首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 750 毫秒
1.
Reaction times (RTs) and lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs) were studied to find out whether response preparation begins after mental rotation finishes, as assumed by discrete-stage models. Stimuli were disoriented normal or mirror-image characters, with character name determining which hand would respond. In Experiment 1, the normal/mirror-image information determined whether the response was to be executed (go) or withheld (no-go), and LRPs indicated that responses were weakly prepared before the end of mental rotation. Mental rotation was not required in Experiment 2, and significantly more response preparation was observed. In Experiment 3, probe RT trials embedded in the mental rotation task indicated that hand information is available to the response preparation process during rotation. Apparently, some response preparation occurs before mental rotation finishes, but rotation interferes with response preparation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
The effect of character disorientation on mirror–normal judgments was found to be partially attenuated with increasing task overlap with a preceding tone-frequency discrimination judgment. These results suggest that orientation-sensitive processing, used to prepare disoriented stimuli for mirror–normal discrimination, can be initiated and proceed in parallel with mental activities required for a tone-frequency discrimination task. The attenuation indicates that at least some of the orientation effect on mirror–normal discriminations had its locus prior to the dual-task processing bottleneck. The possibility that the partial attenuation of the orientation effect was due to attenuation of the effect of orientation on character identification, but not on the mental rotation required for mirror–normal judgments, was examined and rejected. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Examined the relative roles of mental rotation and stimulus-response (SR) compatibility in mirror-image and left-right decisions. 15 Ss, aged 19–43 yrs, were shown rotated letters and asked to indicate whether the letters were normal or backward (mirror-image task). Ss were then asked whether a dot would be located to the left or right of each letter if the letter was upright (viewer-centered left-right task) or if the letter was both upright and normal (letter-centered left-right task). The functions relating reaction time (RT) to angular orientation were parallel across the 3 tasks, suggesting that SR compatibility played no role, and that the Ss mentally rotated the letters to the upright in each case. A marked increase in RT to backward letters in the letter-centered task suggested a 2nd rotation in depth to restore the letters to normal. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Recent studies have shown that response time in mental rotation increases with the angular deviation between the current and preceding stimuli, suggesting a frame rotation process in which the intrinsic frame of the previous stimulus is brought into congruence with the coordinates of the current stimulus. In contrast, we show that this process involves image rotation in which the present stimulus is brought into alignment with the orientation of the previous stimulus. Such "backward alignment" succeeds only for shape-preserving sequences (i.e., identical stimuli at different orientations). Four experiments show that the backward alignment process (a) competes with the uprighting process typically found in mental rotation, and the response is determined by the process requiring the shortest rotational path; (b) is related to the tendency to repeat the previous response; (c) is insensitive to the position of the vertical; (d) is indifferent to the representation of the stimulus in long term memory; and (e) is different from the process underlying preparation for a stimulus in a specified orientation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Four experiments are reported that investigate whether images or reference frames are transformed during a mental rotation task. In all experiments a display of four identical letters (P1) was presented at either +90° or –90° from upright, and subjects had to decide whether the letters were normal or mirror-image reflections. A single letter (P2) was then presented 100 ms later in a variable orientation with the same task instructions. Reaction times to P2 were assessed to determine whether an image of P2 was rotated to upright or whether an internal reference frame was rotated into congruence with P2 from the orientation of P1. The results as a whole suggest that transformations of P2 can be initiated either relative to upright or relative to the orientation of P1. They further indicate that the probability of using each reference orientation can be changed by procedural variations. The findings are most parsimoniously interpreted as suggesting that mental rotation involves the transformation of reference frames rather than the transformation of template-like representations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
The "mental rotation" literature has studied how subjects determine whether two stimuli that differ in orientation have the same handedness. This literature implies that subjects perform the task by imagining the rotation of one of the stimuli to the orientation of the other. This literature has spawned several theories of mental representation. These theories imply that mental representations cannot be both orientation-free and handedness-specific. We present four experiments that demonstrate the contrary: mental representations can be both orientation-free and handedness-specific. In Experiment 1 we serendipitously discovered a version of R. N. Shepard and J. Metzler's (1971) "mental rotation" task in which subjects accurately discover the handedness of a stimulus without using "mental rotation," i.e., in which reaction time to compare the handedness of two forms is not a function of the angular disparity between the two forms. In Experiment 2 we generalize this finding to different experimental procedures. In Experiment 3 we replicate this finding with a much larger group of subjects. In Experiment 4 we show that when we preclude the formation of an orientation-free representation by never repeating a polygon, subjects carry out the handedness comparison task by performing "mental rotation."  相似文献   

7.
Eighty subjects viewed and visually imagined upright or rotated alphanumeric characters and later judged whether test characters were previously seen or imagined (reality monitoring). Identification and test characters were presented verbally or visually. When characters were identified and tested verbally, source confusions (misjudging a seen character as "imagined" and vice-versa) were infrequent and were comparable for rotated and upright characters. When characters were identified and tested visually, source confusions were more frequent and were influenced by character rotation. Memories for imagined characters were especially susceptible to source confusion. Also source confusions for seen characters increased when characters were rotated. These results are consistent with the proposal that increasing sensory similarity between perceived and imagined items increases source confusion and that perceived rotation generates cognitive operations similar to those generated when the subject imagines a character rotated.  相似文献   

8.
The remarkable accuracy with which healthy subjects can monitor their orientation while walking in darkness has been attributed to a process whereby awareness of orientation is automatically updated by information derived from active locomotion. The aim of this study was, first, to determine the contribution of vestibular information to the perception of orientation without vision, by comparing the accuracy of judgments of orientation following passive (seated) rotation about an earth-vertical axis with those following active (locomotor) rotation. The second aim was to assess whether monitoring orientation is indeed automatic, or whether it requires some degree of mental effort. This was evaluated by assessing whether accuracy in monitoring multiple passive or active rotations was affected by asking subjects to perform a mental task (that is, counting backwards) during rotation. The results indicated that although reliance on the primarily vestibular information available during passive rotation enabled subjects to accurately monitor single turns of up to 180 degrees, subjects were able to judge orientation after multiple turns more accurately after active rotation than after passive rotation, owing to the additional sensorimotor feedback gained from active locomotion. Accuracy in judging orientation was substantially impaired by backwards counting during both passive and active locomotion. This finding confirms that monitoring orientation during multiple turns in darkness necessitates central processing and adds to the growing body of evidence for the influence of mental activity on the perception and control of orientation.  相似文献   

9.
10.
Ss made mirror-normal discriminations on alphanumeric characters shown in different orientations in the picture plane. Either the characters or the background rotated during stimulus presentation in Exps 1–3. Character rotation in the direction of mental rotation facilitated mental rotation, whereas rotation in the opposite direction inhibited it. In Exp 4, characters were presented in different surface media so as to stimulate only 1 representation at a time. Mental rotation performance was similar whether the stimuli were defined by luminance, color, texture, relative motion, or binocular disparity, suggesting that mental rotation occurs at a level beyond that of the independent analyses of these different media. These results support those of Exps 1–3 in excluding the participation of low-level motion analysis centers in the mental rotation processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Previous research has shown pigeons to be insensitive to the orientation of visual test stimuli both for response latency and for discrimination ratio. Discrimination of stimulus orientation has been more difficult to learn than discrimination of small arbitrary differences between stimuli. This has suggested that the visual processing of pigeons is orientation invariant, which would obviate the need for mental rotation such as is often observed in studies with human subjects. Contrary to previous findings, the current experiment obtained linear effects of orientation on response latency and discrimination ratio, with a go/no-go procedure. Pigeons (Columbia livia) first learned to discriminate among line drawings of similar objects and then were tested with rotated versions of the drawings. The pattern of data is similar to that found in studies of human recognition of rotated objects. One speculative explanation of this finding is the mental rotation of stimuli by pigeons. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Six experiments tested the assumption that the mental rotation process is purely inserted into a mirror–normal discrimination task. In Exp 1, Ss took significantly longer to respond to upright characters in blocks containing rotated stimuli than in blocks containing only upright stimuli. Exps 2 and 3 showed that this rotational uncertainty effect was not caused by the need to determine stimulus orientation, and Exp 4 showed that it was independent of the visual quality of the stimulus. Exp 5 showed that the effect was greatly reduced when Ss performed a go–no-go task rather than choice reaction time (RT), and Exp 6 showed that it was independent of the complexity of the response required in the choice task. Results suggest that response selection in a choice RT mirror–normal discrimination task is altered when mental rotation is added, violating the assumption of pure insertion. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Two experiments, with 16 university students, revealed that errors of identification for alphanumeric characters increased approximately linearly with increased angular deviation from the standard upright orientation. Thus, pattern recognition does not seem to proceed via the extraction of "orientation-invariant features." It is argued that prior failures to discover systematic effects of orientation on the identification of simple patterns may have resulted from the use of RT as a dependent measure. Based on the data from Exp II, it was estimated that as little as 15 msec of processing time may have been sufficient to compensate fully for a 180° rotation of a simple pattern in the present experimental situation. Thus, RT experiments seeking effects of orientation must be designated to detect effects of smaller magnitude than may have been previously anticipated. (French abstract) (27 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Behavioral data (response time (RT) and accuracy) and psychophysiological data (event-related brain potentials of ERPs, and lateralized readiness potential or LRP) were studied in an experiment in which rotated alphanumeric characters were presented normally or mirror-reversed. In half of the trials, character classification (letter versus digit) determined whether or not the response was to be executed (go versus nogo) and parity determined the responding hand. In the other half, classification determined the responding hand and parity determined go versus nogo. LRP data indicated that response preparation occurred before mental rotation was finished. These data contradict strictly sequential discrete models of information processing and suggest continuous flow of information.  相似文献   

15.
Blindfolded adult participants (7 male and 9 female) were asked to point to previously seen targets after a body rotation. In 1 condition, participants had to update their positions relative to the targets during rotation; in another condition, they had to ignore the rotation and to imagine that they were still in their initial orientation. In the updating condition, replicating research of J. J. Rieser (1989), response latencies were only slightly affected by the magnitude of the body rotation. In the ignoring condition, however, response latencies increased with the angular difference between the participants' new position and their original orientation, suggesting that the participants updated their positions and then retrospectively "undid" this updating to mentally reestablish their original orientation. The results are supportive of the idea that heading is updated automatically as a person moves so that she or he is always primarily oriented with respect to her or his actual position. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
The paper gives a brief overview of five experimental approaches in which memory processes were studied by means of event-related brain potentials (ERPs). Some of the results were already published in English (Study 1), while others are new and will be reported in greater length as full paper elsewhere (Studies 2, 3, 4, and 5). Study 1 revealed that retrieval of information from episodic long-term memory is accompanied by a systematic slow negative potential. The topography of this slow wave depends on the quality of the reactivated information (spatial vs. verbal), and its amplitude reflects the difficulty of the retrieval process. In experiment 2 ERPs were recorded while subjects acquired either explicit or implicit knowledge about a sequential stimulus-response pattern. The data suggest that explicit learners who posses verbalizable knowledge about sequential dependencies have formed both perceptual and motor representations, while implicit learners have formed motor representations only. In study 3 fact retrieval in mental arithmetic was activated by a verification task. Incongruent solutions evoked an arithmetic N400-effect whose amplitude varied with the associative distance between an expected and an actually perceived solution to a multiplication problem. In study 4 ERPs were recorded during mental rotation tasks. A set of experiments revealed that mental rotation is always accompanied by a systematic negative variation over the parietal cortex. The amplitude of this "rotation specific negativity" increases with an increasing angular disparity between a perceived sign and its normal upright template. It was shown that this negativity is functionally distinct from a P300-complex which is often superimposed on it within the same latency window. Finally, study 5 examined ERPs in a sentence reading task in which grammatically legal but infrequent sentence constructions had to be processed. A left-anterior negativity was observed whenever an explicit case marker (the definite article in German) signalled a nominal phrase at a noncanonical position. The LAN phenomenon appears to be a manifestation of a syntax processor which performes a first-pass formal analysis of a sentence and which possibly allocates working memory resources whenever a word cannot be assigned immediately to an expected propositional role.  相似文献   

17.
Typically, mental chronometry is performed by means of introducing an independent variable postulated to affect selectively some stage of a presumed multistage process. However, the effect could be a global one that spreads proportionally over all stages of the process. Currently, there is no method to test this possibility although simple linear regression might serve the purpose. In the present study, the regression approach was tested with tasks (memory scanning and mental rotation) that involved a selective effect and with a task (word superiority effect) that involved a global effect, by the dominant theories. The results indicate (1) the manipulation of the size of a memory set or of angular disparity affects the intercept of the regression function that relates the times for memory scanning with different set sizes or for mental rotation with different angular disparities and (2) the manipulation of context affects the slope of the regression function that relates the times for detecting a target character under word and nonword conditions. These ratify the regression approach as a useful method for doing mental chronometry.  相似文献   

18.
19.
Human spatial representations of object locations in a room-sized environment were probed for evidence that the object locations were encoded relative not just to the observer (egocentrically) but also to each other (allocentrically). Participants learned the locations of 4 objects and then were blindfolded and either (a) underwent a succession of 70° and 200° whole-body rotations or (b) were fully disoriented and then underwent a similar sequence of 70° and 200° rotations. After each rotation, participants pointed to the objects without vision. Analyses of the pointing errors suggest that as participants lost orientation, represented object directions generally "drifted" off of their true directions as an ensemble, not in random, unrelated directions. This is interpreted as evidence that object-to-object (allocentric) relationships play a large part in the human spatial updating system. However, there was also some evidence that represented object directions occasionally drifted off of their true directions independently of one another, suggesting a lack of allocentric influence. Implications regarding the interplay of egocentric and allocentric information are considered. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Three experiments are reported that replicate and extend the finding of M. C. Corballis and R. McLaren (1982) regarding the interaction between mental rotation and the rotation aftereffect. In Experiment 1, participants saw tilted characters and made the usual parity judgment. In Experiment 2, participants were explicitly instructed to rotate the characters mentally into one direction or the other. In Experiment 3, participants had to report the direction into which they had mentally rotated. In all experiments, the function relating RT to the rotational angle was influenced by the aftereffect. RT was accelerated if the mental rotation was congruent with the aftereffect and decelerated if it was incongruent. A strategic change of direction of the mental rotation accounted only for some portion of this effect. The major portion has to be attributed to a direct interaction of two movements. The data suggest that the speed of the aftereffect combines with the speed of the mental rotation and, therefore, that imagery and perception share 1 representational medium. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号