共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Electric charge has long been hypothesized to be the effective stimulus variable that determines loudness evoked by directly stimulating the auditory nerve. This 'equal-charge, equal-loudness' hypothesis predicts that stimulus amplitude and duration can be traded linearly to produce equal loudness. Loudness sensations from threshold to maximum loudness were measured systematically as a function of stimulus amplitude and duration in cochlear implant listeners. The measured data do not support the equal-charge, equal-loudness hypothesis: an increment in stimulus amplitude produces a significantly louder sensation than the same change in stimulus duration. Instead of the linear equal-charge model, a power-function model successfully predicts the measured data and should be used to encode loudness in electric hearing. 相似文献
2.
Listening to relatively intense tones at one frequency and weak tones at another makes the latter relatively louder. The auditory system's relative response to low-frequency (f?) and high-frequency (f?) tones depends on the separation between f? and f?. When f? and f? differ little, loudness matches change little with shifts in mean sound pressure levels (SPLs) at each frequency; but when f? and f? differ more, matches change markedly, showing how the auditory system "recalibrates" its responses to f? and f?. The magnitude of recalibration and its frequency bandwidth also depend to a modest degree on the range of SPLs, their mean level, and the experimental paradigm. The representation of loudness reflects the processing and recalibration of multidimensional peripheral inputs within a higher level, context-sensitive (adaptation-like) mechanism. Other perceptual modalities show evidence of analogous mechanisms. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
3.
Roberts Brian; Glasberg Brian R.; Moore Brian C. J. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2008,34(4):992
[Correction Notice: An erratum for this article was reported in Vol 34(6) of Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (see record 2008-16831-007). The year listed is incorrect. The article should have been dated 2008.] The tendency to hear a tone sequence as 2 or more streams (segregated) builds up, but a sudden change in properties can reset the percept to 1 stream (integrated). This effect has not hitherto been explored using an objective measure of streaming. Stimuli comprised a 2.0-s fixed-frequency inducer followed by a 0.6-s test sequence of alternating pure tones (3 low [L]-high [H] cycles). Listeners compared intervals for which the test sequence was either isochronous or the H tones were slightly delayed. Resetting of segregation should make identifying the anisochronous interval easier. The HL frequency separation was varied (0-12 semitones), and properties of the inducer and test sequence were set to the same or different values. Inducer properties manipulated were frequency, number of onsets (several short bursts vs. one continuous tone), tone:silence ratio (short vs. extended bursts), level, and lateralization. All differences between the inducer and the L tones reduced temporal discrimination thresholds toward those for the no-inducer case, including properties shown previously not to affect segregation greatly. Overall, it is concluded that abrupt changes in a sequence cause resetting and improve subsequent temporal discrimination. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
4.
5.
Auditory spatial acuity was measured in mice using prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle reflex as the indicator response for stimulus detection. The prepulse was a “speaker swap” (SSwap), shifting a noise between two speakers located along the azimuth. Their angular separation, and the spectral composition and sound level of the noise were varied, as was the interstimulus interval (ISI) between SSwap and acoustic startle reflex elicitation. In Experiment 1 a 180° SSwap of wide band noise (WBN) was compared with WBN Onset and Offset. SSwap and WBN Onset had near equal effects, but less than Offset. In Experiment 2 WBN SSwap was measured with speaker separations of 15, 22.5, 45, and 90°. Asymptotic level and the growth rate of PPI increased with increased separation from 15 to 90°, but even the 15° SSwap provided significant PPI for the mean performance of the group. SSwap in Experiment 3 used octave band noise (2–4, 4–8, 8–16, or 16–32 kHz) and separations of 7.5 to 180°. SSwap was most effective for the highest frequencies, with no significant PPI for SSwap below 8–16 kHz, or for separations of 7.5°. In Experiment 4 SSwap had WBN sound levels from 40 to 78 dB SPL, and separations of 22.5, 45, 90, and 180°: PPI increased with level, this effect varying with ISI and angular separation. These experiments extend the prior findings on sound localization in mice, and the dependence of PPI on ISI adds a reaction time-like dimension to this behavioral analysis. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
6.
Using single and multislice functional MRI at high field strength (4 T) we studied cerebellar activation in 12 subjects making a series of alternating wrist flexion and extension movements against constant inertial loads. Three spatial patterns of activation were observed: (i) parasagittal bands of activity localized primarily in the ipsilateral intermediate and lateral zones of the cerebellar hemispheres, (ii) medio-lateral bands which in some subjects followed the contour of individual folia and (iii) fragmented regions of activation covering extensive areas of the cerebellum. Bilateral activation of the cerebellum was observed in all subjects with measurable activity. Mean statistically significant activation intensity ranged from 2.34 to 13.54% above baseline. 相似文献
7.
8.
The mammalian auditory cortex contains multiple fields but their functional role is poorly understood. Here we examine the responses of single neurons in the posterior auditory field (P) of barbiturate- and ketamine-anesthetized cats to frequency-modulated (FM) sweeps. FM sweeps traversed the excitatory response area of the neuron under study, and FM direction and the linear rate of change of frequency (RCF) were varied systematically. In some neurons, sweeps of different sound pressure levels (SPLs) also were tested. The response magnitude (number of spikes corrected for spontaneous activity) of nearly all field P neurons varied with RCF. RCF response functions displayed a variety of shapes, but most functions were of low-pass characteristic or peaked at rather low RCFs (<100 kHz/s). Neurons with strong responses to high RCFs (high-pass or nonselective RCF response function characteristics) all displayed spike count-SPL functions to tone burst onsets that were monotonic or weakly nonmonotonic. RCF response functions and best RCFs often changed with SPL. For most neurons, FM directional sensitivity, quantified by a directional sensitivity (DS) index, also varied with RCF and SPL, but the mean and width of the distribution of DS indices across all neurons was independent of RCF. Analysis of response timing revealed that the phasic response of a neuron is triggered when the instantaneous frequency of the sweep reaches a particular value, the effective Fi. For a given neuron, values of effective Fi were independent of RCF, but depended on FM direction and SPL and were associated closely with the boundaries of the neuron's frequency versus amplitude response area. The standard deviation (SD) of the latency of the first spike of the response decreased with RCF. When SD was expressed relative to the rate of change of stimulus frequency, the resulting index of frequency jitter increased with RCF and did so rather uniformly in all neurons and largely independent of SPL. These properties suggest that many FM parameters are represented by, and may be encoded in, orderly temporal patterns across different neurons in addition to the strength of responses. When compared with neurons in primary and anterior auditory fields, field P neurons respond better to relatively slow FMs. Together with previous studies of responses to modulations of amplitude, such as tone onsets, our findings suggest more generally that field P may be best suited for processing signals that vary relatively slowly over time. 相似文献
9.
H Gomes E Sussman W Ritter D Kurtzberg N Cowan HG Vaughan 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1999,35(1):294-302
In behavioral studies, children's memory for tonal frequency has been found to persist for less time than adults' (T. A. Keller & N. Cowan, 1994). The present study was done to evaluate the argument that this effect is due to changes in auditory sensory memory and not to attentional mechanisms. This question was investigated using mismatch negativity (MMN), an auditory event-related potential considered to be insensitive to attention. Participants were 6-7-, 8-10-, and 11-12-year-old children and adults. They were presented with trains of stimuli, beginning with either a standard (1000 Hz) or a deviant (1200 Hz) tone with trains separated by either 1 s or 8 s. All 4 groups exhibited MMNs after delays of 1 s, but only the adults and oldest children exhibited MMNs after 8 s, indicating that there are maturational changes in the duration of auditory sensory memory. 相似文献
10.
S Pijl 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1997,18(4):316-325
There is little support in the dental literature to justify the use of lasers for periodontal root therapy. To the contrary, there are several in vitro studies suggesting potentially adverse effects when lasers are applied to root surfaces. The purpose of this study was to evaluate, in vivo, soft tissue attachment to root surfaces following CO2 laser irradiation. Using a four-quadrant design with one quadrant serving as an untreated control, the remaining quadrants in each of two dogs were treated by (1) scaling and root planing, (2) laser only, and (3) laser followed by scaling and root planing. Prior to the assigned treatments, the roots of three teeth in each quadrant (including the control) were exposed by flap reflection and ostectomy. After root therapy the flaps were repositioned and allowed to heal for 28 days. Clinical attachment levels were determined prior to surgery, at 28 days, and by histologic measurement. Results indicate that specimens treated with laser only lost attachment compared to controls and other treatment groups. Furthermore, there was no histologic evidence of soft tissue attachment to a laser-treated surface that featured a residual char layer. 相似文献
11.
The purposes of this investigation were to determine 1) if auditory peripheral maturity is present in the newborn; 2) if not, at what age maturational changes occur in the peripheral auditory system from preterm to full-term; and 3) how results of tests used to identify auditory dysfunction in neonates, such as distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPEs) and auditory brain stem responses (ABRs), change during this period. Longitudinal DPE amplitude and ABR wave I latency measurements were obtained from a single ear of 18 preterm neonates. The DPEs were evoked at f2s of 2, 3, 4, and 5 kHz. The longitudinal data revealed that in general, DPE amplitude increased and ABR wave I latency decreased as a function of postconceptional age. These findings suggest that 1) the peripheral auditory system has not reached maturity in the preterm neonate; 2) maturational changes continue from preterm to full-term; and 3) these changes are reflected in ABR and DPE measurements. 相似文献
12.
PL Madsen R Linde SG Hasselbalch OB Paulson NA Lassen 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1998,18(7):742-748
In the clinical setting it has been shown that activation will increase cerebral glucose uptake in excess of cerebral oxygen uptake. To study this phenomenon further, this study presents an experimental setup that enables precise determination of the ratio between cerebral uptake of glucose and oxygen in the awake rat. Global CBF was measured by the Kety-Schmidt technique, and the ratio between cerebral uptake rates for oxygen, glucose, and lactate was calculated from cerebral arterial-venous differences. During baseline conditions, rats were kept in a closed box designed to minimize interference. During baseline conditions CBF was 1.08 +/- 0.25 mL x g(-1) x minute(-1), and the cerebral oxygen to glucose uptake ratio was 5.5. Activation was induced by opening the sheltering box for 6 minutes. Activation increased CBF to 1.81 mL x g(-1) x minute(-1). During activation cerebral glucose uptake increased disproportionately to cerebral oxygen uptake, and the cerebral oxygen to glucose uptake ratio was 4.2. The accumulated excess glucose uptake during 6 minutes of activation amounted to 2.4 micromol/g. Activation was terminated by closure of the sheltering box. In the postactivation period, the cerebral oxygen to glucose uptake ratio rose to a maximum of 6.4. This response is exactly opposite to the excess cerebral glucose uptake observed during activation. 相似文献
13.
E Hammarén A Yli-Hankala PH Rosenberg M Hynynen 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1996,77(3):360-364
Unbound, rather than total, plasma concentrations may be related to the anaesthetic action of propofol. Therefore, we measured plasma concentrations of propofol and recorded Nb wave latencies of auditory evoked potentials (AEP) during continuous infusion of propofol in 15 patients undergoing coronary artery bypass grafting (CABG) surgery. After induction of anaesthesia with fentanyl, propofol was infused continuously at a rate of 10 mg kg-1 h-1 for 20 min, and then the rate was reduced to 3 mg kg-1 h-1. Administration of heparin before cardiopulmonary bypass (CPB) did not affect total or unbound propofol concentration. Initiation of CPB decreased mean total propofol concentration from 2.6 to 1.7 micrograms ml-1 (P < 0.01). Simultaneously, mean unbound propofol concentration remained at 0.06 micrograms ml-1 because of a slight increase in the mean free fraction of plasma propofol (from 2.3 to 3.5%; P > 0.05). During hypothermic CPB, mean total propofol concentration increased to concentrations measured before bypass (to 2.1 micrograms ml-1; P > 0.05 vs value before CPB) and the mean unbound propofol concentration was at its highest (0.07 microgram ml-1; P < 0.05 vs value before heparin). After CPB and administration of protamine, the mean total propofol concentration remained lowered (1.7 micrograms ml-1; P < 0.05 vs value before heparin) and the mean unbound propofol concentration returned to the level measured before heparin (P < 0.001 vs value during hypothermia). The latency of the Nb wave from recordings of AEP increased after induction of anaesthesia, reached its maximum during hypothermia and was prolonged during the subsequent phases of the study. The latency of the Nb wave did not correlate with total or unbound propofol concentration. We conclude that the changes in total and unbound concentrations of plasma propofol were not parallel in patients undergoing CABG. During CPB or at any other time during the CABG procedure, the unbound propofol concentration did not decrease and Nb wave latency was prolonged compared with baseline values measured after induction of anaesthesia before the start of CPB. 相似文献
14.
Ultrastructural characteristics of 15 strains and isolates of ehrlichiae belonging to three genogroups, or clades of genetically related organisms united in the genera Ehrlichia, Cowdria, Anaplasma, Neorickettsia and a strain of Wolbachia pipientis which represents a fourth genogroup in this cluster of species, were studied in continuous cell culture or in vivo: E. canis (Oklahoma strain and VHE isolate), E. muris (AS 145), E. chaffeensis (Arkansas, 91HE17 and Sapulpa), human granulocytic ehrlichiae (HGE)(BDS, 96HE27, 96HE37, #54, #55 and #72), E. equi (MRK), E. sennetsu (Miyayama), E. risticii (HRC-IL). Wolbachia pipientis was studied in the naturally infected Aedes albopictus mosquito cell line Aa23. All organisms were similar in the normal ultrastructure of individual cells and in the ability to form abnormal, pathological ehrlichial cells of the same type irrespective of the species. Normally all ehrlichiae studied in cell culture existed in two morphological forms - reticulate and dense-cored cells, both of which could divide by binary fission. Most alterations were related to their membranes, especially the cell wall. Differences in the structure of intravacuolar microcolonies (morulae) of ehrlichiae and their inter-relations with the host cells allowed differentiation of the genogroups: the E. canis-E. chaffeensis-E. muris genogroup formed large morulae, with many ehrlichiae, often suspended in a fibrillar matrix, and the host cell mitochondria and endoplasmic reticulum usually aggregated near the morulae and were in contact with the morula membrane; the E. phagocytophila-E. equi-HGE group morulae had no fibrillar matrix, no contacts with host cell mitochodria, and they did not aggregate around the morulae; E. sennetsu-E. risticii group usually developed in small individual vacuoles that did not fuse with each other and divided along with the ehrlichiae. 相似文献
15.
It is well known that discrimination response variability increases with stimulus intensity, closely related to Weber's Law. It is also an axiom that sensation magnitude increases with stimulus intensity. Following earlier researchers such as Thurstone, Garner, and Durlach and Braida, we explored a new method of exploiting these relationships to estimate the power function exponent relating sound pressure level to loudness, using the accuracy with which listeners could identify the intensity of pure tones. The log standard deviation of the normally distributed identification errors increases linearly with stimulus range in decibels, and the slope, a, of the regression is proportional to the loudness exponent, n. Interestingly, in a demonstration experiment, the loudness exponent estimated in this way is greater for females than for males. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
16.
Young adult and aged F344 rats were compared on a silent gap variant of the prepulse inhibition paradigm. Animals were tested using a 50-ms single tone cue, followed by 8 days of silent gap testing. The first 3 days of gap testing were long gaps (range 2 to 100 ms) followed by 5 days of short gaps (range 2 to 10 ms). The effects of gap length, prior experience, and age, on the magnitude and direction (facilitation vs. attenuation) of the acoustic startle response, were examined. The young rats showed stronger and more reliable acoustic startle responses (uncued trials) during all acoustic startle tasks as compared to the old. The younger animals also exhibited a more consistent attenuated response across cues and days. Depending on silent gap length, both reduction (inhibition) and enhancement (facilitation) of startle were observed. Finally, only the young adult animals showed an experience-related shift from facilitation to attenuation in response to very short silent gap cues, and this initial early facilitation predicted later attenuation following additional experience. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
17.
Here we report that training-associated changes in neural activity can precede behavioral learning. This finding suggests that speech-sound learning occurs at a pre-attentive level which can be measured neurophysiologically (in the absence of a behavioral response) to assess the efficacy of training. Children with biologically based perceptual learning deficits as well as people who wear cochlear implants or hearing aids undergo various forms of auditory training. The effectiveness of auditory training can be difficult to assess using behavioral methods because these populations are communicatively impaired and may have attention and/or cognitive deficits. Based on our findings, if neurophysiological changes are seen during auditory training, then the training method is effectively altering the neural representation of the speech/sounds and changes in behavior are likely to follow. 相似文献
18.
Hepatitis E has been the predominant type of acute hepatitis in Nepal both in adults and children, in sporadic and epidemic forms. We examined six hepatitis E virus (HEV) isolates obtained during an 8-year period, from 1987 to 1995, in the Kathmandu valley of Nepal. Analysis of portions of the putative helicase, polymerase and capsid genes demonstrated close genetic relatedness among themselves (> 96.4% identity) and with the Burmese (> 95.5%) and Indian (> 95.3%) isolates, and less so with the African (> 94.4%) and the Chinese (> 91%) isolates within the Asian genotype. Phylogenetic analysis placed the Nepali isolates in the Burma-India evolutionary branch and showed that the oldest isolate, TK78/87 was more similar to the Burmese isolates whereas the most recent isolates were closer to the Indian ones. Assuming no frameshifts, the Nepali isolates showed high amino acid conservation, but also unique changes when compared to other HEV isolates. Amino acid residue 614 of the capsid protein was identified as a possible marker to distinguish the Burma-Nepal-India from the China-Central Asian Republics subgenotype, and the Mexico genotype. 相似文献
19.
周鲜娇 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2011,39(15)
[目的]探讨不同培养条件对海洋红酵母类胡萝卜素累积的影响,确定最佳培养条件.[方法]以海洋红酵母HN-3为材料,考察温度、pH、盐度和培养时间对海洋红酵母类胡萝卜素累积的影响.[结果]最佳培养条件为温度30℃、pH 6、盐度3%、培养时间48 h.该培养条件下海洋红酵母摇瓶发酵的类胡萝卜素产量为588.6 μg/g,比初始发酵条件下的类胡萝卜素产量(478.8μg/g)提高了22.9%.[结论]为确定海洋红酵母累积类胡萝卜素的培养条件提供理论依据. 相似文献
20.
Loudness balances have been obtained between 10-, 20-, 100- and 1,000-ms tones of different frequencies (62.5, 125, 250, 500, 1,000, 2,000, 4,000, 8,000 and 12,000 Hz), and a constant reference tone of 1,000 Hz and 1,000 ms duration at 40 dB SPL. The absolute thresholds were also measured on the same observers at each duration. Progressive shifts of the equal loudness contours appear when the durations become shorter, in the same direction as the corresponding shifts of the threshold curves; the shifts of the equal loudness contours and the corresponding shifts of the threshold curves do not however appear completely similar; some differences may also exist between tones of differing frequencies. These experiments were preliminary ones; some problems arise, in relation with the technique used; improvements appear necessary. 相似文献