首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
In the first portion of this study, the effects of two levels of contralateral masking on the auditory brainstem response (ABR) were investigated in 10 normal-hearing subjects. No significant changes were observed in the mean latency-intensity functions or the mean amplitude-intensity functions of this group of subjects when noise of various levels was added to the nontest ear. In the second portion of this study, ABRs were also recorded from the poorer ear of four subjects with a profound unilateral sensorineural hearing loss. Results from the latter group revealed a crossed-over wave V in all cases when the stimulus was delivered to the poorer ear and the nontest (better) ear was not masked. Contralateral masking obliterated this "crossed ABR" in all four unilaterally impaired subjects. These results provide support for the use of contralateral masking when recording from the poorer ear of subjects having asymmetrical hearing loss.  相似文献   

2.
Examined the utility of cardiac habituation response recovery as a method for assessing infant cerebral asymmetries in auditory perception in a dichotic listening test. In a within-Ss design 12 3-mo-old infants were given a series of 4 10-trial tests during which their cardiac responses were habituated to a pair of dichotic speech syllables or music notes. The 10th trial in each test was a test trial on which one ear received its habituation stimulus while the other ear received a novel stimulus of the same type as the habituation pair (speech or music). Both stimulus type and ear receiving the novel stimulus were counterbalanced across tests. Overall, Ss' cardiac responses habituated during the tests and showed differential recovery to the novel stimuli. Specifically, greater response recovery occurred when a novel speech syllable was presented to the right, than to the left ear. Conversely, greater response recovery was found when a novel music note was presented to the left than to the right ear. Results indicate that young infants show a pattern of auditory perceptual asymmetries much like that found in older children and adults. Findings are consistent with the theory that in man the left hemisphere is superior at processing speech and the right hemisphere superior with nonspeech. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Intensity coding mechanisms are explored in a paradigm involving both forward and simultaneous masking. For intensity discrimination of 1000-Hz pure tone in quiet, a near-miss to Weber's law is observed. However, as more stimulus components are added to this relatively simple experiment, interactions among components produce a more complex pattern of results. An intense forward masker, while not causing any threshold shift for the test tone, produces a nonmonotonic intensity discrimination function ["the midlevel hump," Zeng et al., Hearing Res. 55, 223-230 (1991)]. The midlevel hump can be removed by the presence of additional notched noise [Plack and Viemeister, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 92, 1902-1910 (1992)] or narrow-band noise whose level is increased along with the test tone's standard level. The same midlevel hump can also be enhanced by a fixed-low-level notched noise or a high-level, high-pass noise which causes minimal masking at the test frequency. Interactions of forward masking and simultaneous masking present a serious problem for a clear interpretation of these results. For example, the notched noise was originally intended to restrict off-frequency listening, but on-frequency masking compromised this original purpose and confounded the interpretation of the notched noise effects. By measuring systematically the growth-of-masking functions, the present study identified various interactions of forward and simultaneous masking and clarified the role of off-frequency listening in forward-masked intensity discrimination. Both peripheral and central mechanisms may have contributed to the occurrence, reduction and enhancement of the midlevel hump under these masking conditions.  相似文献   

4.
5.
This paper addresses the question of whether the nonlinear amplitude response of the ear depends only on stimulus level or on both level and frequency. The level of the quadratic distortion product fh + fl was estimated using the tone-on-tone masking paradigm under a 2AFC transformed UP-DOWN procedure. The higher frequency tone (fh) was maintained at 1.2 times the lower (fl = 250, 500, 1000, or 2000 HZ). Changes in the maximum and minimum threshold as maskee phase changes, [the monaural phase effect (MPE) amplitude], changes in the phase of the aural distortion product, and changes in the growth of the combination tone amplitude were examined as functions of primary frequency and level. The results indicate that MPE amplitude is simply a function of S/N ratio between the combination tone and the masking at that frequency. Distortion product phase varies over 360 degrees with respect to the primary tones and is unrelated to primary frequency or intensity. Within listeners the range of distortion product phase is more narrow (70-90 deg) at a single frequency. The absolute level of the summation tone does not correlate well with primary frequency and the growth rate of the combination tone varies with frequency and observers over a range of 1.1-2.3 dB/dB. The results suggest frequency independent nonlinearity when the ratio of primary frequencies is constant.  相似文献   

6.
An attempt has been made to relate the masking effects studied under dichotic listening conditions to masking seen in the free field. Rather than use a free-field masking paradigm combined with monaural and binaural listening conditions, broadband maskers presented in virtual auditory space (VAS) have been used. Two virtual locations were tested: One was the right interaural axis (+90 degrees from the anterior midline) and the other was 40 degrees right of the anterior midline. Narrow-band (critical bandwidth) dichotic and diotic maskers were also derived from the VAS masker by bandpass filtering around the test frequency. This procedure preserved the interaural differences within the critical band about the test frequency but removed information outside the critical band. Using a diotic target tone of 0.6 kHz with a narrow-band masker centered on 0.6 kHz there was an increase in signal detection in the dichotic conditions when compared to that attributable to either ear alone. Furthermore, there was no further advantage in signal detection at this target frequency when a broadband VAS masker was used. This suggests that for low-frequency targets, the binaural differences within the critical band about the target frequency are sufficient for effective unmasking. In contrast, for a target frequency of 4 kHz, a dichotic narrow-band masker resulted in a reduction in detection compared to that attributable to either ear. However, detection improved to the level attributable to the far ear when a broadband VAS masker was used. This suggests that information outside the critical band is involved in the unmasking of high-frequency targets.  相似文献   

7.
The objective of this study was to improve vestibular evoked potentials as a qualitative parameter for vestibular function in small laboratory animals. Linear upward acceleration pulses (up to 8 g within 1 ms) were applied to the head of anesthetized chinchillas. Electrophysiologic responses recorded by a chronically implanted electrode within the facial nerve canal consisted of an initial negative potential, labeled N1, within the first millisecond following the onset of acceleration. This potential was followed by a series of positive and negative potentials found to be highly labile to acoustic masking. The initial negative potential was only minimally sensitive to acoustic masking and persisted following surgical cochlear ablation, but completely disappeared following administration of potassium chloride into the inner ear. Recorded from the contralateral ear, N1 was unaffected by these procedures. Amplitudes of N1 decreased with attenuating stimulus intensity (1.45 microV/dB), whereby N1 latencies slightly increased (-0.015 ms/dB). These data, when coupled with the ability to completely abolish N1 with potassium intoxication while the contralateral ear remained intact, indicate that this potential represents electrophysiologic activity resulting from activation of the ipsilateral vestibular labyrinth.  相似文献   

8.
MC Liberman  JJ Guinan 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1998,31(6):471-82; quiz 483; 553
Both MEM and MOC systems are sound-evoked reflexes to the auditory periphery which can be elicited by sound in either ear. Both MEM and MOC systems can increase thresholds in the auditory periphery: the MEM system acts by stiffening the ossicular chain, the MOC system by decreasing outer hair cell amplification of sound-induced motion in the inner ear. MEM-induced attenuations are largest for low frequency stimuli, MOC-induced attenuations are largest for mid- to high-frequency sounds. Both MEM and MOC systems can have anti-masking effects. The MEM reflex can decrease the masking of high-frequency signals by low-frequency noise (i.e., the upward spread of masking). The MOC reflex is complementary in that it minimizes masking of high-frequency transient signals by high-frequency continuous noise. MEM anti-masking arises by reducing suppressive masking and can improve masked thresholds at high frequencies. MOC anti-masking arises by counteracting excitatory masking. It does not improve masked thresholds, but can improve the detectability of small suprathreshold intensity increments. Anti-masking effects of both MEM and MOC systems should be reduced in cases of sensorineural hearing loss.  相似文献   

9.
Observations of the suppression of stimulus frequency evoked otoacoustic emissions (SFOAEs) by a second tone were made in human subjects. Measurements were made with the suppressor tone at frequencies and levels at, above, and below the stimulus tone generating the SFOAE. Data were collected with the stimulus tone at a range of levels (20-60 dB SPL), over a range of frequencies and in six different ears, in three subjects. The results were fitted to a phenomenological model, which allowed the data to be expressed as two parameters against the frequency of the suppressor tone. Characteristics of the suppression were examined, in particular the asymmetry between suppression by tones of higher and lower frequency than the tone evoking the SFOAE. At relatively low levels of suppressor tone, suppressors with frequencies higher than the stimulus tone were more effective suppressors than lower frequencies. At higher levels of suppressor tone, the situation was reversed, with lower frequencies being more effective than higher frequencies. These results were discussed in terms of nonlinear interaction between waves in the cochlea. This interpretation was used to estimate the shape of the traveling wave envelope produced by the stimulus tone, from the results of the suppression experiments. It was shown that the estimates of the shapes of the traveling wave envelope were nonlinear, the peak of the envelope becoming sharper at lower levels of stimulus. A simple quantitative model of SFOAE suppression was formulated using concepts of energy flow within the cochlea. This model produced SFOAE suppression results with all the major characteristics of SFOAE suppression from a real, human ear.  相似文献   

10.
Hamsters were trained to go left and right to sounds on their left and right sides, respectively. Silent trials were occasionally given in which no sound was presented. Hamsters exposed to a loud 2- or 10-kHz tone in 1 ear often shifted their responding on the silent trials to the side of the exposed ear, suggesting that they perceived a sound in that ear (i.e., tinnitus). The degree of tinnitus was related to the degree of the accompanying hearing loss (estimated by the auditory brainstem response). However, a conductive hearing loss (plugging 1 ear) did not cause a hamster to test positive for tinnitus. Tinnitus could be demonstrated within minutes following tone exposure, indicating an immediate onset, as occurs in humans. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Forty-three children (ages 7.0-14.5 years old) with and without attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), combined type had thresholds for detection of a 500-Hz pure tone estimated with and without a noise masker in the contralateral ear. The ear receiving the signal in the masked condition was varied randomly. A single-interval maximum-likelihood method estimated thresholds and false-alarm rate. Whereas the increase in threshold in children with ADHD in the presence of contralateral masking was comparable with controls, the increase in false-alarm rate was significantly greater. This dissociation between changes in sensitivity and response bias in the presence of masking noise supports suggestions that children with ADHD have difficulty inhibiting maladaptive responses and indicates that this deficit is quantifiable using psychoacoustic methods. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
During stages of remission, patients with paranoid schizophrenia seldom show severe attentional or information-processing dysfunctions, except in cases of long-term chronicity. The diagnostic specificity of four putative psychological vulnerability indicators of schizophrenia - the Span of Apprehension, the degraded stimulus Continuous Performance Test (dsCPT), the degraded stimulus visual backward masking task and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) - was examined in a group of patients with paranoid schizophrenia. Since no single test seems to identify all patients, the use of a combination of measures may be a useful strategy. Accordingly, the four tests were administered to 18 paranoid schizophrenic patients, 18 depressed patients and 18 normal subjects. Paranoid schizophrenic patients could be distinguished from normal subjects primarily on the basis of their performance on the backward masking task and secondarily by the dsCPT and the WCST. Paranoid schizophrenic and depressed patients could be differentiated to some extent by their performance on an information-mask condition of the backward masking task. Thus, of the four measures studied, only the degraded stimulus backward masking appeared to be a specific indicator of paranoid schizophrenia.  相似文献   

13.
A brief target that is visible when displayed alone can be rendered invisible by a trailing stimulus (metacontrast masking). It has been difficult to determine the temporal dynamics of masking to date because increments in stimulus duration have been invariably confounded with apparent brightness (Bloch's law). In the research reported here, stimulus luminance was adjusted to maintain constant brightness across all durations. Increasing target duration yielded classical U-shaped masking functions, whereas increasing mask duration yielded monotonic decreasing functions. These results are compared with predictions from 6 theoretical models, with the lateral inhibition model providing the best overall fit. It is tentatively suggested that different underlying mechanisms may mediate the U-shaped and monotonic functions obtained with increasing durations of target and mask, respectively. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
The tonotopicity of the cat's primary auditory cortex (AI) is thought to provide the framework for frequency-specific processing in that field. This study was designed to assess this postulate by examining the spatial distribution of neurons within AI that are activated by a single tonal frequency delivered to the contralateral ear. Distributions obtained at each of several stimulus levels were then compared to assess the influence of stimulus amplitude on the spatial representation of a given stimulus frequency in AI. Data were obtained from 308 single units in AI of four adult, barbiturate-anesthetized cats, using extracellular recording methods. Stimuli were 40-ms tone pulses presented through calibrated, sealed stimulating systems. In each animal, the CF (stimulus frequency to which the unit is most sensitive), threshold at CF, response/level function at CF, and binaural interactions were determined for isolated neurons (usually one per track) in 60-90 electrode tracks. For each unit, regardless of its CF, responses to 40 repetitions of contralateral tones of a single frequency, presented at each of four or five sound pressure levels (SPLs) in the range from 10 to 80 dB were obtained. Different test frequencies were used in each of four cats (1.6, 8.0, 11.0, and 16.0 kHz). For tones of each SPL, we generated maps of the response rates across the cortical surface. These maps were then superimposed on the more traditional maps of threshold CF. All units whose CF was equal to the test frequency could be driven at some SPL, given an appropriate monaural or binaural configuration of the stimulus. There was a clear spatial segregation of neurons according to the shapes of their CF tone response/level functions. Patches of cortex, often occupying more than 2 mm2, seemed to contain only monotonic or only nonmonotonic units. In three cortices, a patch of nonmonotonic cells was bounded ventrally by a patch of monotonic cells, and in one of these cases, a second patch of monotonic cells was found dorsal to the nonmonotonic patch. Contralateral tones of any given SPL evoked excitatory responses in discontinuous cortical territories. At low SPLs (10, 20 dB), small foci of activity occurred along the isofrequency line representing the test frequency. Many of these cells had nonmonotonic response/level functions. (ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

15.
Nonsimultaneous two-tone interactions were studied in the primary auditory cortex of anesthetized cats. Poststimulatory effects of pure tone bursts (masker) on the evoked activity of a fixed tone burst (probe) were investigated. The temporal interval from masker onset to probe onset (stimulus onset asynchrony), masker frequency, and intensity were parametrically varied. For all of the 53 single units and 58 multiple-unit clusters, the neural activity of the probe signal was either inhibited, facilitated, and/or delayed by a limited set of masker stimuli. The stimulus range from which forward inhibition of the probe was induced typically was centered at and had approximately the size of the neuron's excitatory receptive field. This "masking tuning curve" was usually V shaped, i.e., the frequency range of inhibiting masker stimuli increased with the masker intensity. Forward inhibition was induced at the shortest stimulus onset asynchrony between masker and probe. With longer stimulus onset asynchronies, the frequency range of inhibiting maskers gradually became smaller. Recovery from forward inhibition occurred first at the lower- and higher-frequency borders of the masking tuning curve and lasted the longest for frequencies close to the neuron's characteristic frequency. The maximal duration of forward inhibition was measured as the longest period over which reduction of probe responses was observed. It was in the range of 53-430 ms, with an average of 143 +/- 71 (SD) ms. Amount, duration and type of forward inhibition were weakly but significantly correlated with "static" neural receptive field properties like characteristic frequency, bandwidth, and latency. For the majority of neurons, the minimal inhibitory masker intensity increased when the stimulus onset asynchrony became longer. In most cases the highest masker intensities induced the longest forward inhibition. A significant number of neurons, however, exhibited longest periods of inhibition after maskers of intermediate intensity. The results show that the ability of cortical cells to respond with an excitatory activity depends on the temporal stimulus context. Neurons can follow higher repetition rates of stimulus sequences when successive stimuli differ in their spectral content. The differential sensitivity to temporal sound sequences within the receptive field of cortical cells as well as across different cells could contribute to the neural processing of temporally structured stimuli like speech and animal vocalizations.  相似文献   

16.
Three absolute identification experiments examined the effects on sequential dependencies of 2 factors: stimulus information and the number of stimuli used. Data were analyzed by multivariate information analysis. In Exp 1 with 5 university studentswith experience in absolute identification experiments, the stimulus modality was pure tone frequency under masking and no-masking conditions and 4, 6 10, and 16 tones were used. In Exp 2 with 5 university students, luminance levels of light circles were used as stimuli and number of luminance levels was 10 or 16. In Exp 3 with 6 university students, the stimuli were pointer positions along a horizontal line and they were presented either within a small or a large range to manipulate the stimulus information available. Results show that the sequential dependencies became larger as the stimulus information was decreased and as the number of stimuli was increased. Results are discussed in the context of models of sequential dependencies. Results of a multiple regression analysis on the data were compared with those of the information analysis. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Two experiments with normal participants examined the effects of masking and masking task load on latent inhibition (LI, poorer learning for a previously exposed irrelevant stimulus than for a novel stimulus) as a function of level of schizotypality. In Experiment 1, a masking task was needed to produce LI. In Experiment 2, with low load, LI was present in low- but not high-schizotypal participants. In high load, LI was abolished in low-schizotypal participants, but only approached significance in high-schizotypal participants. The data support a distraction- rather than a resource-limitation model of attentional dysfunction in high-schizotypal normal participants. In addition, the data indicate that obtaining LI requires that some attention be initially allocated to the preexposed stimulus and then reduced. Implications of the model for understanding attentional dysfunction in schizophrenia are discussed.  相似文献   

18.
Velocity discrimination thresholds for drifting luminance gratings were measured as a function of the time interval between test and reference gratings, using a 2-interval, forced-choice procedure. Discrimination thresholds, expressed as Weber fractions (ΔV/V), were independent of interstimulus interval (ISIs) ranging from 1 to 30 sec, demonstrating perfect short-term retention of velocity information. When a 3rd grating was briefly presented halfway through a 10-sec ISI, memory masking was observed. Discrimination thresholds in memory masking were unaffected by maskers of the same velocity but increased by 100% when test and masker velocity differed by a factor of 2. The results are interpreted with reference to a model where the short-term memory for simple stimulus attributes is assumed to be organized in terms of arrays of memory stores linked in a lateral inhibitory network. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Measured electrophysiological analogs of visual masking by light in single retinula (photoreceptor) cells of the lateral eye of Limulus. The intensity required for a 30-msec test flash to produce a criterion response increment in the presence of a 400-msec masking flash was determined over a range of stimulus onset asynchronies. The variation of the criterion intensity with stimulus onset asynchrony yielded a masking function quite similar to that obtained in human psychophysics, which suggests that this kind of response integration occurs in the most peripheral cell and that network interactions are not needed to account for masking by light. The implication that photoreceptor dynamics alone determine masking was examined: The criterion test flash intensity increased with receptor potential increases, but the relation could best be described by a bilinear function. It is suggested that the upper limb of the bilinear function may represent receptor saturation. Results are consistent with the notion that masking by light is a special case of the increment threshold and that receptor dynamics adequately account for dynamic factors in masking by light at this level of the visual system. (12 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
A sample of 22 subjects was studied from a population of adults who had suffered from bacterial meningitis in childhood. Audiovestibular, oculomotor and neuropsychological investigations were performed and quality of life was assessed. An age-matched control group of 20 subjects was recruited. In the meningitis group, nine subjects had abnormal pure tone audiograms. One was previously undiagnosed and a progression was found in four. There was an overrepresentation of subclinical vestibular pathology (6 out of 9 (67%)) in this group. Audiovestibular test results showed a peripheral pattern and oculomotor tests were normal. The quality of life scores of those with hearing loss were significantly higher than those in the control group. Neuropsychological tests of brain dysfunction were abnormal in six out of 22 (27%) who had recovered from meningitis. The prevalence of such dysfunctions was not related to audiovestibular disorder. The quality of life scores of those with brain dysfunctions were similar to those of the control group. The findings of reduced auditory memory and tone level perception in four out of 22 (18%), suggest that lesions of central auditory pathways may follow from bacterial meningitis. The results support the idea that inner ear damage is the major cause of hearing loss after bacterial meningitis. Despite the absence of brainstem involvement, central nervous system lesions with disturbed auditory processing and language functions can be of significance. The high frequency of discrete brain dysfunctions indicate that a thorough neuropsychological investigation is required after bacterial meningitis.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号