首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The effects of the dopamine receptor antagonist flupenthixol were examined on the development of conditioned partner preference induced by paced copulation in female rats. In Experiment 1, ovariectomized, hormone-primed rats were conditioned to associate scented and unscented males with paced and nonpaced copulation, respectively. Females in Experiment 2 associated albino or pigmented males with paced or nonpaced copulation. Flupenthixol or saline was administered before each conditioning trial. During a final drug-free preference test, females could choose to copulate with either a pacing-related or nonpacing-related male. Saline-trained females copulated preferentially with the pacing-related male, whereas flupenthixol disrupted odor but not strain conditioning. The role of dopamine in conditioned partner preference depends on the type of stimuli to be learned. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Paced copulation induces conditioned place preference in female rats. The authors examined whether associating almond-scented males with paced copulation induces conditioned partner preference. The paired group received 4 paced copulations with almond-scented males and 4 nonpaced copulations with unscented males sequentially at 4-day intervals. The unpaired group received the opposite order of association, whereas the randomly paired group received random associations. A 4th group received a single pairing. On the final test, females were placed into an open field with 2 males, 1 scented and 1 unscented. Females in the paired group solicited the scented male more frequently, and most chose the scented male for their 1st ejaculation. Thus, an odor paired with paced copulation elicits conditioned partner preference in female rats. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Female rats exhibit a conditioned place preference (CPP) for a context paired with mating. The present experiment tested the hypothesis that the activation of the pelvic nerve mediates the reinforcing effects of mating for female rats. Rats underwent bilateral pelvic nerve or sham transection and then received paced mating, nonpaced mating, or the control treatment during a CPP procedure. Pelvic nerve transection did not affect the CPP for paced or nonpaced mating. In tests of paced mating behavior, contact-return latencies following intromissions were significantly shorter in rats with pelvic nerve transection than they were in rats with sham transections. These results show that the pathway conveying the reinforcing effects of mating stimulation does not depend on the integrity of the pelvic nerve, but that activation of the pelvic nerve contributes to the display of paced mating behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Paced mating reduces the aversive properties and increases the positive characteristics of mating, inducing a reward state. Pacing is able to induce conditioned place preference (CPP), whereas nonpaced mating does not. The authors hypothesized that the aversive properties of mating are caused by androgens from adjacent males or from the mother during fetal life. To test whether aromatization of androgens induces the aversive properties of mating, female rats were treated perinatally with 1,4,6-androstatriene-3, 17-dione (ATD) to inhibit aromatization. When adults, these females were ovariectomized and hormonally primed to evaluate CPP after paced and nonpaced mating. During paced mating, control females showed higher return latencies after ejaculation, whereas ATD-treated females did not show a similar increase. In CPP tests, both paced and nonpaced mating induced a reward state in ATD-treated females, whereas only paced mating induced a reward state in control females. These results show that the perinatal inhibition of aromatization enhances the rewarding properties of mating, suggesting that estradiol induced the aversive properties of mating and/or modified the perinatal organization of the neuronal pathways in females. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Sexual partner preference in female rats has been difficult to establish experimentally because the vaginocervical stimulation the female receives during the preference test can be rewarding or aversive depending on the context. G. A. Coria-Avila, A. J. Ouimet, P. Pacheco, J. Manzo, and J. G. Pfaus (2005) (see record 2005-06959-008) reported that female rats can be conditioned to show partner preference for a male that is scented with a sexually neutral odor if they are mated repeatedly with that male in a paced mating test. These results suggest that establishment of a partner preference depends on rewarding characteristics of the vaginocervical stimulation the female receives during an initial mating and that selection of a sexual partner can be determined by olfactory stimuli associated with that stimulation. These results are discussed within the context of the appetitive-consummatory construct of sexual behavior and the evolutionary significance of conditioned partner preference. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
The conditioned responses of male and female Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) were compared in a Pavlovian conditioning procedure in which presentation of a brief conditioned stimulus was immediately followed by the release of a copulation partner. Male quail vigorously approached the conditioned stimulus and were much more likely to enter the compartment housing their copulation partner than were female birds (Experiment 1). In females, sexual conditioning resulted in increased squatting (Experiment 2). This response was the reflection of sexual behavior rather than more general social behavior (Experiment 3). These findings provide the first definitive evidence of sexual learning in female quail and are consistent with the interpretation that sexual conditioning increases sexual arousal or receptivity in both sexes but the increase has different behavioral manifestations in male and female quail. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Sexual behavior in male rats induces a positive affect as evaluated by conditioned place preference (CPP). In addition, when females control or "pace" the rate of sexual interaction, a clear CPP is also observed. The reward state induced by mating in male rats is blocked by the injection of the opioid antagonist naloxone. In the present experiment, a dose of 4 mg/kg of naloxone completely blocked the CPP induced in females by paced mating. It appears that a common opioid system is involved in the positive affect induced by sexual behavior in both male and female rats. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Male rats display a conditioned preference to ejaculate with a female bearing an odor paired previously with copulation to ejaculation. The present study examined the role of endogenous opioid and dopamine systems in this preference. Male rats received saline, the opioid receptor antagonist naloxone, or the dopamine receptor antagonist flupenthixol prior to 10 conditioning trials in a pacing chamber with an almond-scented female. On the final test, all males were injected with saline and given access to 2 females, 1 scented and the other unscented, in an open field. Only males injected with naloxone during training failed to manifest a conditioned ejaculatory preference. These findings suggest that activation of opioid, but not dopamine, systems during sexual interaction are necessary for conditioned ejaculatory preference in male rats. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
The authors assessed the behavioral effects of dopamine (DA) receptor antagonists, Cis (Z) flupentixol and S(+)-raclopride L-tartrate, on conditioned place preference (CPP) induced by paced mating behavior. Ovariectomized female rats of the Wistar strain were used. The administration of amphetamine (1 ior. mg/kg) induced a clear CPP that was completely blocked by the DA antagonists flupentixol (0.25 mg/kg) or raclopride (0.125 mg/kg). These doses had no effect on motor coordination. Female rats that mated in a pacing chamber developed a clear CPP. Neither flupentixol nor raclopride blocked the reward state induced by paced mating behavior. These results indicate that DA is not involved in the reward state induced by paced mating behavior in female rats. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Two experiments, with 121 female Long-Evans rats, investigated sexual behavior in intact cycling Ss and ovariectomized and ovariectomized-adrenalectomized hormone-primed Ss. A partitioned test cage was used in which the female controlled the timing of sexual contacts with males. Females received 9 or 10 intromissions in the partitioned test cage (paced) or with the partition removed (nonpaced), or they received solitary exposure to the test cage or to mounts without intromission with the use of vaginal masks. Intact cycling (Exp I) and gonadectomized hormone-primed (Exp II) Ss displayed similar patterns of contact with males. Exits from and latencies to return to the male compartment increased as the intensity of the antecedent coital stimulation increased. Cycling Ss given experience with paced or nonpaced mating on the evening of proestrus did not exhibit differences in pacing behavior on a 2nd test 17–24 days later. Those receiving paced coital stimulation showed a shorter duration of estrus than did those receiving nonpaced stimulation. Gonadectomized Ss given 3 successive doses of estradiol benzoate (20, 40, and 8 μg/kg, sc) in combination with progesterone (2 mg/kg) did not show shorter periods of estrus than nonpaced or mounts-only Ss. Results suggest that ovarian output in response to paced cervical-vaginal stimulation may contribute to the termination of estrus in the rat. (39 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Male rats, neonatally treated with ATD (1,4,6-androstatriene-3,17-dione), which blocks the aromatization of testosterone into estradiol (E?), were tested for adult partner preference behavior (PPB; estrous female vs active male). Castration caused a decrease in preference for the female partner in all males, with ATD males showing lower preference for the female partner than controls. Long-term castrated males did not show preference for either partner. Precastration levels of PPB in control males occurred after treatment with E? or dihydrotestosterone (DHT) plus E?. DHT alone had no effect on PPB. With E? alone, the ATD males clearly preferred the male partner. When DHT was added, these ATD males showed no preference for either partner or a low preference for the female partner. In conclusion, adult PPB in male rats is activated by endogenous testosterone or by both its metabolites (DHT and E?) or by E? alone. ATD males showed a much lower preference for the female. There was a differential effect of DHT and E?: DHT had no effect, but E? clearly caused ATD to prefer the male partner and control males to prefer the female partner. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
The effects of differential mating stimulation on sexual behavior and estrus length were examined in cycling rats that could or could not self-regulate, or pace, the timing of sexual contact. Female rats (Rattus norvegicus) received 30 paced, 30 nonpaced, or 15 nonpaced followed by 15 paced intromissions during mating tests. Decreases in sexual responsiveness were seen during the 2nd half of testing; pacing was associated with greater inter-intromission intervals, decreased proceptivity, and increased rejection behavior at this time. Female rats pacing during the 2nd test half behaved similarly, regardless of prior treatment, showing that the number rather than the timing of prior intromissions affected subsequent behavior. However, estrus length was decreased by prior paced mating. These data suggest that changes in sexual responsivity occur throughout estrus and that the nature of these changes is differentially dependent on the type of mating stimulation received. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Describes 3 experiments with a total of 74 gonadally-normal male and 29 ovariectomized female Long-Evans rats. Male and female reactions toward odors from novel and original partners were observed prior to the male's attaining his 1st or 2nd ejaculation. The male's reaction depended upon the sexual condition of the female. Only prior to their initial ejaculation did the males prefer their original partner's odors to those of novel females and only if the odors were collected from the females prior to copulation. This finding corresponded with observations of the male's random choice of partner during copulation. Females responded nonpreferentially if they had copulated prior to testing and showed a marked decrease in responsiveness as copulation continued. Prior to copulation females preferred the odors from males which had not copulated to those of males which had. (25 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
The possibility that female-paced coital behavior induces a reward state of sufficient intensity and duration to induce conditioning was evaluated by the conditioned-place-preference paradigm. Ovariectomized female rats, treated with estradiol benzoate and progesterone, regulated (paced) their coital interactions with a stud male through a 2-compartment chamber in which only the female could freely move from one compartment to the other. The females that paced their coital interactions showed a clear place preference. In contrast, no change in preference was observed in the females that could not pace their coital contacts. The change in preference in the females that paced their coital interactions was similar to that produced by an injection of morphine (1 mg/kg). These results suggest that coital interactions in females can induce a reward state when the females can control the pace of the sexual interaction. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
This study involved 2 tests of conditioned reward with self-stimulation (SS) of the prefrontal cortex. In Experiment 1, rats were tested for a conditioned taste preference (CTP) induced by pairing a novel flavor with SS of the medial prefrontal cortex (MC). Normal rats displayed a CTP. Rats with bilateral cuts of the connections between the MC and sulcal prefrontal cortex (SC) did not show a CTP. In Experiment 2, similar cuts had no effect on the ability of SC SS to promote a CTP, showing that the cuts spare the ability to learn a CTP. In Experiment 3, rats were tested for a conditioned place preference (CPP) by pairing MC SS with environmental cues. Lesioned rats, but not intact rats, had a CPP. Results suggest the presence of prepotent relations, dependent on intrinsic prefrontal connections, between the rewarding effects of prefrontal stimulation and distinct sensorimotor domains. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
Access to novelty might provide an alternative learning history that competes with conditioned drug reward. We tested this suggestion in rats using a place conditioning procedure with cocaine and novelty. In Experiment 1, rats were conditioned with cocaine to prefer one side of an apparatus. In a subsequent phase, cocaine exposure continued; however, on the unpaired side, separate group of rats had access to novel objects, cocaine injections, or saline with no objects. Pairings with novel objects or cocaine shifted a preference away from the cocaine-paired environment during drug-free and drug-challenge tests. Experiment 2 tested novelty's impact when cocaine exposure was discontinued. The identical procedures were used except drug exposure ceased on the cocaine-paired side during the second phase. Both groups expressed a preference for the cocaine compartment. This preference was maintained for rats that did not have novel objects; however, rats that experienced novelty spent similar amounts of time in both compartments during both tests. Overall, the conditioned rewarding effects of novelty competed with those of cocaine as evidenced by a change in choice behaviors motivated by drug reward. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Ejaculation-induced reward in the male rat was evaluated by the conditioned place-preference paradigm. It was supposed that ejaculation induces a reward state such that it can be conditioned to environmental stimuli. Males were allowed to ejaculate once and were then immediately transferred to a place-preference cage. One ejaculation produced place preference. Naloxone (16 mg/kg) not only blocked this place preference but also induced a place aversion. Naloxone by itself had no effect on place preference. It is suggested that release of endogenous opioids renders ejaculation rewarding. Pimozide, in a dose of 1 mg/kg, had no effect on ejaculation-induced reward. Dopamine thus seems to be of slight importance for that effect of copulation. Perhaps compulsive sexual activity obeys the same mechanisms as compulsive drug use in opiate addicts. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
In 3 experiments rats given 8 sessions of preexposure to wheel running acquired a preference for a flavor that was given immediately after each of 4 subsequent sessions of wheel running. Such flavor preference was less likely when rats were given the same conditioning procedure but without preexposure to wheels (Experiment 1) or when access to flavor was delayed by 30 min following a wheel session (Experiment 2). When rats were given a flavor before each wheel session, the resulting conditioned aversion was greater in rats that had no prior exposure to wheel running (Experiment 3). These results show that whether an aversion or preference for a flavor is produced by wheel running depends on an interaction between prior wheel experience and the sequence of events. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Rats with extensive ibotenic acid lesions centered in the gustatory zone of the pontine parabrachial nucleus (PBN) failed to acquire a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) induced by lithium chloride (LiCl) toxicosis (Experiments 1 and 4). This deficit cannot be explained as an inability to either perceive or process gustatory information because lesioned rats that failed to acquire a CTA readily acquired a conditioned flavor preference (Experiment 2). Similarly, the CTA deficit cannot be attributed to an inability to experience or process visceral input because PBN-lesioned rats that failed to acquire a CTA successfully learned an aversion to a trigeminal stimulus, capsaicin, when paired with LiCl-induced illness (Experiment 3). This pattern of results supports the view that cell bodies within the PBN are essential for the associative processes that govern CTA learning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Previous studies have demonstrated that central administration of vasopressin but not oxytocin facilitates pair bonding in the monogamous male prairie vole. This study tested vasopressin and oxytocin in the formation of the female vole's preference for a particular male partner. Initial studies showed that in monogamous female prairie voles (but not in nonmonogamous congeners), mating was followed by a partner preference that endured for at least 2 weeks. Nonmating prairie vole females developed a partner preference following oxytocin infusions, but not after vasopressin or cerebrospinal fluid infusions. Females given a selective oxytocin antagonist showed normal mating behavior, yet failed to develop a partner preference. The vasopressin antagonist failed to block partner preference formation in mated females. These results suggest that oxytocin, released with mating, may be critical to formation of a partner preference in the female prairie vole; this contrasts to vasopressin, which appears to be more important for pair bonding in the male of this species. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号