complex sine wave gratings were used as visual conditional stimuli (CS). By manipulating the fundamental spatial frequency of the displays, we were able to construct pairs of stimuli that varied in discriminability. One group of subjects was given an “easy” discrimination, and another was exposed to a “difficult” CS+ and CS–. A 3rd group was exposed to a stimulus that was paired with the unconditional stimulus (UCS) 50% of the time and served as a control. Skin conductance response (SCR) and continuous UCS expectancy data were measured concurrently throughout the experiment. Differential UCS expectancy was found only in the easy discrimination group. Differential SCRs were found in the easy discrimination group as well as in the difficult discrimination group, but not in the 50% contingency control. The difficult discrimination group did not exhibit differential UCS expectancy but did show clear differential SCR. These observations support a dual process interpretation of classical conditioning whereby conditioning on an implicit level can occur without explicit knowledge about the contingencies. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
A new functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) method was used to investigate the functional neuroanatomy of face perception and memory. Whole-brain fMRI data were acquired while four types of stimuli were presented sequentially in an unpredictable pseudorandom order at a rate of 0.5 Hz. Stimulus types were a single repeated memorized target face, unrepeated novel faces, nonsense scrambled faces, and a blank screen. Random stimulus sequences were designed to generate a functional response to each stimulus type that was uncorrelated with responses to other stimuli. This allowed fMRI responses to each stimulus type to be examined separately using multiple regression. Signal increases were found for all stimuli in ventral posterior cortex. Responses to intact faces extended to more anterior locations of occipitotemporal cortex than did responses to scrambled faces, consistent with previous studies of face perception. Responses evoked by novel faces were in regions of ventral occipitotemporal cortex medial to regions in which significant responses were evoked by the target face. The repeated target face stimulus also evoked activity in widely distributed regions of frontal and parietal cortex. These results demonstrate that cortical hemodynamic responses to interleaved novel and repeated stimuli can be distinguished and measured using fMRI with appropriate stimulus sequences and data analysis methods. This method can now be used to examine the neural systems involved in cognitive tasks that were previously impossible to study using positron emission tomography or fMRI.  相似文献   

19.
Studied how N? and P? reflect a continuous process of matching inputs to a memory-based probabilistic model of the environment. 12 Ss performed a selective 2-channel auditory frequency discrimination while their evoked responses were registered in the attended and rejected channels. The auditory N? component was suppressed for all stimuli in the rejected channel at all probability levels, suppression being greatest when target stimuli were most probable. The rejected channel responded to reduced stimulus probability with N? augmentation. P? amplitude increased linearly as targets were made rarer, but only in the attended channel. When targets were most frequent, P? amplitude in the attended channel dropped to the same level as P? in the rejected channel. Selective attention is a necessary, but not sufficient condition for a selective P? response. Results indicate that attended inputs are modified selectively on the basis of location and pitch prior to the generation of N?, and information about stimulus probability is processed both inside and outside the focus of attention. Results with respect to P? point to a different probability-sensitive mechanism whose operation is contingent on focal attention. (French abstract) (19 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
A large negative deflection with a latency of 3 ms was observed in the auditory brainstem response (ABR) waveforms of some patients with peripheral profound deafness. This deflection was termed the N3 potential. In this paper, we review patients with the N3 potential and discuss the characteristics of abnormal ABR waveforms. The origin of the N3 potential was also discussed, especially with respect to vestibular evoked potentials. In most of the patients, audiograms showed no response to the maximum output of an audiometer in the high-frequency range and a residual response in the low-frequency range. The N3 potentials were noted at intensities of 80 dB nHL or greater. As the stimulus intensity increased, the amplitude of the potential increased and the latency decreased. A high repetition rate (83.3/s) of the click stimulus influenced the latency and amplitude of the N3 potential. The potential was replicated on retest within less than a month, and had a consistent latency and amplitude over the scalp. The results indicate that the N3 potential is not an electrical artifact but a physiological neural response evoked by a loud sound. The N3 potential is most likely not an auditory evoked response from cochlear or a response from a semicircular canal, because it has a 3-ms latency, a sharp waveform, and is unassociated with vertigo. The results suggest that the N3 potential may be a saccular acoustic response.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号