首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 218 毫秒
1.
本文在深入理解面向服务的体系结构S0A的基础上,重点研究传统网管迁移到基于SOA的业务管理过程中需要解决的若干关键问题,并提出一种基于Web服务的业务管理体系架构WSSM.  相似文献   

2.
首钢股份公司基于自动车牌识别、诱导屏、反向寻车技术,精心设计了智慧停车场管理系统。该系统设计采用模块化、松散耦合方式,实现了系统、应用、资源管理之间的平滑交互,主体架构采用基于服务体系架构(SOA)、Web Service的服务集成框架,打造了全新的信息化管理平台,为企业在停车管理方向带来了商业价值,为管理提供了科学、量化的依据。  相似文献   

3.
郑立宪 《山东冶金》2012,34(2):62-64
为实现济钢出口商务订单的信息化管理,济钢采用MVC开发模式,构建SOA体系架构,开发了出口商务电子订单一体化系统。该系统的投入使用后,加速了信息的传递和业务处理速度,缩短了交易周期,提升了流程化管理的步伐,提高了客户的满意度。  相似文献   

4.
硅谐振式加速度计(SOA)精度高,稳定性好,数字输出,且体积小,成本低,易于集成,是新一代的微小型加速度计,具有极佳的应用前景.介绍了SOA的结构原理和发展概况,从谐振器,微杠杆,加工工艺,改变刚度方式,激励和检测方式等几方面对SOA的关键技术进行了综述.分析了SOA的机械耦合,加工工艺误差,梁弯曲刚度非线性等三大技术难点,并提出解决方案.最后对SOA的发展趋势进行了展望.  相似文献   

5.
针对客户提出多目标要求的定制化需求,尚无有效办法对生产工艺参数及目标性能进行合理优化,为此,开发了为满足企业需求的多性能目标定制化生产工艺设计与优化系统,将多输出支持向量回归算法、工艺参数控制能力分析、改进的粒子数优化算法等进行整合,搭建基于SOA架构的参数设计与优化系统,实现针对不同钢种类别、多样化订单需求的多输出力学性能预测及工艺参数设计优化功能,可以帮助钢铁企业提高产品质量的管控水平。  相似文献   

6.
基于SOA架构的PDM系统与制造管理系统集成设计与实现   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
针对大型装备制造企业单件定制、边设计边变更、老系统众多的特点,分析了产品数据管理系统(PDM)与制造管理系统集成的业务场景,提出了一种基于面向服务架构(SOA)的信息集成方法,有效地满足了装备制造企业数据量大、及时性高以及集成系统多的集成要求。针对装备制造企业支撑产品设计管理最主要的PDM系统和下游生产管理系统的集成要求,给出了完整的设计方案和系统实现,并描述了具体应用效果。  相似文献   

7.
针对目前兴起的虚拟化技术,对比传统架构模式,对企业的网络架构和服务器架构进行改造,升级到虚拟化技术环境,从而改变了传统架构的硬件环境依赖性问题,并进一步提高了系统的可靠性、可用性,提升了负载;同时,介绍了企业应用架构的虚拟化方法,并讨论虚拟化技术的部分问题。  相似文献   

8.
酒钢信息中心开发大型的应用系统采用的技术架构经历了从C/S应用双层架构以及简单的B/S架构到采用多层架构模式开发的过程。通过研究多层架构模型开发方法,自主设计实现的多层架构模型已应用于多个应用系统的开发过程中。结果表明:该模型经过多次修正和完善,应用效果已经非常良好,特别是为开发大型的应用系统提供了稳定可靠的架构模型。  相似文献   

9.
通过分析山钢集团各主要生产经营单位办公自动化系统的现状,结合集团管控模式和办公业务需求,采用HA、SOA、SSO等技术,成功开发了地址簿共享,为集团公司战略管控目标的实现打造了一个集信息交互、知识管理为一体的协同办公平台。  相似文献   

10.
针对新成立能源企业特点,本文从企业信息化战略、应用架构、集成架构、基础设施架构、实施路线图、保障体系等方面阐述如何做好企业信息化规划.  相似文献   

11.
Salient objects in the visual field tend to capture attention. The present study aimed to examine the time-course of salience effects using a probe-detection task. Eight experiments investigated how the salience of different orientation singletons affected probe reaction time as a function of stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between the presentation of a singleton display and a probe display. The results demonstrate that salience consistently affected probe reaction time at the shortest SOA. The effect of salience disappeared as SOA increased. These results suggest that contrary to the assumption of major theories on visual selection, salience is transiently represented in our visual system allowing the effects of salience on attentional selection to be only short-lived. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Examined how the prior visual presentation of a word from one of a bilingual S's languages can facilitate the identification of an associated word from the other language. Results from performance on lexical decision tasks with 72 Ss show a different pattern of effects for intra- and interlingual conditions at 2 different stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). At a short SOA, no facilitation effect was observed between associated words from different languages. At a long SOA, facilitation was observed for both between- and within-language conditions, but the between-language effect was weaker. Results do not suggest direct interlingual links in the bilingual lexicon. (French abstract) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
The present experiments tested the claim that phonological recoding occurs "automatically" by assessing whether it uses central attention in the context of the psychological refractory period paradigm. Task 1 was a tone discrimination task and Task 2 was reading aloud. The joint effects of long-lag word repetition priming and stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) were underadditive in Experiment 1, suggesting that an early component of lexical processing does not use central attention. In contrast, nonword letter length and grapheme-phoneme complexity yielded additive effects with SOA in Experiments 2, 3, and 4, suggesting that assembled phonology uses central attention. Further, orthographic neighborhood density also yielded additive effects with SOA in Experiments 5, 6, and 7, suggesting that lexical contributions to phonological recoding use central attention. Taken together, the results of these experiments are inconsistent with the widespread claim that phonological codes are assembled and/or addressed automatically. It is suggested that "automaticity" should be replaced by accounts that make more specific claims about how processing unfolds. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Placed 30 morphine addicted and 15 nonaddicted female albino rats on a 21-day conditioning regimen which involved the daily alternation of access to either water, sucrose-octa-acetate (SOA), or no liquid for 1 hr. The addicted Ss received injections of morphine after either the SOA sessions or the no-liquid sessions. Nonaddicted Ss were injected with morphine after the SOA sessions. Following the last injection, Ss were given a 2-bottle preference test between SOA and water. Results show that the addicted Ss that received morphine-SOA pairings had an increased preference for SOA, whereas the nonaddicted Ss showed a decrease in SOA preference after the same conditioning treatments. Addicted Ss that received the morphine injections on the no-liquid days showed no change in SOA preference. (21 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
This article reports 4 experiments that used the psychological refractory period procedure to characterize how people perform multiple tasks concurrently. For each experiment, a primary choice-reaction task was paired with a secondary choice-reaction task that had two levels of response-selection difficulty. Experiments 1 and 2 varied secondary-task response-selection difficulty by manipulating the number of stimulus-response (S-R) pairs. The effect of this factor on secondary-task reaction times (M) decreased reliably as the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) decreased. Experiments 3 and 4 varied secondary-task response-selection difficulty by manipulating S-R compatibility. Again, the effect of this factor on secondary-task M decreased reliably as SOA decreased. These results raise doubts about the existence of an immutable structural response-selection bottleneck and suggest that response selection for 2 concurrent tasks may overlap temporally. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
In 2 experiments, each with 16 college students and laboratory staff members, Ss searched 4- and 8-letter arrays for the presence of a T or an F. The position of the target was indicated by a bar marker presented at 1 of 7 stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs): –200, –25, 50, 125, 200, 275, or 350 msec. In Exp I, SOA conditions were blocked; in Exp II, SOA conditions varied randomly from trial to trial. In both experiments array size and SOA interacted. With 8-letter arrays, reaction time (RT) increased linearly with SOA with a slope less than 1. With 4-letter arrays, RT increased with SOA but reached asymptote at the level of no-cue control RTs at the 125-msec SOA. Results support the notion that cue search and comparison processes may function concurrently. (French summary) (18 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
The authors compared sublexical and supralexical approaches to morphological processing with unambiguous and ambiguous inflected words and words with ambiguous stems in 3 masked and unmasked priming experiments in Finnish. Experiment 1 showed equal facilitation for all prime types with a short 60-ms stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) but significant facilitation for unambiguous words only with a long 300-ms SOA. Experiment 2 showed that all potential readings of ambiguous inflections were activated under a short SOA. Whereas the prime-target form overlap did not affect the results under a short SOA, it significantly modulated the results with a long SOA. Experiment 3 confirmed that the results from masked priming were modulated by the morphological structure of the words but not by the prime-target form overlap alone. The results support approaches in which early prelexical morphological processing is driven by morph-based segmentation and form is used to cue selection between 2 candidates only during later processing. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
In a large proportion of the complex kanji characters resulting from the combination of two radicals, the left radical gives information about the meaning of the whole character, and the right one gives cues to pronunciation. In this study, this feature was exploited to investigate the relative contribution of semantic and phonological information in the process of recognizing a word written in kanji. In 2 experiments, a technique was used in which part of the target character (i.e.. the left radical, carrying the semantic information; the right, phonetic radical, or a fragment) was presented shortly (60 or 180 ms stimulus onset asynchrony [SOA]) before the exposure of the whole character, which had to be named as fast as possible. Earlier exposure of the phonetic radical produced facilitation of the naming response, which was stronger at the 180-ms SOA than at the 60-ms SOA, whereas preexposure of the semantic radical had a weak facilitatory effect at the 60-ms SOA and some inhibition at the 180-ms SOA. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Should we prefer one long look to two quick looks of equal overall duration? The authors systematically compared conditions in which a circular letter array was available either for a single look of 2d ms duration (onset asynchrony [SOA] from target to mask) or for two separate looks of d ms each. On the basis of the geometry of the underlying psychometric function relating the SOA to the probability of target identification, separate activation models with peaked hazard functions predict that for short SOAs single long looks would lead to better identification performance and that this advantage decreases (and may turn into a disadvantage) with increasing SOA. These predictions were tested, confirmed, and extended in 5 letter-identification experiments that focused on the mechanism by which information extracted from single arrays is integrated across different looks. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
The unconditioned reactions of Mongolian gerbils to peanuts rolled in sucrose octa-acetate (SOA) or denatomium benzoate (Bitrex) were studied in Experiment 1. Although SOA evokes as high a bitterness rating among humans as quinine salts, the gerbils' latency to ingest a nut flavoured with the former was similar to that to an unflavoured nut. Their oral latency to a nut that was flavoured with Bitrex was longer than to either an unflavoured one or one flavoured with SOA. However, the fact that they ingested a nut flavoured with the "most bitter substance known to man" within 70 sec after contact is surprising. Experiment 2 dealt with the effects of a peanut that was either unflavoured or flavoured with SOA or quinine and then paired with a LiCl injection. The oral latency of these gerbils to a nut conveying such flavours was assessed for six days after this conditioning. A nut flavoured with quinine acquired aversive properties but not ones that were flavoured with SOA or were unflavoured. Similar results were observed in Experiment 3, when hamsters were tested under comparable conditions. The theoretical implications of these results are discussed.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号