首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Distractor color heterogeneity refers to a condition in which a target is presented among distractors of different chromaticities. In the present study, the amount distractor color heterogeneity was varied systematically to determine how efficiently the visual system processes displays composed of search elements of multiple colors. Distractor color heterogeneity was expressed by selecting distractor chromaticities from sectors of various angles in a cone‐based normalized color space. The generalizability of the measurements was tested using two different visual search paradigms. An accuracy search task was used in which the search displays were presented as brief flashes and the dependent variable was search accuracy. A latency search task was used in which the display presentation time was under the participant's control and the dependent variable was reaction time. Compared to a homogeneous condition with distractors of a single color, distractor color heterogeneity had a deleterious effect on search performance in both paradigms. In normalized units, the measurements were similar across participants, target chromaticities, and tasks, but the same measurements expressed in non‐normalized units showed clear and systematic individual differences. © 2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2011  相似文献   

2.
Labels are indispensable visual (communication) elements that completely deliver the geospatial message of maps. The objective of this study is to investigate the impact of complementary colors between the map's background and text on the readability of cartographic texts and thus on the efficiency of the map user's search task. This is compared with the use of the “traditional” black labels on the corresponding colored backgrounds. Furthermore, a number of user characteristics, such as gender and expertise, are taken into account as well. The users' eye movements were registered to study their attentive behavior during the visual search task. In addition to the complement of the color's hue, the analyses were based on the difference in luminance, which could also affect the labels' readability. The difference between the black and colored label design was significantly different versus the eye‐tracking metrics. A correlation was found between the color difference and reaction time measurement and between the luminance difference and fixation duration. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 40, 437–445, 2015  相似文献   

3.
Part I of this article found, inter alia, that chroma resembles log inverted luminance. This article develops three math models of Munsell chroma and associated colorfulness from (1) inverted luminous reflectance Y, (2) inverted chromatic luminance, and (3) inverted chromatic luminance combined (over the mid‐spectrum 480–580 nm) with the unimodal curve for spectral absorptance of M cones. The first two models are simple but of limited accuracy and demonstrate that inverted luminance (of any form) cannot fully account for varying relative chroma around the hue cycle, particularly the minor minimum and maximum about 490 and 520 nm (which also feature in B:L functions). The third model is rather complex but very accurate, apparently the only accurate model of Munsell chroma or other experimentally based scales of relative chromaticness in the literature. It adjusts to any level of luminance or purity, as demonstrated for several levels. Three models of brightness (B:L ratio) for 20 field aperture colors are given, based on either Munsell chroma or log inverted chromatic luminance. The former provides two accurate and simple models of the CIE B:L function: (1) log chroma = B:L ratio ±0.1, and (2) (chroma/k)x = B:L ratio ±0.1. The latter also predicts B:L for nonspectral colors and those of lower purities, e.g., object colors. The results finally solve the relationship between brightness and chroma and demonstrate that B:L ratio (a contrast in constant luminance) arises directly from chroma (also a form of contrast in constant luminance), or the reverse. © 2008 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 34, 55–67, 2009.  相似文献   

4.
The relationship between lamp color characteristics and brightness perception is not well known. In this study, nine lighting environment with correlated color temperature (3000 K, 5000 K, and 8000 K) and illuminance (1000 lx, 300 lx, and 100 lx) were created. Both the side by side visual matching and spatial brightness scaling experiments are designed to verify the effects of correlated color temperature on spatial brightness perception. The results of the study show that lighting with high correlated color temperature will have stronger spatial brightness perception than lower ones. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2011  相似文献   

5.
6.
A new proposal tentatively called “integrated photometric system” is given together with a detailed explanation of its structure. The proposal gives the following procedures: step 1 can determine CIE photometric quantities, step 2 can determine their perceived‐photometric correlates by applying the correction to the quantities derived in step 1. The present proposal solves the problem of coexistence of the two kinds of expected requirements in photometry suggested by Judd. In the present article, the formulas used in the step 2 are given only for luminous colors. The formulas for object colors will be given in a future article. Five applications of luminous colors are given for the step 2 of the proposed system. These applications will make it possible to promote the use of the proposed photometric system. © 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 32, 440–448, 2007  相似文献   

7.
Associations of colors with a wide variety of visual forms were examined. Fifty visual forms were presented to Japanese participants, who were asked to report a color suitable to each visual form out of 15 colors. Correspondence analysis was applied to the responses, and four dimensions of configurations were obtained. The first and second dimensions were related to chromaticity or hue. In these dimensions, red and orange were associated with round symmetric shapes, yellow with shapes with sharp corners, violet and purple with irregular shapes with smooth curves, and cold colors such as blue and green with forms that consisted of simple geometrical elements such as triangles, squares, and circles. The third dimension was related to L* and b* in the CIELAB color space and to the stability of visual forms. Dimension 4 was not significantly related to any of L*, a*, and b*. In another experiment, participants rated the affective impressions of the colors and visual forms on semantic differential (SD) scales. The analyses of the SD data combined with the results of the color‐form associations suggest that the role of emotional meanings of colors and visual forms in mediating the associations was rather minor, and that world‐knowledge contributed considerably to the observed color‐form associations. Kandinsky's color‐form assignment was also examined, and except for the yellow‐triangle association, the assignment was not supported.  相似文献   

8.
To calibrate a CRT color monitor, several assumptions are often made about its performance, one of the most accepted being constanel channel chromaticity. However, when the measurements are taken in a extended field of CRT screen, where it is assumed that only one primary phosphor is excited, the relative spectral radiance of the other primary phosphors that are contained in that field also contributes, if the background luminance is not zero. This contribution is greater at low levels of RGB channel stimulation and would invalidate the results obtained with calibration methods based on the hypothesis of constancy of chromaticity coordinates of RGB channels. We have studied this effect on the calibration of the CRT color monitor and, as a result, we propose a calibration that takes this effect into account. to evaluate the accuracy of the method, we have calculated color differences between the chromaticity that we wanted to reproduce and the one displayed with the CIELUV color-difference formula, and the color tolerances with the line element of MacAdam ellipses.  相似文献   

9.
A study was done to investigate preference responses for foreground–background color relationships. To do this, 123 university undergraduates in Ankara, Turkey, were asked to view eight background colors selected from HSB color space on which color squares of differing hues, saturations, and brightnesses were presented. Subjects were asked to show the color square they preferred on the presented background color. Findings showed that colors having maximum saturation and brightness were most preferred. Blue was the most preferred hue regardless of background. The findings for preferences for foreground–background color relationships are also included in this article. © 2002 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 27, 199–207, 2002; Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/col.10051  相似文献   

10.
Account information for over 1 million Twitter users was downloaded and analyzed to determine color preference. Blues were found to be the most preferred color, whereas greens were least preferred. Distinct gender‐specific differences were found. Males preferred blues to a greater extent than females, whereas females preferred magentas to a much greater extent than males. Males preferred darker colors to a greater extent than women. Density plots within hue, saturation, and brightness space summarize the distribution of color choices and illustrate color preferences for both males and females. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 38, 196–202, 2013.  相似文献   

11.
12.
Although the CIE1931 and 1964 color matching functions have been used in color specification for decades, many researchers, from Allen in 1970 to Hu and Houser in 2006, have found that there still exists a great visual mismatch on the discrimination of color difference as in terms of the CIE color matching functions. Hence, some significant error would be made on color specification due to employing the CIE1931 and 1964 color matching functions. Therefore, six color difference formulae developed from different experimental methods are used to derive various deviate visual functions (DVFs) respectively, and to investigate the effect of these DVFs on the performance of the color difference formulae tested in estimating visual color difference. The results indicate that the performance of the color difference formulae in estimating color difference is significantly improved by the deviate visual functions derived in this study. The CIE94 color difference formula has the best performance in predicting the total visual color difference (ΔVT) using the DVFs and DVFIIs having the mean values 29 and 27 in PF/4 unit, respectively, while the CMC(l:c) the worst the ones 37 and 38. © 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 34, 115–127, 2009  相似文献   

13.
Many tablets are designed to change display brightness or color with surround for enhancing visual comfort. Although both color and brightness of a surround may vary a lot, few studies investigated how text‐background lightness combination of a tablet display and surround jointly affect visual comfort, and how display white point affects visual comfort. In this study, 20 observers evaluated visual comfort of 20 text‐background lightness combinations of a 9.7‐inch tablet display through paired comparisons under five surrounds—a dark surround and four ambient lighting conditions comprising two levels of correlated color temperature (CCT)—3500 and 6500 K—and illuminance—300 and 3000 lx. The combination of a black text and a light‐gray background (i.e., L*background = 75.33; L*text = 1.6) was evaluated the most comfortable when there was ambient light regardless of CCT and illuminance. It was also evaluated the third most comfortable under the dark surround. The observers also evaluated the visual comfort of a dark text on five different white backgrounds under 3500 and 6500 K at 1000 lx. The color of the background that was judged as the most comfortable neither had the whitest appearance nor matched the color of the ambient light. The simultaneous adjustment of the display white point and the text‐background lightness combination merits further investigations.  相似文献   

14.
“Visual clarity” or “feeling of contrast” of object colors under illumination is affected significantly by changing color‐rendering property of light source used. Though the feeling of contrast is considered one of the most important characteristics on color‐rendering properties of light sources, it cannot be estimated adequately by using the present Ra method. The new index FCI is proposed for estimating the effect of feeling of contrast quantitatively under any light sources. The FCI is derived using a simple transformation of the gamut area, which is constituted by a specially selected four‐color combination in CIE LAB color space. The FCI correlates well with the illuminance ratio for equal feeling of contrast (or equal visual clarity) on various light sources reported so far. Using the FCI together with the present CIE Ra, the color‐rendering capability of a light source can be well clarified. © 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 32, 361–371, 2007  相似文献   

15.
Color as a fundamental quality of edible oils has been determined primarily by visual comparison. The automatic colorimeters introduced recently made it possible to avoid operator variability associated with visual color measurement. The feasibility of using an automatic colorimeter, Colourscan, to measure the color of refined and bleached cottonseed oils was investigated. Good agreement, r2=0.99, between automatic color readings vs. visual color measurement by using the Lovibond-AOCS Color Scale was obtained. Near-linear correlation of light paths from 133.4 to 2.5 mm permits dark oil to be assessed with the automatic colorimeter.  相似文献   

16.
17.
A psychophysical experiment was carried out to investigate visual comfort for reading on an iPad under various illuminance levels, ranging from 50 lx to 1200 lx, in order to see whether and how the following variables can influence the visual comfort: observer's age, gender, the illuminance of ambient lighting, and the background colour in a document layout. A panel of 21 young and 22 older Taiwanese observers participated in the study. The paired comparison method was used for data collection and analysis. The experimental results show that for all lighting conditions, young observers tended to prefer reading documents that had a moderate CIELAB lightness difference between text and background, while older observers tended to prefer reading those with an extremely large lightness difference. The results also show that female observers tended to feel less comfortable than male observers when reading documents with an extremely large lightness difference. These findings were found not to be affected by the ambient illuminance. Regarding the influence of document background colour on visual comfort, the observers tended to feel more comfortable reading documents that had a gray background than reading those with a background colour of either white or black. It was also found that the visual comfort was slightly higher for positive polarity than for negative polarity, but the difference between the two settings was insignificant. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 42, 352–361, 2017  相似文献   

18.
Visual uncertainty, while reported, is not used routinely when evaluating color‐difference formula performance in comparison with visual data; rather, data are analyzed assuming no uncertainty; that is, repeating the experiment would result in the identical average results. Previously, Shen and Berns developed three methods to determine whether a color‐difference formula was well‐fitting, under‐fitting, or over‐fitting visual data when visual uncertainty was considered, the method dependent on how the uncertainty was reported and the colorimetric sampling of the color‐difference stimuli. The “nonellipsoid standard error method” was used in the current analyses. Three datasets were evaluated: BFD‐P, Leeds, and Witt. For the BFD‐P data, incorporating visual uncertainty led to the same performance results as the average results, that CIEDE2000 was an improvement over CIE94, which was an improvement over CIELAB. For the Witt data, incorporating visual uncertainty led to the same performance results as the average results, that CIEDE2000 and CIE94 had equivalent performance, both an improvement over CIELAB. However, both formulas under‐fitted the visual results; thus, neither formula was optimal. For the Leeds dataset, the visual uncertainty analysis did not support the improvement of CIEDE2000 over CIE94 that occurred when evaluating the average results. Both formulas well fit the visual data. These analyses also provided insight into the tradeoffs between the number of color‐difference pairs and the number of observations when fitting a local contour of equal perceived color difference: In particular, increasing the number of observations was more important than increasing the number of color‐difference pairs. Finally, average standard error could be used to approximate visual uncertainty defined using STRESS. © 2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2011  相似文献   

19.
化肥施用与土壤环境安全效应的研究   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
介绍我国肥料使用现状,分析化肥施用对土壤、环境的影响,并提出了提高化肥施用安全的若干建议。  相似文献   

20.
The objectives of this work were to develop a comprehensive visual dataset around one CIE blue color center, NCSU‐B1, and to use the new dataset to test the performance of the major color difference formulae in this region of color space based on various statistical methods. The dataset comprised of 66 dyed polyester fabrics with small color differences ($\Delta E_{{\rm ab}}^* < 5$ ) around a CIE blue color center. The visual difference between each sample and the color center was assessed by 26 observers in three separate sittings using a modified AATCC gray scale and a total of 5148 assessments were obtained. The performance of CIELAB, CIE94, CMC(l:c), BFD(l:c), and CIEDE2000 (KL:KC:KH) color difference formulae based on the blue dataset was evaluated at various KL (or l) values using PF/3, conventional correlation coefficient (r), Spearman rank correlation coefficient (ρ) and the STRESS function. The optimum range for KL (or l) was found to be 1–1.3 based on PF/3, 1.4–1.7 based on r, and 1–1.4 based on STRESS, and in these ranges the performances of CIEDE2000, CMC, BFD and CIE94 were not statistically different at the 95% confidence level. At KL (or l) = 1, the performance of CIEDE2000 was statistically improved compared to CMC, CIE94 and CIELAB. Also, for NCSU‐B1, the difference in the performance of CMC (2:1) from the performance of CMC (1:1) was statistically insignificant at 95% confidence. The same result was obtained when the performance of all the weighted color difference formulae were compared for KL (or l) 1 versus 2. © 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2011  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号