首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 437 毫秒
1.
The hippocampus mediates associative learning involving spatial and temporal information. Specifically, paired associations in which a trace interval separates the elements appear to be associated within CA1. In contrast, CA3 appears to be involved in associations containing spatial elements. This suggests that CA3, but not CA1, is involved as long as the spatial association does not contain temporal elements; conversely, CA1 is involved when a temporal element is included, regardless of whether there are spatial elements present. In the present study, rats were run on an object-trace-place paired-associate learning paradigm. Rats with CA3 as well as rats with CA1 lesions showed deficits in the acquisition of this task. These results suggest that CA1 is involved in making arbitrary associations involving a temporal (trace) element, whereas CA3 is involved in making associations that involve spatial elements; furthermore, CA1 and CA3 interact in the presence of both spatial and temporal information. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
In the present study, the relationship between performance on temporal and pitch discrimination and psychometric intelligence was investigated in a sample of 164 participants by means of an experimental dissociation paradigm. Performance on both temporal and pitch discrimination was substantially related to psychometric intelligence (r = .43 and r =.39). Regression analysis and structural equation modeling suggested that both psychophysical domains can be considered as valid predictors of psychometric intelligence. Both predictor variables contributed substantial portions of both shared and unique variance to the prediction of individual differences in psychometric intelligence. Thus, the present study yielded further evidence for a functional relationship between psychometric intelligence and temporal as well as pitch discrimination acuity. Eventually, findings are consistent with the notion that temporal discrimination - in addition to general aspects of sensory discrimination shared with pitch discrimination - reflects specific intelligence-related aspects of neural information processing. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
The effect of small bilateral cortical lesions on pure-tone audiograms and on the ability to discriminate between two types of Japanese macaque coo vocalizations was determined in 4 Japanese macaques. A lesion that included the middle portion of the superior temporal gyrus of both hemispheres, that is, the primary and secondary auditory areas, resulted in a partial hearing loss as well as an inability to discriminate the vocalizations. Lesions that included the ventral portions of the superior temporal gyrus of both hemispheres but spared auditory cortex on one side also resulted in a partial hearing loss but had either a small effect or no effect on the ability to discriminate the vocalizations. Bilateral ablation of the dorsal superior temporal gyrus and adjacent parietal and occipital areas did not result in a hearing loss and had no effect on the ability to discriminate the vocalizations. Results suggest that a hearing loss may be produced by lesions that involve small portions of the ventral two-thirds of the superior temporal gyrus bilaterally although the resulting loss is not as great as that observed with larger lesions. However, the aphasia-like deficit appears to result from a lesion of primary and/or secondary auditory cortex. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
In natural visual experience, different views of an object or face tend to appear in close temporal proximity as an animal manipulates the object or navigates around it, or as a face changes expression or pose. A set of simulations is presented which demonstrate how viewpoint-invariant representations of faces can be developed from visual experience by capturing the temporal relationships among the input patterns. The simulations explored the interaction of temporal smoothing of activity signals with Hebbian learning in both a feedforward layer and a second, recurrent layer of a network. The feedforward connections were trained by competitive Hebbian learning with temporal smoothing of the post-synaptic unit activities. The recurrent layer was a generalization of a Hopfield network with a low-pass temporal filter on all unit activities. The combination of basic Hebbian learning with temporal smoothing of unit activities produced an attractor network learning rule that associated temporally proximal input patterns into basins of attraction. These two mechanisms were demonstrated in a model that took grey-level images of faces as input. Following training on image sequences of faces as they changed pose, multiple views of a given face fell into the same basin of attraction, and the system acquired representations of faces that were approximately viewpoint-invariant.  相似文献   

5.
Two experiments examined identification and bisection of tones varying in temporal duration (Experiment 1) or frequency (Experiment 2). Absolute identification of both durations and frequencies was influenced by prior stimuli and by stimulus distribution. Stimulus distribution influenced bisection for both stimulus types consistently, with more positively skewed distributions producing lower bisection points. The effect of distribution was greater when the ratio of the largest to smallest stimulus magnitude was greater. A simple mathematical model, temporal range frequency theory, was applied. It is concluded that (a) similar principles describe identification of temporal durations and other stimulus dimensions and (b) temporal bisection point shifts can be understood in terms of psychophysical principles independently developed in nontemporal domains, such as A. Parducci's (1965) range frequency theory. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Investigated the influence of arousal level on visual preferences by observing the looking preferences of 12 full-term neonates once in a more aroused condition (before feeding while unswaddled) and once in a less aroused condition (after feeding while swaddled). The stimuli were unpatterned light panels illuminated at temporal frequencies of 1, 2, 4, and 8 Hz. Significant linear relationships between amount of looking and stimulus frequency were found in both arousal conditions. However, the effects were in opposite directions in the 2 conditions. Ss looked more as temporal frequency increased when they were less aroused and looked less as temporal frequency increased when more aroused. Thus, even when awake and attending, infants' visual preferences vary systematically with changes in both internal and external amounts of stimulation such that there is an inverse relationship between level of arousal and preferred temporal frequency. (15 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
BACKGROUND: Patients with hypothalamic hamartomas present with epileptic attacks of laughter and later experience multiple seizure types and cognitive decline, suggestive of secondary generalized epilepsy. It has been suggested in the past that gelastic seizures originate in the temporal lobes rather than in the hamartoma, but temporal resections have been ineffective. Recent electrophysiologic evidence suggests that the epileptogenic discharges may originate in the hamartoma itself. METHODS: We used proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging to quantify the amount of neuronal damage in the temporal lobes and hamartomas of patients with hypothalamic hamartomas and gelastic seizures. Five patients were studied and the relative intensity of N-acetylaspartate to creatine (NAA/Cr) was determined for both temporal lobes as well as for the hamartoma. These values were compared with signals from the temporal lobes and hypothalami of normal control subjects. RESULTS: NAA/Cr was not significantly different from normal control subjects for either temporal lobe, nor was there a significant asymmetry between the two temporal lobes for any of the patients. NAA resonance signals were present in the hamartomas, and the ratio of NAA to Cr was decreased in the hamartomas compared with the hypothalami of normal control subjects (t = 4.5, p = 0.005). CONCLUSIONS: We found no detectable neuronal damage in the temporal lobes of patients with hypothalamic hamartomas and gelastic epilepsy. This is further evidence that gelastic seizures do not originate in the temporal lobes of these patients.  相似文献   

8.
We have studied the spatiotemporal receptive-field organization of 144 neurons recorded from the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN) of adult cats and kittens at 4 and 8 wk postnatal. Receptive-field profiles were obtained with the use of a reverse correlation technique, in which we compute the cross-correlation between the action potential train of a neuron and a randomized sequence of long bright and dark bar stimuli that are flashed throughout the receptive field. Spatiotemporal receptive-field profiles of LGN neurons generally exhibit a biphasic temporal response, as well as the classical center-surround spatial organization. For nonlagged cells, the first temporal phase of the response dominates, whereas for lagged neurons, the second temporal phase of the response is typically the largest. This temporal phase difference between lagged and nonlagged cells accounts for their divergent behavior in response to flashed stimuli. Most LGN cells exhibit some degree of space-time inseparability, which means that the receptive field cannot simply be viewed as the product of a spatial waveform and a temporal waveform. In these cases, the response of the surround is typically delayed relative to that of the center, and there is some blending of center and surround during the time course of the response. We demonstrate that a simple extension of the traditional difference-of-Gaussians (DOG) model, in which the surround response is delayed relative to that of the center, accounts nicely for these findings. With regard to development, our analysis shows that spatial and temporal aspects of receptive field structure mature with markedly different time courses. After 4 wk postnatal, there is little change in the spatial organization of LGN receptive fields, with the exception of a weak, but significant, trend for the surround to become smaller and stronger with age. In contrast, there are substantial changes in temporal receptive-field structure after 4 wk postnatal. From 4 to 8 wk postnatal, the shape of the temporal response profile changes, becoming more biphasic, but the latency and duration of the response remain unchanged. From 8 wk postnatal to adulthood, the shape of the temporal profile remains approximately constant, but there is a dramatic decline in both the latency and duration of the response. Comparison of our results with recent data from cortical (area 17) simple cells reveals that the temporal development of LGN cells accounts for a substantial portion of the temporal maturation of simple cells.  相似文献   

9.
In two experiments we addressed the roles of temporal and pitch structures in judgments of melodic phrases. Musical excerpts were rated on how good or complete a phrase they made. In Experiment 1, trials in the temporal condition retained the original temporal pattern but were equitonal; trials in the pitch condition retained the original pitch pattern but were equitemporal; and trials in the melody condition contained both temporal and pitch patterns. In Experiment 2, one pattern (pitch or temporal) was shifted in phase and recombined with the other pattern to create the pitch and temporal conditions. In the melody condition, both patterns were shifted together. In both experiments, ratings in the temporal and pitch conditions were uncorrelated, and the melody condition ratings were accurately predicted by a linear combination of the pitch and temporal condition ratings. These results were consistent across musicians with varying levels of experience. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Stochastic wind velocity fields are usually simulated as N-variate stationary Gaussian processes by using the spectral representation method (SRM). However, the temporal statistics estimated from one SRM-simulated sample wind process cannot coincide with the target characteristics; the disagreements can be described by the bias and stochastic errors. For controlling the errors efficiently, this paper assesses the errors produced by both the Cholesky decomposition-based SRM (CSRM) and the eigendecomposition-based SRM (ESRM). The SRM is revisited first, followed by computing the temporal mean value, correlation function, power spectral density (PSD), and standard deviation of the SRM-simulated wind process. It is shown that the temporal correlation function and standard deviation are Gaussian, while the temporal PSD is non-Gaussian. Further, as mathematical expectations and standard deviations of the corresponding temporal estimations, the bias errors and stochastic errors of all the first- and second-order statistics are obtained in closed form for both the CSRM and the ESRM; the closed-form solutions are then verified in the numerical example. More importantly, this example is employed for taking a clear look at and making a comparison between the stochastic errors produced by the CSRM and by the ESRM; observations suggest that (1) in sum, the ESRM produces smaller stochastic errors than the CSRM and (2) if the ESRM is employed, stochastic errors will be distributed to each component of the wind process in a more uniform pattern. Moreover, some practical approaches are proposed to control the stochastic errors.  相似文献   

11.
Previous studies of the cortical input to the mammalian dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) have identified a number of possible functions for the corticogeniculate pathway, including alteration of LGN spatial frequency selectivity and facilitation of both binocular interactions and orientation selectivity. These changes may be due to either a tonic or a phasic cortical facilitation or both. The temporal differences between each of these inputs suggests that their impact on LGN cell temporal tuning should be unique. To test this hypothesis, we reversibly blocked the visual cortex (VI) and measured the effects on several indices of the temporal properties of LGN cells, including peak frequency, bandwidth, and response phase. Macaque monkeys were anesthetized and paralyzed during single cell recording from the LGN while area VI was cryogenically deactivated. Single-cell responses were visually evoked with drifting, luminance-modulated, sine-wave gratings and discrete-Fourier analyzed. Cortical cooling produced statistically significant increases or decreases in response amplitude in 64% of cells recorded. In most cases, alterations in response amplitude occurred for stimuli that varied in spatial as well as temporal frequency. For those cells influenced by changes in stimulus temporal frequency, the majority showed changes over a broad range of frequencies. A minority of cells showed changes in either peak temporal tuning or temporal frequency bandwidth. Response phase angles for all temporal frequencies tested were unaffected by cortical cooling. Overall, these results suggest that the cortical input may alter the temporal response properties of LGN cells, perhaps by tonic, but not exclusively excitatory, corticofugal influences.  相似文献   

12.
The recognition of high level clinical scenes is fundamental in patient monitoring. In this paper, we propose a technique for recognizing a session, i.e. the clinical process evolution, by comparison against a predetermined set of scenarios, i.e. the possible behaviors for this process. We use temporal constraint networks to represent both scenario and session. Specific operations on networks are then applied to perform the recognition task. An index of temporal proximity is introduced to quantify the degree of matching between two temporal networks in order to select the best scenario fitting a session. We explore the application of our technique, implemented in the Déjà Vu system, to the recognition of typical medical scenarios with both precise and imprecise temporal information.  相似文献   

13.
The durations of sequences of functionally related movements, or action units, were analysed in the baboons Papio hamadryas and Papio anubis. Action units are completed within a narrow time span or temporal segment as found previously in pongids and humans. Although the temporal segmentation is generally similar in the three species, baboons show several differences from both chimpanzees and humans. Firstly, their temporal segments are shorter and less variable and the different sorts of action units, such as hand-body contact or interactions with an object, show slight but significant differences in duration. Secondly, those action units that consist of movements occurring twice last almost twice as long as action units without repetitions. In contrast, in chimpanzees and humans, repetition of a set of movements compresses the first set so that the action unit duration does not increase. This is thought to be due to a form of presyntactical motor planning. Its absence in baboons shows that presyntactical motor planning is confined to those primates with language ability and so provides further support for a relationship between motor and language systems.  相似文献   

14.
Four experiments demonstrated cross-modal transfer (CMT) of classification rules for temporal intervals. In Exp I, 20 male albino Norway rats learned a temporal discrimination between a 2- and an 8-sec signal in one modality (vision or audition). Results show positive transfer to a temporal discrimination between a 2- and an 8-sec signal in the other modality when the response rule was maintained, and negative transfer when the response rule was reversed. Exp II with 20 Ss demonstrated positive CMT in a temporal generalization procedure. Exp III demonstrated CMT of both duration and temporal location in a procedure in which 20 Ss were exposed to 3 successive signal durations. Exp IV demonstrated CMT of both duration and temporal order in a procedure in which 5 Ss were exposed to simultaneously presented signal durations, one auditory and one visual. It is concluded that rats can abstract temporal attributes from modality-specific aspects of a signal. (38 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
We tested the hypothesis that partial forms of retrograde amnesia were associated with highly asymmetric lesions to the inferior and anterior-medial temporal lobe. Postencephalitic subjects EK and DR were both impaired on standardized retrograde memory tests, but showed strikingly different profiles in cognitive tasks of name stem completion, name:face matching, temporal ordering, forced choice recognition, and occupational judgments of famous names and faces from the past 3 decades. EK sustained left inferior and anterior-medial temporal lobe lesion with a small right temporal polar lesion, and showed near-complete loss of retrieval, knowledge, and familiarity associated with famous names but minimal deficiencies with famous faces. DR sustained right inferior and anterior-medial temporal lobe lesion and showed a milder retrograde loss limited to utilizing famous face prompts in name stem completion, name:face matching, occupational judgments, and forced choice recognition. These impairments were also different from the memory retrieval deficit, but intact recognition shown by a case of ruptured anterior communicating artery aneurysm with presumed basal forebrain damage. We hypothesize that EK's extensive loss of famous name knowledge was related to left inferior temporal lobe damage, particularly in the lateral and medial occipitotemporal gyri. This region in the left temporal lobe may serve as a critical processing area for retrograde memory that permits activation of established semantic, temporal, and visual (i.e., facial) associations biographically dependent on the category of proper names. On the basis of connectional anatomy patterns in the nonhuman primate, this region receives extensive hippocampal output and is interconnected with the temporal polar region and cortical association areas, which have been implicated in retrieval and storage aspects of retrograde memory. In the right hemisphere, the occipitotemporal gyri may serve an important role in famous face processing as part of a bilateral neural network.  相似文献   

16.
In the past decade, connectionism has proved its efficiency in the field of static pattern recognition. The next challenge is to deal with spatiotemporal problems. This article presents a new connectionist architecture, RST (réseau spatio temporel [spatio temporal network]), with such spatiotemporal capacities. It aims at taking into account at the architecture level both spatial relationships (e.g., as between neighboring pixels in an image) and temporal relationships (e.g., as between consecutive images in a video sequence). Concerning the spatial aspect, the network is embedded in actual space (two) or three-dimensional-, the metrics of which directly influence its structure through a connection distribution function. For the temporal aspect, we looked toward biology and used a leaky-integrator neuron model with a refractory period and postsynaptic potentials. The propagation of activity by spatiotemporal synchronized waves enables RST to perform motion detection and localization in sequences of video images.  相似文献   

17.
The acoustic startle reflex (ASR) is potentiated during negative emotion, but attenuated during positive emotional experience. The modulation of the ASR by fear depends critically on the amygdala. The authors investigated ASR modulation to fearful, disgusting, pleasant, and neutral stimuli in 12 patients with unilateral damage to the anteromedial temporal lobe including the amygdala (6 left, 6 right), 1 patient with bilateral temporal lobe damage including the amygdala, and 12 comparison participants. Both groups with unilateral damage, as well as the subject with bilateral damage, showed a complete lack of ASR potentiation to both fear and disgust stimuli. The findings suggest that potentiation of the ASR by disgust and fear depends on the integrity of the anteromedial temporal lobe. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
AIMS: To evaluate the inferior to temporal neuroretinal rim width ratio and superior to temporal rim width ratio as measures of rim shape for diagnosis of glaucoma. METHODS: Colour stereo optic disc photographs of 527 normal subjects, 100 ocular hypertensive individuals with normal visual fields, and 202 open angle glaucoma patients with a mean perimetric defect of less than 10 dB were morphometrically evaluated. Eyes with an optic cup area of < 0.2 mm2 were excluded. RESULTS: In the normal subjects, inferior to temporal rim width ratio (1.67 (SD 0.53)) was significantly (p < 0.0001) higher than superior to temporal rim width ratio (1.56 (0.49)). Both ratios were significantly (p < 0.0001) higher the more vertically the optic disc was configured. In the normal eyes, both ratios were statistically independent of disc size, rim area, refractive error, age, and sex. With the differences being more marked for the inferior to temporal ratio than for the superior to temporal ratio, both rim width ratios were significantly (p < 0.005) lower in the ocular hypertensive group than in the normal group. Despite the high significance of the differences, diagnostic power of the inferior ratio and the superior ratio was 59% and 58%, respectively, indicating a marked overlap between the groups. CONCLUSIONS: Abnormally low inferior to temporal and superior to temporal rim width ratios can indicate glaucomatous optic nerve damage in some ocular hypertensive eyes. Being independent of optic disc size and ocular magnification, the rim width ratios may be taken as one among other variables for the ophthalmoscopic optic disc evaluation, taking into account, however, a pronounced overlap between normal eyes and ocular hypertensive eyes.  相似文献   

19.
Although attention is distributed across time as well as space, the temporal allocation of attention has been less well researched than its spatial counterpart. A temporal analog of the covert spatial orientation task [Posner MI, Snyder CRR, Davidson BJ (1980) Attention and the detection of signals. J Exp Psychol Gen 109:160-174] was developed to compare the neural systems involved in directing attention to spatial locations versus time intervals. We asked whether there exists a general system for allocating attentional resources, independent of stimulus dimension, or whether functionally specialized brain regions are recruited for directing attention toward spatial versus temporal aspects of the environment. We measured brain activity in seven healthy volunteers by using positron emission tomography (PET) and in eight healthy volunteers by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The task manipulated cued attention to spatial locations (S) and temporal intervals (T) in a factorial design. Symbolic central cues oriented subjects toward S only (left or right), toward T only (300 msec or 1500 msec), toward both S and T simultaneously, or provided no information regarding S or T. Subjects also were scanned during a resting baseline condition. Behavioral data showed benefits and costs for performance during temporal attention similar to those established for spatial attention. Brain-imaging data revealed a partial overlap between neural systems involved in the performance of spatial versus temporal orientation of attention tasks. Additionally, hemispheric asymmetries revealed preferential right and left parietal activation for spatial and temporal attention, respectively. Parietal cortex was activated bilaterally by attending to both dimensions simultaneously. This is the first direct comparison of the neural correlates of attending to spatial versus temporal cues.  相似文献   

20.
New learning of semantic information is impaired in amnesia. Several reports have demonstrated that "errorless" learning techniques have allowed patients with amnesia to acquire at least some form of new semantic information, although this information appears to be relatively inflexible. Using insights and principles from connectionist modeling of cortical and medial temporal lobe memory systems, the authors describe why errorless learning procedures act as a poor proxy for the medial temporal lobe, suggesting that they artificially eliminate the variability that defines semantic information. The authors trained a patient with severe amnesia on new semantic sentences both with and without variance and then tested him on both repeated and related novel sentences to assess generalization. He successfully learned new semantic information in both conditions but demonstrated better generalization of semantic concepts following training with variance. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号