共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Conducted 3 experiments with 53 gerbils to assess the conditions for ultrasonic emission. Exp I indicates that isolated Ss rarely emitted ultrasounds, males emitted at a higher rate than females, and emission rates ordinarily decreased with continual testing. Ultrasonic emissions were stimulated by olfactory cues from conspecifics. Exp II demonstrates that dominant males emitted more ultrasounds than subordinate males and responded to cues from anesthetized, shaved, or dead animals or from anesthetized animals in the dark. Exp III shows that body hair elicited ultrasounds from males unless organic materials were removed from the hair with chloroform. Hair was an effective stimulus at a distance only when an airstream blew the volatiles over the recipient. (31 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
2.
Adult male Mongolian gerbils formed conditioned social aversions to young male conspecifics. The basic paradigm consisted of pairing an S with a young animal (test stimulus) for 5 min and then injecting the S with LiCl to produce gastrointestinal distress. When retested 48 hrs later, the S avoided the test stimulus, as indicated by significantly decreased investigation time and approach frequency. Through a series of 5 experiments with 290 Ss on dose level, delay of toxin injection, and stimulus generalization, it was demonstrated that a dose of .03 ml/g of .15 M LiCl was effective, the toxin should be given within 15 min of the preliminary social pairing, the social aversion was specific to the particular first paired stimulus animal, and isolation between test sessions was necessary for an aversion to develop. This paradigm extends knowledge of the learned aversion process and demonstrates that this type of learning can be generalized to a social situation by gerbils. (36 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
3.
Noise-localization thresholds and the ability to localize pure tones at 60° separation were determined for gerbils. The gerbils were trained using a two-choice procedure with observing response in which the gerbils made a left or right response to sounds emanating from their left or right side in order to obtain food. The average 75% correct localization threshold of 5 gerbils for a 100-ms noise burst was 27° with chance performance (p?>?.01) reached at 12°. The ability of 4 gerbils to localize both low- and high-frequency pure tones indicates that gerbils are able to use both phase- and intensity-difference locus cues. The frequency at which tone localization was poorest was 2.8 kHz, well below the theoretical frequency of ambiguity of the phase cue but within the frequency range at which phase locking declines in the mammalian auditory system. The sound localization ability of gerbils is typical of small rodents, and there is no obvious sign that it is affected by the degenerative disorder of the central auditory system which has been recently discovered in gerbils. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
4.
Gerbils learned to approach a spatial-olfactory stimulus that signaled access to their pairmate. Experiments 1 and 3 used a discrimination procedure in which 1 conditioned stimulus (the CS+) was presented immediately before access to the pairmate and another (the CS–) was presented alone. Both male and female gerbils came to approach the CS+ sooner than the CS– and spent more time near the CS + than the CS–. Discrimination learning was facilitated by making the CS+ and CS– spatially distinct (Experiment 3). Learning also was demonstrated in male gerbils, using a between-subjects design with a single CS. Pairing the CS with the opportunity for social interaction resulted in greater approach to the CS within 10 trials than presenting the CS and social opportunity in an unpaired fashion (Experiment 2). These findings demonstrate social-affiliative learning in the Mongolian gerbil. Similarities and differences between these findings and sexual conditioning effects in other species are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
5.
Mongolian gerbils were presented with overhead visual stimuli in different environmental situations. The organization of escape movements was investigated with video-based image analysis methods. In Exp 1, gerbils in an open field established a home base and organized their escape trajectories with reference to both the home base and the location of the stimulus. In Exp 2, gerbils were provided pairs of refuges. They used these refuges as home bases and organized their responses with reference to the home base. In Exp 3, pairs of gerbils were tested in a field with refuges. Gerbils organized their escape movements with reference to their distance to the home base in relation to the distance of the other member of the pair to the home base. The results of these experiments suggest that escape movements in gerbils can be characterized as orienting to safety rather than to fleeing from risk. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
6.
Tested D. S. Olton's (1978) hypothesis that the gerbil's ability to easily learn the radial maze task is due to a natural "win-shift" foraging strategy, a tendency to avoid recently visited food sites. 25 Mongolian gerbils at weaning were put in 1 of 2 environments—one in which the source of food and water was always located in the same place and the other in which the source changed location from day to day—until they were 60 days of age. They then were tested in a 17-arm radial maze. Ss reared in the environment with variable locations of food and water learned the maze task more quickly than Ss reared in the other environment. Results are consistent with Olton's hypothesis. (10 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
7.
Investigated whether adult gerbils could use an allocentric frame of reference to efficiently solve a spatial memory task. 38 male Ss were allowed to explore an arena containing an object. The external reference frame was reduced to a single visual landmark. After habituation, Ss entered the arena from a new direction. A 2nd object, identical to the 1st object, was placed symmetrically with regard to the landmark. The 2nd object was explored more than the 1st object, a result showing that the 2nd object was differentiated on the basis of location. This result suggests that, during the course of exploration, Ss had learned about the spatial features of the experimental situation. It is suggested that Ss' final discrimination performance (preference for the novel stimulus location) reflected an allocentric rather than egocentric frame of reference. (15 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
8.
Conducted 3 experiments to determine the ability of infant gerbils to approach an auditory stimulus. In Exp I, 48 16–23 day old Ss were tested in a circular apparatus with a central start area and a movable sound source located at 1 of 8 positions around the perimeter. Stimuli included high- and low-intensity presentations of a tape-recorded gerbil social call, a broad-band white noise stimulus, and a no-stimulus control condition. Ss showed a strong tendency to approach the low-intensity social call and a less pronounced tendency to approach the white noise. In Exp II, 24 16–23 day old Ss were tested in the same apparatus with or without ear blocks to determine the role of binaural cues in directional approach responding. The tendency to approach a low-intensity vocalization was disrupted by obstruction of one ear but not by blocking both ears. Thus, binaural balance was shown to be important for early sound localization. In Exp III, using 6 12–27 day old Ss, the tendency to approach a social call was compared at 12–25, 16–29, 20–23, and 24–27 days after birth. Approach responses were first seen at 16–29 days of age. The responses continued at 20–23 days of age but began to wane at 24–27 days of age. (33 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
9.
Conducted 3 experiments with adult male gerbils to demonstrate that ventral scent marking can act to transfer body heat to the object marked. Exp I, with 16 Ss, showed that surgical removal of the ventral gland pad in 8 Ss reduced the amount of heat transferred by 50%, even though intact and glandless Ss did not differ in the average frequency of scent marking. Exp II, with the same 16 Ss used in Exp I, demonstrated that the difference in heat transfer due to the presence or absence of the scent pad was not due to differential pressure applied to the substrate during marking. Exp III, with 14 Ss, showed that the pattern of ventral hair spread that occurred as an S moved over an object was different between intact and glandless Ss but that this difference did not account for the difference in heat transfer. It is suggested that heat transfer to the environment with ventral scent marking may increase thermoregulatory competence and also may function to volatilize sebum used in chemocommunication. (15 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
10.
Studied depth perception in gerbils and spiny mice with a modified visual cliff that varied the height of a platform from 5.08 to 25.4 cm and presented Ss with an apparent drop-off to a patterned or a white field. Ss were 6 8–22 mo old female gerbils and 12 7–28 mo old spiny mice. Time to descend from the platform and orienting response frequency were recorded. For gerbils, neither measure varied significantly between the platforms. Both mice species increased descent time and orienting response frequency as platform height increased. Results suggest that both gerbils and spiny mice perceived depth, though there appeared to be a difference between the species' use of sensory cues in descending from a visual cliff. The spiny mice appeared to rely more on visual cues than did the gerbils. Data also reveal subtle behavioral differences between the mice species that may relate to their successful sympatry. Results are discussed by alluding to ecological differences between these species in their natural habitats. (24 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
11.
Valsecchi Paola; Choleris Elena; Moles Anna; Guo Cong; Mainardi Marisa 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1996,110(3):243
Experiments were carried out with Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) to assess whether a socially mediated acquisition of diet selection exists in this species. Results showed that a gerbil was influenced in its diet choices by information extracted during a brief period of interaction with a familiar conspecific that had recently eaten a novel food. Data revealed that the acquisition of a food preference from a conspecific depends on the existence of a social bond between the interacting gerbils. Either genetic relatedness (being brother or sister raised in different litters) or familiarity (being bred in the same litter or being member of a reproductive pair) is necessary for the transfer of information. Unfamiliar and unrelated observer gerbils did not selectively choose their demonstrator's food. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
12.
Investigated the functional significance of male gerbil scent-marks in 4 experiments with 60 male and female Mongolian gerbils. In Exps I and II, males in a novel test environment marked more, groomed more, and urinated less in the presence of odors of a strange male than in their absence. Female odors elicited male marking more than did male odors. In Exps III and IV, females were selectively less aggressive toward familiar-smelling males in comparison with unfamiliar-smelling males. These results, in conjunction with field observations of related species, call into question the hypothesis that gerbil scent-marks function territorially and instead suggest that the primary targets are adult females. (34 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
13.
Effects of testosterone (T) on parental behavior of male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) were examined. After undergoing gonadectomy, castrated males were implanted with empty capsules or capsules containing T; sham-operated males were implanted with empty Silastic capsules. Subsequently, each male was paired with a pregnant female, and after delivery, families were observed 15 min/day for 20 days. Gonadectomized males without T spent more time in contact with, huddling over, and licking pups than did either sham-operated males or gonadectomized males with T When given a choice between nest sites and displaced pups, females and males with low T preferred pups, whereas intact males and castrated males with T preferred nest sites. The findings are consistent with E. D. Ketterson and V. Nolan's (1994) hypothesis implicating T in mediating trade-offs between parental and sexual effort. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
14.
Lejeune Helga; Cornet Sabine; Ferreira Mario Augusto; Wearden John H. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1998,24(3):352
Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) received training on a temporal differentiation task, where reinforcement was contingent on remaining on a small platform for more than t, and t was varied over a range from 2 to 15 s. Adjunctive behaviors during platform residence were also observed. Mean time of residence on the platform almost exactly matched the time requirement, and coefficients of variation of residence times remained constant at times above 2 s, in accordance with scalar timing. The rate of adjunctive behaviors varied linearly with reinforcement rate, as predicted by the behavioral theory of timing (BeT; P R. Killeen & G. J. Fetterman, 1988). The number of adjunctive behaviors per platform response and the nature of adjunctive sequences were both, however, less consistent with BeT predictions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
15.
In 4 experiments, the authors explored effects of interaction with both sexually active adult female and unfamiliar adult male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) on young female gerbils' ages at first parturition. Presence of a natural mother retarded development of her daughters. However, presence of a natural mother had no greater effect on her daughters' development than did presence of any other familiar animal, either male or female. Further, exposing young female gerbils to an unfamiliar male accelerated their development even when their reproductively active mothers were present. The data indicate that maintaining young female gerbils in stable family groups results in both inhibition of sexual maturation (caused by exposure to familiar individuals) and failure to activate sexual development (caused by lack of exposure to unfamiliar males). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
16.
Observation data of 20 male adult gerbils indicate that they autogroomed their body parts in the following order: mouth-nose, face, ears, flank, ventrum, and tail. This is the identical order in which the autogroom matures during the 1st 33 days of life. Thus, the sequence of the fixed-action pattern follows the order in which it develops. The parallel sequencing increases the understanding of neurophysiological processes underlying movement patterns and also indicates how movement patterns evolve. (14 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
17.
Replicated an earlier finding by the present authors (see record 1986-08500-001) that female proceptive behaviors toward Harderianectomized males are diminished. In the present study, it was shown that estrous females directed fewer proceptive acts toward males lacking Harderian glands. In addition, it was found that females were more defensively aggressive toward Harderianectomized males and preferred to spend more time in the vicinity of intact males. It is speculated that Harderian spread allows the female to assess the reproductive competence of the male. (8 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
18.
Lumia Augustus R.; Westervelt Miriam O.; Rieder Corinne A. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1975,89(9):1091
Male Mongolian gerbils, selected for high marking frequency, were paired with male opponents, and marking and fighting behavior were recorded. Animals then underwent castration, bilateral bulbectomy, unilateral bulbectomy, the combined operations, or a sham operation; and their behavior was again observed. All operated animals showed drastic reduction in both marking and aggressive encounters. Injections of testosterone propionate (TP) produced complete restoration of marking in castrates, but not in bilaterally or unilaterally bulbectomized animals or combined operates. The exhibition of aggression after injections of TP, however, was enhanced to supernormal levels in bulbectomized or bulbectomized-castrated animals. The results suggest the following: that removal of the olfactory bulbs may eliminate a critical neural input necessary for the normal expression of marking and agonistic behavior; that although marking and fighting are influenced by olfactory input and gonadal steroids, their regulatory mechanisms may not be identical; and that removal of the bulbs may sensitize a neural mechanism controlling aggression, thus potentiating its elicitation following administration of exogenous androgen. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
19.
Carr W. J.; Choi So Young; Arnholt Elizabeth; Sterling Marc H. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1983,97(3):260
During a 1-hr feeding test, 34 hungry male Long-Evans rats that had been reared individually from weaning with a Swiss Webster house mouse were less likely to feed on the intact carcass of a freshly sacrificed mouse than were 39 rats that had been reared individually with another rat. Likewise, 42 hungry mice that had been reared individually with a rat were less likely to feed on a dead rat than were 29 mice that had been reared individually with another mouse. Regardless of the social conditions during rearing, hungry mice were more likely to feed on a dead mouse than were hungry rats to feed on a dead rat. Findings suggest that the tendency by rats to reject conspecific flesh stems, at least in part, from prior experience with conspecifics and with their own bodies. The experiential factors mediating the tendency by mice to reject conspecific flesh remain unclear. (35 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
20.
Reviews the book, Person-centered counselling: An experiential approach by David Rennie (1998). In this book, David Rennie gives priority to the working alliance between counsellor and client, and he outlines various ways in which that alliance can be enhanced. This work began as a training guide, which was eventually expanded to include reflections on the way others have approached central issues in the counselling experience. Rennie draws on a range of theorists such as Carl Rogers and Eugene Gendlen, and makes clear his agreements and disagreements with their positions. In some preliminary chapters, Rennie addresses general issues in counselling before embarking on ideas about counselling itself. He emphasizes, in the Rogerian tradition, the importance of empathy for what clients are feeling as they enter the somewhat unusual context of counselling. The remaining chapters are devoted to the therapeutic craft, including such topics as how to listen to clients, the counsellor's reactions to the client's account of his or her experience, openness and transparency in the relationship with the client, identifying process issues, metacommunication, and the working alliance. This book is a refreshing alternative for practitioners and for students students learning the art of therapeutic intervention. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献