首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 218 毫秒
1.
The authors tested the hypothesis that chronic treatment with a dose of deoxycorticosterone acetate (DOCA) known to elicit a robust sodium appetite can negatively affect the hedonic state of rats. Daily treatment with DOCA with no opportunity to ingest saline produced a rightward shift in the midpoint (effective current 50) of lateral hypothalamic self-stimulation (LHSS) current-response functions and reduced intakes of a palatable sucrose solution. Providing rats with 0.3 M saline during DOCA treatment prevented the rightward shift in LHSS response functions and the decrease in sucrose intake. The authors concluded that a chronic sodium appetite, with no opportunity to attenuate the appetite, can elicit a reduced responsiveness to reward. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
The effects of subfornical organ (SFO) lesions on salt and water intakes after sodium depletion were studied. Water and salt intakes were measured over 45 hr during a regimen that combined furosemide diuresis and access to low-sodium diet. Water was solely available for 23 hr after diuresis, and water and 0.3 M NaCl solution were available in choice for the next 22 hr. After diuresis, rats with SFO lesions drank significantly less water in 2 hr than controls but achieved equivalent water and sodium balances before salt access 20 hr later. After salt access, rats with SFO lesions drank significantly less saline and water in 2 hr than controls but had similar saline and water intakes over the next 20 hr. Thus, SFO lesions blunted acutely, but not chronically, saline and water intakes to sodium depletion, and the blunted intakes are not explainable by hydrational status. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Adult male rats consumed 50–250% more 0.5 M NaCl solution than females did during a 7-hr drinking test when robust salt appetite was elicited by dietary sodium deprivation for 8 days, daily injections of deoxycorticosterone, or adrenalectomy followed by 2 days of sodium deprivation. In contrast, male rats drank much less saline after systemic treatment with the natriuretic agent furosemide, adrenalectomy followed by 1 day of sodium deprivation, or subcutaneous/ly (sc) treatment with colloid solution after 2 days of sodium deprivation, and female rats drank comparably small volumes. Conversely, 30-day-old prepubescent male and female rats showed equally robust salt appetites after 8 days of sodium deprivation. These and other findings support an inhibitory role of estrogen on salt appetite in rats, which appears to occur only when the appetite is especially pronounced. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
The effect of sodium depletion on the value of lateral hypothalamic (LH) stimulation and on competition and summation between saline reward and LH reward was assessed. Sodium depletion did not alter curves relating the number of LH rewards earned per trial to the number of stimulation pulses per train, suggesting that negative sodium balance does not alter the reward value of the stimulation. In contrast, sodium depletion increased the ability of saline (0.9% NaCl) to compete with LH stimulation in a forced-choice preference test. Summation between the effects of the 2 rewards was also seen under sodium depletion. These findings imply that the rewarding effects of saline and LH stimulation are evaluated within a common system of measurement and that negative sodium balance modulates the value of the saline reward upstream from the point where the rewarding effects of saline and LH stimulation are combined. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
The influence of chronic exposure to immobilization (IMO) on sodium appetite as well as sodium and potassium renal excretion in adult male Wistar rats was studied. The animals were individually housed and all variables under observation were measured in metabolic cages the first, seventh, and thirteenth days once the experiment had started. Half of the rats had access to water, and the remainder of the rats had access to both water and saline solution (1.5% NaCl). IMO reduced the intake of saline solution. Renal water, sodium, and potassium excretion in those IMO rats having access to saline were lower than in control rats. The effects of IMO were very similar during all observation days; therefore no evidence of adaptation to repeated stress was found. The present data indicate the following: (i) IMO stress reduced sodium appetite, probably as a secondary effect to the deficit in sodium renal excretion; (ii) IMO caused antidiuresis and antikaliuresis, only in those rats taking saline solution; (iii) no adaptation to repeated IMO stress was found in any of the tested variables. The reduction of sodium appetite observed in stressed rats might be a homeostatic mechanism to maintain sodium balance after impairment of renal sodium excretion caused by stress.  相似文献   

6.
Angiotensin receptors in the most ventral part of the ventral median preoptic nucleus (VVMnPO) or organum vasculosum laminae terminalis appear to be important for salt appetite to angiotensin in rats. If so, then small lesions of this region should reduce salt appetite that is dependent on angiotensin. In separate experiments, the lesion greatly reduced salt appetite after treatments with chronic oral captopril or sodium depletion. On the other hand, the VVMnPO lesion actually enhanced salt appetite to deoxycorticosterone acetate. The lesion did not affect water intake to water deprivation, combined food-water deprivation, isoproterenol, or hypertonic saline, and basal plasma osmolality and sodium values were normal. These experiments suggest that VVMnPO lesions selectively affect angiotensin-induced salt appetite without producing the gross hydrational deficits that occur with larger lesions of the ventral forebrain. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Adrenalectomy (ADX) is known to block the acquisition of intravenous cocaine self-administration. A previous study therefore examined whether ADX decreases sensitivity of the 'brain reward system' in general, or its response to cocaine in particular, by measuring thresholds for intracranial self-stimulation with and without concurrent cocaine administration. ADX had no effect on thresholds for lateral hypothalamic self-stimulation (LHSS) and did not alter the cocaine dose-response curve for lowering the LHSS threshold. This result suggested that ADX does not affect sensitivity of the brain reward system. However, medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) appears to be an important site in the mediation of cocaine reinforcing effects, and MPFC self-stimulation (MPFCSS) is mediated by a neural substrate that is largely independent of that which mediates LHSS. The present study therefore assessed whether ADX diminishes cocaine facilitation of MPFCSS. It was found that the threshold-lowering effect of cocaine (5.0, 10.0 and 20.0 mg/kg, i.p. ) did not differ between ADX rats maintained on 0.7% saline, ADX rats maintained on corticosterone (50 microg/ml) in 0.7% saline, and sham-operated controls. However, there was a trend toward desensitization of MPFCSS, itself, following ADX in the group that did not receive corticosterone supplementation. Based on this observation, and the similar responses of MPFCSS and cocaine self-administration to noncontingent priming stimulation, stress, and NMDA receptor antagonism, it is speculated that acquisition of MPFCSS and cocaine self-administration may be dependent upon a common sensitization process that is regulated by corticosterone.  相似文献   

8.
Water intake was elevated in sodium-depleted rats during a daytime salt appetite test, but other rats drank a similar amount of water when saline was not available for drinking during the test. This water intake stimulated by sodium depletion was blocked by an inhibition of angiotensin (ANG) II synthesis with a high dose of captopril (100 mg/kg, sc). Captopril did not reduce water intake by causing hypotensive shock or uremia, because water and saline intakes were increased rather than decreased after a low dose of captopril (5 mg/kg) that also reduced blood pressure and elevated blood urea nitrogen. The water intake, but not salt appetite, induced by sodium depletion was greatly reduced by a lesion of the subfornical organ (SFO) in one-bottle tests, and this was not clearly related to any effects of the lesion on blood pressure. A physiological role for ANG II in water intake induced by sodium depletion has recently been disputed, but the simplest explanation for the data remains that elevated levels of circulating ANG II bind to receptors in the SFO to generate daytime water drinking during sodium depletion.  相似文献   

9.
Interactions among sodium homeostasis, fatigue, mood, and cardiovascular regulation have been described previously. The present study investigates the effects of sodium deficiency on an index of mood (hypohedonia; Experiment 1), cardiovascular function (Experiment 2), and plasma electrolytes (Experiment 3) in rats. Following 48 hr of sodium depletion with a diuretic (furosemide) and a sodium deficient diet, rats displayed hypohedonia evidenced by reduced responding for rewarding electrical brain stimulation into the hypothalamus. Also, sodium depletion produced increased heart rate and reduced heart rate variability. Plasma sodium levels were lower in sodium-depleted rats versus control rats, whereas potassium levels were unchanged. Thus, mild sodium depletion produces hypohedonia and cardiovascular alterations, which has implications for understanding behavioral and cardiovascular consequences of sodium deficiency. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
The authors investigated the effects of postnatal manipulations of oxytocin (OT) on the subsequent tendency to form a partner preference in male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Neonatally, males received either an injection of OT, an oxytocin antagonist (OTA), 0.9% saline vehicle, or handling without injection. As adults, males were tested for partner preference following 1 hr of cohabitation with a nonestrous female. In a 3-hr preference test, males neonatally exposed to exogenous OT exhibited a significant partner preference, not seen in males receiving OTA or saline. Both OT and OTA voles had significantly higher levels of social contact than saline controls. A single neonatal injection of OT increased both total and selective social behaviors in male prairie voles. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Studied potassium appetite in normal female Sprague-Dawley rats and in rats in which the total body potassium had been reduced by 15-20%. Potassium depletion resulted in increased ingestion of solutions of NaCl, KCl, CaCl2, and quinine sulphate in concentrations that were unacceptable to normal Ss. The amount of potassium ingested was related to the degree of potassium depletion and repletion was usually completed within 24 hr. when potassium was offered. Potassium-depleted Ss also drank large quantities of aversive concentrations of sodium chloride. This was preferred to potassium chloride and its ingestion appeared to be unrelated to need. The appetite state was reversed by prior intragastric repletion with potassium but not with sodium salts. (19 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Infusing conscious unrestrained rats with either 0.5 M NaCl-CSF or 0.7 M sucrose-CSF into the lateral cerebral ventricle (IVT) at 38 μl/hr for 4 hr induced drinking. Although the infusates were nearly equiosmotic, water drinking during the 0.5 M NaCl-CSF was greater than during 0.7 M sucrose-CSF. However, IVT infusions of 0.7 M mannitol-CSF at rates of 9.4 μl/hr or 38 μl/hr for 4 hr or 10 μl/hr for 4 days failed to induce water drinking. Also, IVT infusion of 0.27 M mannitol-CSF at 38 μl/hr for 4 hr failed to significantly alter water drinking. CSF [Na] was reduced by IVT infusion of either 0.7 M sucrose-CSF or 0.7 M mannitol-CSF. In contrast, CSF [Na] was increased by 4-hr IVT infusion of 0.5 M NaCl in rats denied access to water during the infusion. Intake of 0.5 M NaCl was not altered significantly from control intakes by any of the above IVT infusions. It is concluded that water drinking in the rat may be initiated by stimulation of either a sodium sensitive sensor alone or with an osmoreceptor system and that species specific differences in the induction of both water drinking and hypertonic saline drinking are apparent. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Rats given access to a 32% sucrose solution and then downshifted to a 4% solution exhibit less contact with the sipper tube than unshifted controls always given access to 4% solution. This phenomenon, called consummatory successive negative contrast, was facilitated in Experiment 1 by a posttrial injection of corticosterone (3 mg/kg) administered immediately after the first downshift trial. Experiment 2 demonstrated that this facilitatory effect of posttrial corticosterone does not occur when administered 3 hr after the first downshift trial. These results support the hypothesis that corticosterone strengthens an aversive emotional component elicited by the surprising downshift in reward magnitude during the initial downshift trial. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Unilateral renal artery plication in dogs reduced renal blood flow by 80% and produced a sustained elevation in arterial pressure whereas plasma renin activity increased for only 4 days. Sodium was retained for 3 days after plication, but this response is similar to that after a sham operation. Of seven dogs studied chronically, elevated arterial pressure was sustained for 27 days or longer in six animals. In three dogs hypertension continued for 2 mo before collateral vessels developed and arterial pressure fell; ligation of these collaterals restored hypertension. Arterial pressure was unaffected by an infusion of [1-sarcosine, 8-alanine] angiotensin II in chronic hypertensive dogs on a normal sodium intake. This angiotensin antagonist lowered arterial pressure after sodium depletion, but became ineffective following rapid sodium repletion. Chronic hypertensive dogs showed normal responses to deoxycorticosterone acetate. These findings suggest that the renin-angiotensin system is not critically involved in maintenace of chronic two-kidney renovascular hypertension in the dog. The data also show that the homeostatic role played by the renin-angiotensin system in the maintenance of arterial pressure remained intact in chronic hypertension.  相似文献   

15.
Methylphenidate (MPH) is one of the most widely prescribed drugs for treating attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Previous research suggested that MPH is a reinforcer for rats, but not all of the manipulations to show that lever pressing is controlled by the contingency to obtain MPH have been examined. In Experiment 1, responding for MPH on a progressive ratio (PR) schedule was assessed. Rats self-administered varying doses of MPH (0.056–1.0 mg/kg/infusion) on a PR schedule of reinforcement, and self-administered more MPH than saline, with maximal responding occurring at a unit dose of 0.56 mg/kg/infusion. Experiment 2 examined if there were differences in responding between contingent and noncontingent MPH (0.56 mg/kg/infusion) on a fixed ratio schedule of reinforcement. Results showed that rats responded for contingent MPH, and that responding was not maintained when MPH was delivered noncontingently. Experiment 3 examined self-administration of MPH (0.1 or 0.3 mg/kg/infusion) during long access (6 hr) compared to short access sessions (1 hr). Results showed that rats given long access to MPH showed an escalation of intake across sessions, with this escalation being more pronounced at the lower unit dose (0.1 mg/kg/infusion); in contrast, rats given short access to MPH did not show an increase in MPH self-administration across sessions at either MPH dose tested. Taken together, these results indicate that MPH is an effective intravenous reinforcer for rats and that, similar to other stimulants such as cocaine, amphetamine and methamphetamine, MPH is subject to abuse as reflected by dysregulated intake across repeated long access sessions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
Sodium-deficient adult rats prefer NaCl to other monochloride salts (e.g., Denton, 1991; Schulkin, 1991). However, it is not known when or how this specificity develops. Our experiments charted the development of the ionic specificity of sodium appetite aroused by sodium depletion or intracerebroventricular injection of renin. We compared intake of 3% NaCl to three other monochlorides, potassium (K), ammonium (NH4), and lithium (Li), and calcium chloride (CaCl2) at various ages between 72 hr postnatal and weaning. This revealed a biphasic developmental scheme: The adult pattern of discrimination between the salts emerges between 3 and 18 days of age. Subsequently, the preference for Na over the other salts increases into adulthood.  相似文献   

17.
A single sodium depletion enhances the salt appetite that is expressed after a second and subsequent sodium depletions. The enhanced salt intake, as measured by a decrease in latency to drink and an increase in volume of 3% NaCl ingested, is not accounted for by an increased sodium loss. The enhanced salt intake occurs even when the interval between first and second depletion is as long as 4 months. The enhanced salt appetite does not depend on the drinking of salt after the animal's first sodium depletion and is specific for strong 0.52 M NaCl but not for 0.52 M KCl. Moreover, it can be produced without sodium depletion by the actions of the hormones aldosterone and angiotensin on the brain. These results suggest that angiotensin and aldosterone, which are released in response to sodium depletion, (a) increase renal sodium conservation, (b) evoke a salt appetite to restore the lost sodium, and (c) produce enduring changes in the brain that prepare it for more rapid and more vigorous expression of salt appetite in response to future sodium depletions. Thus the neural mechanisms that govern salt appetite are not only activated by the hormones of sodium conservation but appear also to be organized by them for a lifelong increase in avidity for salty substances. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
The authors tested whether the level of hydration after furosemide diuresis and 22 hrs of sodium depletion affects the amount of water or 0.3 M NaCl solution consumed by rats with intact brains or with lesions of the subfornical organ (SFO). Rats received 2 (underhydrated) or 10 (euhydrated) ml/kg water by gavage as the only fluid input 2, 4, and 20 hrs after 10 mg/kg furosemide. These hydration treatments had little or no effect on the amount of saline consumed in 2 hrs by intact rats. SFO lesions reduced water intake regardless of hydration condition. Euhydrated, SFO-lesioned rats drank a normal amount of saline, but underhydrated, lesioned rats drank less saline than any other group. Thus, euhydration may facilitate salt appetite in SFO-lesioned rats, and the deficits in salt appetite noted in SFO-lesioned rats may result from deficits in water ingestion rather than from a destruction of angiotensin II receptor sites that directly provoke salt appetite. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
The present study was carried out to determine whether the increased salt intake induce by increased specific sodium appetite in pregnant rats modifies water-salt homeostasis throughout pregnancy. Two groups of pregnant rats were used, one fed ad libitum with a normal sodium (NS) diet consisting of standard rat chow and distilled water, and the other fed with a high-sodium (HS) diet with free access to chow, distilled water plus saline solution (1.5% NaCl). Virgin rats in dioestrus were also studied as non-pregnant controls. Pregnant animals were studied on days 4, 9, 14, 20 and 21 of gestation at which time body weight, water and saline intake, sodium excretion, plasma atrial natriuretic peptide (ANP) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) concentrations, as well as plasma osmolality were determined. Data showed that water intake was higher in the NS group, but total fluid intake (water plus saline) was higher in the HS group throughout pregnancy. Dietary sodium intake was the same for both groups but total sodium intake (chow plus saline) was 60-98% higher in the HS rats. Pregnant HS rats excreted more fluid (35-50%) and sodium (up to 100%) compared with NS rats, indicating that the animals could change their renal excretion in response to a 2.5-fold higher dietary sodium intake compared with the control level. Salt satiety during pregnancy did not modify plasma ANP concentration. In both groups of pregnant rats ANP levels increased 3-fold on day 14 without significant alteration in sodium excretion, suggesting that the natriuretic action of ANP is attenuated at least after the second week of pregnancy. High sodium intake did not change plasma AVP concentration or osmolality and both groups showed the same gradual decrease in plasma osmolality (approximately 8 mosmol kg-1) at the end of pregnancy that was not accompanied by decreased plasma AVP concentration. The present data show that rats maintain the special homeostatic equilibrium that occurs in normal pregnancy even when they are allowed to increase sodium intake to satisfy their salt appetite during this period of the reproductive cycle.  相似文献   

20.
Two experiments were designed to test whether angiotensin (ANG) synthesis or receptor activation in the ventral preoptic region is critical for ANG-induced salt appetite in rats. In Experiment 1, infusions of ANG into the subfornical organ (SFO) produced water drinking without saline intake, but infusions near the organum vasculosum laminae terminals (OVLT) produced both water and saline drinking. Thus, forebrain areas that support water drinking to ANG do not all support salt appetite. In Experiment 2, rats were given oral captopril (CAP) to enhance daily intake of water and saline solution by increasing ANG II synthesis in the brain. CAP microinjected into the SFO reduced CAP-enhanced water drinking without affecting saline intake, but CAP in the OVLT reduced enhanced saline intake without affecting water drinking. Thus, ANG acting in the OVLT, the most ventral part of the median preoptic nucleus, or other nearby structures is important for ANG-induced saline appetite. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号