首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Suggests that previous studies of the role of demand characteristics in the E. Velten (1968) mood induction procedure have not provided equivalent information and demands to both the experimental and demand characteristics groups. When these differences were corrected, the authors were unable to reject the hypothesis that the apparent effects of the mood induction were entirely due to demand characteristics. It is concluded that use of the induction procedure to manipulate mood experimentally or to test the relationship between cognition and mood is unwarranted. (6 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Investigated the use of a musical mood induction technique as an alternative to E. Velten's (1968) method in 2 experiments with 80 undergraduates. Results show that music induced affect differences between elated and depressed groups, as measured by the Depression Adjective Checklist. In addition, psychomotor performance was affected by mood induction. Moreover, these effects were achieved without the strong demand characteristics and gender differences typically associated with the Velten technique. (12 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
R. O. Frost et al (see record 1980-01011-001) claim that self-devaluative aspects of the Velton Mood Induction Procedure (VMIP) do not lower mood or otherwise mimic depression but that the elements of the VMIP that suggest depression-related somatic states do. An experiment involving 52 undergraduates indicated that both aspects of the VMIP have a powerful impact on mood and that the self-devaluation statements have a priming effect on the accessibility of positive and negative memories different from that of the VMIP elation statements. (4 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Argues that the commonly encountered advice that a single test score cannot be used in making important decisions about students is incorrect for 2 reasons: the advice is imprecise regarding the nature of testing, and the advice deviates from what is said in the Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing (1985). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Comments that A. Ortmann and R. Hertwig's (see record 1997-04731-011) call to outlaw the use of all forms of deception in psychological research is both methodologically unsound and ethically misguided. Deception procedures differ so much in the nature and degree of deception used that even the harshest critic would be hard-pressed to state unequivocally that all deception is unacceptable. When the methodological requirements of an investigation lead the researcher to conclude that the only way a study can be carried out is by using deceptive tactics, the decision to deceive necessarily results in additional ethical responsibilities for the researcher, and the degree of deception should be held at a level that does not exceed what is required by the research. An absolute rule prohibiting the use of deception in all psychological research would have the egregious consequence of preventing researchers from carrying out a wide range of important studies. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Suggests that criticisms of the basis of auto-instructional programming misperceive the role ot the stimulus–response (S–R) model of learning when they fail to acknowledge that the principles of programmed instruction were derived from the S–R model. (0 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Argues that criticism of psychological tests is misplaced and ignores the major advantages of objective measurement. Professionally prepared multiple-choice tests are efficient yet searching. Tests achieve their power through aggregation and consequently indicate general tendencies, which are valid for aggregate behavioral tendencies. The apparent precision of numerical scores fosters an overdependence on test scores that must be resisted. Quantitative measurement can provide an indication of social problems. For example, large ethnic differences in average test scores are probably not the result of test bias but probably indicate the extent of educational disadvantage experienced by minorities. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Comments on B. Schwartz's (see record 2000-13324-008) discussion of excessive personal freedom, autonomy, self-determination, and life satisfaction and meaning. A positive psychology, according to Schwartz, must constrain "tyranny of choice" (p. 81), and psychologists should "deemphasize freedom" because "American society has created an excess of freedom" (p. 79). R. Solomon suggests that a positive psychology for the 21st century must enhance and facilitate the creation of freedom, not undo it. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Discovering truth is not the sole agenda for every scientist. The rewards of achieving paradigm hegemony are huge and serve to justify all manner of strategies and tactics. In this issue and some that have preceded it, scientific paradigm clashes have been brought into the bright light of public scrutiny. I believe a science that is critical progresses faster than one driven solely by induction. Debate is undeniably a part of science, one from which spectators (practitioners and researchers) benefit. A greater understanding of each position is made easier by a direct and clear statement of that position and of what its proponents view as problems with the opposing paradigm. It is not expected that those who read an extreme position will adopt it; however, they may come to understand it better and draw their own conclusions accordingly. Controversy, properly understood and used, can serve the scientific community by sharpening the focus on the true issues where research is most vital to further our understanding of human behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
T. Curran and D. L. Hintzman (see record 1995-42725-001) claim to have shown that the independence assumption underlying the process-dissociation procedure (L. L. Jacoby, see record 1992-07943-001) is not justified. They argued that correlations between processes at the level of items can result in an underestimation of automatic processes large enough to produce artifactual dissociations between process estimates. In contrast, the authors show that the effects of extremely high correlations at the level of items are likely to be trivial, and not differential across conditions. Curran and Hintzman's dissociations probably reflect violations of boundary conditions for use of the process-dissociation procedure, rather than violations of independence. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Comments on Sikes and Cleveland's (see record 1969-07237-001) article on a program to improve the social relationships between police and community. If verbal labels are ignored, the modal response for both police and community is on the negative side of the scale. Not using more rigorous procedures leaves the results of their evaluation ambiguous. (0 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Describes experiences with the editors of psychological journals in response to an article by H. Rodman (see record 1970-15565-001). In the writer's experience, editors returned submissions with productive comments for improving them in 2 wks or less. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Discusses possible misconceptions that may arise from an article by L. J. Cronbach (see record 1976-03506-001) regarding the interpretation of the army intelligence test results of World War I and the ramifications of these misconceptions. W. Lippmann challenged basic assumptions of tests along with the antidemocratic conclusions conferred upon their results by army psychologists. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Contradicts M. J. Saks's (1970) assertions that the role of military psychology is in conflict with the ethical standards of psychologists. Emotional pacificism and the equating of military defense with murder may prove to be more destructive than the pacificism that led to World War II. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Maintains that many psychological investigations are accused of failure to generalize to the real world because of sample bias or artificiality of setting. It is argued in this article that such generalizations often are not intended. Rather than making predictions about the real world from the laboratory, it is possible to test predictions that specify what ought to happen in the lab. Even "artificial" findings may be regarded as interesting because they show what can occur, even if it rarely does; or, where generalizations are made, they may have added force because of artificiality of sample or setting. A misplaced preoccupation with external validity can lead to dismissing good research for which generalization to real life is not intended or meaningful. (18 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
Reviews the special issue of The Humanistic Psychologist, Psychotherapy for freedom: The daseinsanalytic way in psychology and psychotherapy edited by Erik Craig (Volume 16, Number 1, Spring 1988, 278 pp.). The editor brings together a uniquely developed collection of essays, seminars, and interviews delving into foundational as well as clinically-oriented issues regarding the application of Martin Heidegger's philosophy to the fields of psychiatry and psychotherapy. Craig's edition exhibits a cohesiveness not often found in edited volumes, and it manages to be inspiring to the more advanced reader while remaining accessible to the introductory student. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Comments on the article by Miller and Campbell (see record 2010-17135-004), in which they provide an informative discussion of the importance of considering narcissistic personality trait research when attempting to understand narcissistic personality disorder. Their arguments might seem so straightforward and compelling that they are hardly worth presenting. However, it does seem that this considerable body of literature is at times neglected, if not ignored. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
Presents a brief historical review and statements of policymakers to demonstrate that the central principles of psychologists concerning nuclear war have affected the behavior of some policymakers. It is suggested that J. G. Blight's (see record 1987-16803-001) pessimism about the incompatibility between the views of psychologists and policymakers is unwarranted; considerations of why psychologists have had low impact on national security policy are offered. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
20.
Criticizes M. L. Goldstein's (see 42:4) review of Arnold's theory of emotion because he disregards her major work (1960) and misinterprets her 1950 version. His assumption that a theory must be couched in objectivist terms to be testable is untenable. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号