首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Isradipine and darodipine are dihydropyridine calcium antagonists that affect the serotonergic pathways with a peculiar profile of effects because, at low dose (0.08 and 0.3 mg/kg, respectively) they facilitate, but at high dose (1.60 and 5.0 mg/kg, respectively) they inhibit the serotonergic neurotransmission. To investigate the mechanisms of these effects, the selective 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OHDPAT was injected S.C. to rats pretreated I.P. with isradipine (0.04-1.60 mg/kg) or darodipine (0.3-5.0 mg/kg). By stimulating presynaptic 5-HT1A autoreceptor, 8-OHDPAT induced signs of inhibition of the serotonergic neutransmission (i.e., decrease of the 5-HIIA/5-HT ratio), but it also produced behavioral effects by stimulating postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors (i.e., forepaw treadings). A low dose of isradipine (0.08 mg/kg) or darodipine (0.3 mg/kg) antagonized the presynaptic, but enhanced the postsynaptic effects of 8-OHDPAT, suggesting relief of the autoreceptor-mediated inhibition of the 5-HT release. Thus, the amine released could stimulate postsynaptic receptors, adding its action to that of 8-OHDPAT. A high dose of isradipine (1.60 mg/kg) or darodipine (5.0 mg/kg) left unchanged, or also enhanced, the signs of inhibition of serotonergic neurotransmission displayed by 8-OHDPAT, reducing but not suppressing the increase in the behavioral response to the stimulation of postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors. It was speculated that the effects of isradipine and darodipine on scrotonergic pathways of rat brain could be due to changes in the back-regulation of the neurotransmission, mediated by 5-HT1A autoreceptors. This mechanism of action could be extended to other dihydropyridine calcium antagonists, because blockade of L-type VSCC by these compounds appears to be involved in their effects on brain 5-HT turnover.  相似文献   

2.
In 2 Experiments, startle amplitude and startle stimulus-induced freezing (an index of fear) were measured in an acoustic startle response (ASR) paradigm in rats. Lesions to lateral tegmental tract (LTG), a pathway medial to brachium of the inferior colllciulus (BIC), significantly decreased freezing and produced a persistent 5-fold increase in ASR amplitude compared with sham-operated controls. Lesions to BIC increased both ASR amplitude (2-fold) and freezing. Neither BIC not LTG lesions affected startle amplitude when startle was elicited by a brief footshock stimulus. Characteristics of the lesion effects were tested with manipulations of interstimulus interval, stimulus intensity, and prepulse inhibition. The data suggest (a) an ascending pathway medial to BIC that carries the fear-inducing dimensions of an acoustic stimulus and (b) a descending pathway that provides tonic inhibition of the sensory input to the ASR circuitry. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
When the benzodiazepine inverse agonist DMCM (6,7-dimethoxy-4-ethyl-β-carboline-3-carboxylic acid methyl ester) occupies the benzodiazepine recognition site on the GABAA receptor complex, the inhibitory action of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) is attenuated. DMCM acted as an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) for 1 response associated with fear or anxiety, analgesia, as indicated by a dose-dependent (0.25–2.0 mg/kg) suppression of rats' responses to a formalin injection. This was accompanied by other fearlike responses (defecation and urination). The opioid antagonist naltrexone (1.75–24 mg/kg) did not affect these behaviors. Environmental cues associated with DMCM provoked analgesia and defecation in the absence of the drug. The conditional analgesia was reversed by naltrexone (7 mg/kg). DMCM functions as an unconditional fear stimulus by eliciting fear-related behaviors and conditioning those responses to neutral stimuli. The neural circuitry underlying fear conditioning appears to involve tonically inhibitory GABAergic synapses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
In view of the fact that the amygdala (AMG) and hypothalamic serotonin (5-HT) have an excitatory effect on the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, we have investigated the role of 5-HT in the AMG on this response. In intact freely moving rats, a mildly stressing short photic stimulation caused depletion of median eminence CRH-41, due to its release into the portal circulation and a rise in serum ACTH and corticosterone levels. This effect was significantly inhibited in rats in which 5-HT was depleted in the AMG following local 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine administration, which did not affect hypothalamic 5-HT content. Also, local pretreatment with ketanserin (a 5-HT2 receptor antagonist) in the AMG had the same inhibitory effect on the HPA axis response. These results indicate that AMG 5-HT has an important role in the activation of the HPA axis following neural stimulation and that 5-HT2 receptors are involved.  相似文献   

5.
Three experiments support the hypothesis that mechanisms involved in observational conditioning (OC) of fear are similar to those of direct classical conditioning and involve the organism attempting to detect the causal structure of its environment. Exp 1, a correlational analysis, shows that model monkeys' fear behaviors on snake trials (unconditioned stimulus [UCS]) were highly correlated with observer monkeys' fear (unconditioned response [UCR]) while watching the models' fear. In Exp 2, all observers showed distress while watching the model's fear during Session 1 of OC, but only observers who could see the snake to which the model was reacting continued to show fear during subsequent OC sessions, suggesting that the model's fear is an easily habituable UCS. In Exp 3, observers acquired significant fear of snakes after 1 OC session, indicating that the continued fear of those Exp 2 observers that could see the snake may reflect their own acquired fear of snakes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
The hypothesis that the standard acoustic startle habituation paradigm contains the elements of Pavlovian fear conditioning was tested. In a potentiated startle response paradigm, a startle stimulus and a light conditioned stimulus (CS) were paired. A startle stimulus then was tested alone or following the CS. Freezing behavior was measured to index conditioned fear. The startle response was potentiated on CS trials, and rats froze more in CS than in non-CS periods. In Experiment 1, response to a previously habituated, weak startle stimulus was potentiated. In Experiment 2, response to the same stimulus used as the unconditioned stimulus (US) in training was potentiated. This CS-potentiated response retarded the course of response decrements over training sessions as compared with an explicitly unpaired control group. Conditioned fear is a standard feature of this habituation paradigm, serves to potentiate the startle response, and provides an associative dimension lacking in the habituation process per se. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
A contextual reinstatement procedure was developed to assess the contributions of environmental cues and hippocampal function in the recovery of conditioned fear following extinction in humans. Experiment 1 showed context specificity in the recovery of extinguished skin conductance responses after presentations of an auditory unconditioned stimulus. Experiment 2 demonstrated that fear recovery did not generalize to an explicitly unpaired conditioned stimulus. Experiment 3 replicated the context dependency of fear recovery with a shock as an unconditioned stimulus. Two amnesic patients failed to recover fear responses following reinstatement in the same context, despite showing initial fear acquisition. These results extend the known functions of the human hippocampus and highlight the importance of environmental contexts in regulating the expression of latent fear associations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
The hippocampus is believed to be an important structure for learning tasks that require temporal processing of information. The trace classical conditioning paradigm requires temporal processing because the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US) are temporally separated by an empty trace interval. The present study sought to determine whether the hippocampus was necessary for rats to perform a classical trace fear conditioning task in which each of 10 trials consisted of an auditory tone CS (1 5-s duration) followed by an empty 30-s trace interval and then a fear-producing floor-shock US (0.5-s duration). Several weeks prior to training, animals were anesthetized and given aspiration lesions of the neocortex (NEO; n = 6), hippocampus and overlying neocortex (HIPP; n = 7), or no lesions at all (control; n = 6). Approximately 24 h after trace conditioning, NEO and control animals showed a significant decrease in movement to a CS-alone presentation that was indicative of a conditioned fear response. Animals in the HIPP group did not show conditioned fear responses to the CS alone, nor did a pseudoconditioning group (n = 7) that was trained with unpaired CSs and USs. Furthermore, all groups except the HIPP group showed conditioned fear responses to the original context in which they received shock USs. One week later, HIPP, NEO, and control animals received delay fear-conditioning trials with no trace interval separating the CS and US. Six of seven HIPP animals could perform the delay version, but none could perform the trace version. This result suggests that the trace fear task is a reliable and useful model for examining the neural mechanisms of hippocampally dependent learning.  相似文献   

9.
The presentation of a neutral or conditioned stimulus (CS) at an appropriate interval prior to the presentation of a corneal airpuff or a paraorbital shock (unconditioned stimulus, US) can facilitate the amplitude of the unconditioned nictitating membrane (NM) response in rabbits. In two experiments, it was demonstrated that an associative process mediates the maintenance of that facilitation during repeated CS–US pairings. Although CS-alone presentations produced a substantial decrease in the amount of reflex facilitation in animals not pretrained with the CS, pretraining that consisted of paired CS–US presentations prevented that decrease when CS-alone presentations were subsequently given. Conditioned facilitation of the unconditioned response occurred very rapidly (within 5–22 trials in these experiments) and long before the appearance of overt conditioned responses to the CS. In addition, it was demonstrated that conditioned facilitation can be relatively specific to the tonal frequency of the CS. These results indicate the first sign of conditioning of the NM response is exhibited in the amplitude of the unconditioned response. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Conditioned fear in rats was assessed for the effects of pretraining amygdala lesions (unilateral vs. bilateral) across unconditioned stimulus (US) modalities (white noise vs. shock). In contrast to sham controls, unilateral amygdala lesions significantly reduced conditioned freezing responses, whereas bilateral amygdala lesions resulted in a nearly complete lack of freezing to both the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the context. The lesion effects were more pronounced for CS conditioning but were consistent across US modalities. It was concluded that white noise can serve as an effective US and that unilateral amygdala lesions attenuate but do not eliminate conditioned fear in rats. The results support our interpretation of a recent fear conditioning study in humans (K. S. LaBar, J. E. LeDoux, D. D. Spencer, & E. A. Phelps, 1995).  相似文献   

11.
Trace fear conditioning is a hippocampus-dependent learning task that requires the association of an auditory conditioned stimulus (CS) and a shock unconditioned stimulus (US) that are separated by a 20-s trace interval. Single-neuron activity was recorded simultaneously from the dentate gyrus (DG) and CA1 of rats during unpaired pseudoconditioning and subsequent trace fear conditioning. Single neurons in DG showed a progressive increase in learning-related activity to the CS and US across trace fear conditioning. Single neurons in CA1 showed an early increase in responding to the CS, which developed into a decrease in firing later in trace conditioning. Correlation analyses showed that DG and CA1 units exhibit inverse patterns of responding to the CS during trace fear conditioning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
"Physiological memory" is enduring neuronal change sufficiently specific to represent learned information. It transcends both sensory traces that are detailed but transient and long-term physiological plasticities that are insufficiently specific to actually represent cardinal details of an experience. The specificity of most physiological plasticities has not been comprehensively studied. We adopted receptive field analysis from sensory physiology to seek physiological memory in the primary auditory cortex of adult guinea pigs. Receptive fields for acoustic frequency were determined before and at various retention intervals after a learning experience, typified by single-tone delay classical conditioning, e.g., 30 trials of tone-shock pairing. Subjects rapidly (5-10 trials) acquire behavioral fear conditioned responses, indexing acquisition of an association between the conditioned and the unconditioned stimuli. Such stimulus-stimulus association produces receptive field plasticity in which responses to the conditioned stimulus frequency are increased in contrast to responses to other frequencies which are decreased, resulting in a shift of tuning toward or to the frequency of the conditioned stimulus. This receptive field plasticity is associative, highly specific, acquired within a few trials, and retained indefinitely (tested to 8 weeks). It thus meets criteria for "physiological memory." The acquired importance of the conditioned stimulus is thought to be represented by the increase in tuning to this stimulus during learning, both within cells and across the primary auditory cortex. Further, receptive field plasticity develops in several tasks, one-tone and two-tone discriminative classical and instrumental conditioning (habituation produces a frequency-specific decrease in the receptive field), suggesting it as a general process for representing the acquired meaning of a signal stimulus. We have proposed a two-stage model involving convergence of the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli in the magnocellular medial geniculate of the thalamus followed by activation of the nucleus basalis, which in turn releases acetylcholine that engages muscarinic receptors in the auditory cortex. This model is supported by several recent findings. For example, tone paired with NB stimulation induces associative, specific receptive field plasticity of at least a 24-h duration. We propose that physiological memory in auditory cortex is not "procedural" memory, i.e., is not tied to any behavioral conditioned response, but can be used flexibly.  相似文献   

13.
In vivo microdialysis was used to compare the effects of serotonergic drugs on morphine- and cocaine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine (DA) concentrations in the rat nucleus accumbens (NAc). Systemic administration of the 5-HT2A/2C agonist, 1-(2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodophenyl)-2-aminopropane (DOI) (2.5 mg/kg, s.c. ) prevented the increase in extracellular DA in the NAc produced by morphine (5 mg/kg, i.p.). In contrast, this dose of DOI had no effect on the ability of cocaine (10 mg/kg, i.p.) to increase extracellular DA concentrations in the NAc. A 5-HT2C selective agonist, 6-chloro-2-[1-piperazinyl]-pyrazine (MK-212, 5 mg/kg, s.c.) also inhibited morphine-induced increases in extracellular DA concentrations in the NAc. Pretreatment of rats with the selective 5-HT2A antagonist, amperozide, had no effect on morphine-induced elevation of NAc DA concentrations. In order to determine if inhibition of the firing of 5-HT neurons contributes to the serotonin agonist-mediated inhibition of morphine-induced accumbens DA release, rats were pretreated with the 5-HT1A agonist, 8-OHDPAT. At a dose of 100 microg/kg (sc), 8-OHDPAT did not interfere with morphine's ability to increase DA concentrations in the NAc. These results suggest that the activation of 5-HT2C receptors selectively inhibits morphine-induced DA release in the NAc in a manner which is independent of the inhibition of 5-HT neurons.  相似文献   

14.
Agonists acting at the serotonin-1B receptor (5-HT?BR) and 5-HT?CR have been reported to potentiate and block, respectively, the discriminative stimulus effects of cocaine. The present investigation reassessed the antagonistic effects of the mixed 5-HT?C/?BR agonist m-chlorophenylpiperazine (mCPP) on the discriminative stimulus effects of cocaine in the presence or absence of selective antagonism of the 5-HT?BR or 5-HT?CR. The stimulus effects of cocaine were attenuated by mCPP at doses that reduced response rates. The selective 5-HT?CR antagonist SB 242084, but not the selective 5-HT?BR antagonist GR 127935, reversed the mCPP-evoked attenuation of the cocaine cue and the suppression of response rates. These results demonstrate that the suppressive effects of mCPP on cocaine discrimination are related to stimulation of the HT?CR. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Chemical stimulation of the inferior colliculus (IC) with semicarbazide--an inhibitor of the gamma aminobutyric acid synthesizing enzyme--functions as an unconditioned stimulus in the conditioned place aversion test (CPA), and electrolytic lesions of the basolateral amygdala (BLA) enhance the aversiveness of the IC stimulation. This study examined the effects of inactivation of the BLA with muscimol on the conditioned and unconditioned fear using semicarbazide injections into the IC of rats subjected to conditioned (CPA) or unconditioned (open field) fear tests. In both tests, the rats were injected with semicarbazide or saline into the IC and muscimol or saline into the BLA. Muscimol decreased the CPA and increased the unconditioned fear elicited by IC injections of semicarbazide. These findings indicate that distinct modulatory mechanisms in the BLA are recruited during the conditioned and unconditioned fear triggered by IC activation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
The authors investigated the role of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in the inhibition of conditioned fear in rats using both Pavlovian extinction and conditioned inhibition paradigms. In Experiment 1, lesions of ventral mPFC did not interfere with conditioned inhibition of the fear-potentiated startle response. In Experiment 2, lesions made after acquisition of fear conditioning did not retard extinction of fear to a visual conditioned stimulus (CS) and did not impair "reinstatement" of fear after unsignaled presentations of the unconditioned stimulus. In Experiment 3, lesions made before fear conditioning did not retard extinction of fear-potentiated startle or freezing to an auditory CS. In both Experiments 2 and 3, extinction of fear to contextual cues was also unaffected by the lesions. These results indicate that ventral mPFC is not essential for the inhibition of fear under a variety of circumstances. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Heart rate conditioning is used as an index of conditioned fear and is important for understanding disorders of anxiety and stress, including post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). One important feature of PTSD is that patients generalize conditioned fear from danger signals to safety signals especially when the two signals have overlapping features. What has not been determined is whether generalization occurs between unconditioned stimuli with overlapping features. In the current experiment, heart rate conditioning and conditioning-specific reflex modification of rabbit heart rate were examined as a function of two different unconditioned stimulus locations. Heart rate conditioning occurred at identical terminal levels whether electrical stimulation was presented near the eye or on the back. Despite different heart rate response topographies to electrical stimulation at the two locations, conditioning-specific reflex modification was detected near the eye and on the back and appeared to generalize between the locations. Interestingly, only conditioning-specific reflex modification detected on the back persisted for a week after heart rate conditioning. This persistence may be a model for some features of post traumatic stress disorder. Overgeneralization of unconditioned responses to unconditioned stimuli similar to the trauma may also be an important aspect of PTSD modeled here. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
The basolateral (BLA) and medial nucleus (MeA) of the amygdala participate in the modulation of unconditioned fear induced by predator odor. However, the specific role of these amygdalar nuclei in predator odor-induced fear memory is not known. Therefore, fiber-sparing lesions or temporary inactivation of the BLA or MeA were made either prior to or after exposure to cat odor, and conditioned contextual fear behavior was examined the next day. BLA and MeA lesions produced significant reductions in cat odor-induced unconditioned and conditioned fear-related behavior. In addition, temporary pharmacological neural inactivation methods occurring after exposure to cat odor revealed subtle behavioral alterations indicative of a role of the BLA in fear memory consolidation but not memory retrieval. In contrast, the MeA appears to play a specific role in retrieval but not consolidation. Results show that the BLA participates in the conditioned and unconditioned cat odor stimulus association that underlies fear memory, underscore a novel role of the MeA in predator odor contextual conditioning, and demonstrate different roles of the BLA and MeA in modulating consolidation and retrieval of predator odor fear memory. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues was examined in Gulf War veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) by use of the startle reflex methodology. Veterans participated in a differential aversive conditioning experiment consisting of 2 sessions separated by 4 or 5 days. Each session comprised two startle habituation periods, a preconditioning phase, a conditioning phase, and a postconditioning extinction test. In contrast to the non-PTSD group, the PTSD group showed a lack of differential startle response in the presence of a conditioned stimulus with or without an unconditioned stimulus in Session 1 and an increase in the baseline startle response during Session 2. The PTSD group also exhibited normal differential conditioning following reconditioning in Session 2. These data suggest that individuals with PTSD tend to generalize fear across stimuli and are sensitized by stress. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Pindolol has been shown to be a partial agonist at 5-HT1A receptors in preclinical studies. It has also been reported to inhibit the effects of other 5-HT1A partial agonists such as ipsapirone and buspirone on hormone secretion and body temperature in man, indicating its antagonist action at 5-HT1A receptors in man. To determine if pindolol has 5-HT1A agonist as well as antagonist effects in man, pindolol, 30 mg, p.o. and placebo, were given single blind in random order to 23 normal men with indwelling venous catheters and its effects on hormone secretion and body temperature noted. Pindolol significantly increased basal plasma cortisol concentrations, whereas it decreased plasma prolactin (PRL) concentrations and body temperature. The increase in plasma cortisol due to pindolol suggests a 5-HT1A agonist action and is consistent with a 5-HT1A partial agonist mechanism in man whereas the PRL effects are consistent with an antagonist action at 5-HT1A receptors. The effects of pindolol on plasma cortisol concentration and body temperature were significantly negatively correlated. Furthermore, these results indicate significant differences in the 5-HT1A-dependent regulation of PRL and the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and body temperature, and suggest that human basal PRL secretion is tonically stimulated by 5-HT1A mechanism whereas the HPA axis and body temperature are not. Since rodent studies suggest differences in 5-HT1A receptor sensitivity between males and females, the results reported here need to be replicated in females. These differences in the effect of pindolol are discussed in terms of receptor reserve theory.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号