首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 20 毫秒
1.
Ethanol, morphine, cocaine and amphetamine were examined in place conditioning. After determination of initial preferences, animals were conditioned with ethanol (1 g/kg), morphine (5 mg/kg), cocaine (5 mg/kg) and amphetamine (5 mg/kg) alone or with combinations of these drugs plus naloxone (1 mg/kg). Naloxone prevented the ability of all drugs used to produce a place preference. The reinforcing properties of ethanol and morphine were reduced by sodium nitroprusside at a dose equal to 1/10 of LD50 given before preference testing. Molsidomine (1/10 LD50 and 1/20 LD50) altered the expression of the conditioned place preference produced by ethanol but not by morphine. Results of the present study suggest the involvement of endogenous opioids and probably of nitric oxide in the rewarding actions of drugs of abuse.  相似文献   

2.
The hedonic properties of lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) were assessed using the place conditioning, taste reactivity, and taste avoidance tests. LSD produced a conditioned place preference, but only at the highest dose tested (0.2 mg/kg). A single preexposure to the conditioning chamber (latent inhibition) prevented the establishment of a place preference. When paired with sucrose, doses of 0.05 to 0.2 mg/kg of LSD produced taste avoidance, but no dose of LSD produced an aversion to the taste as assessed by the taste reactivity test. These results suggest that LSD, like other rewarding drugs, produces taste avoidance by a mechanism other than that produced by emetic drugs. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Bilateral ibotenic acid lesions of the lateral, but not the medial, parabrachial nucleus (PBN) blocked conditioned taste aversion (CTA) induced by morphine but not conditioned place preference induced by morphine. The same lateral PBN lesions also blocked conditioned place aversion produced by low intraperitoneal doses of morphine (shown to produce aversion, instead of preference, due to a restricted action on gut opiate receptors). Lateral PBN lesions did not block CTA produced by LiCl. Cerebral peduncle lesions that destroyed the direct descending projections from the visceral cortex to the PBN did not block CTA induced by morphine, nor did ibotenic acid lesions of the tegmental pedunculopontine nuclei (shown to block place preference produced by even high morphine doses). It is suggested that the lateral PBN is a critical link in the neural pathway carrying the aversive motivational effects of opiates from the gut into the CNS, independent of the neural pathway carrying the rewarding motivational effects of morphine. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Rats have been shown to avoid consuming a flavor, but prefer a location, previously paired with amphetamine or morphine. A series of 4 experiments evaluated the hedonic properties of amphetamine and morphine in the house musk shrew (Suncus murinus), an insectivore that (unlike rats) is capable of vomiting when exposed to toxins. Unlike rats, amphetamine (20 mg/kg) and morphine (20 mg/kg) produced both a conditioned sucrose (0.3 M) and saccharin (0.1%) preference in shrews (administered intraperitoneally), when measured by both a 1-and a 2-bottle test. At the same dose, both drugs also produced a place preference in shrews. These results suggest that the potential of rewarding drugs to produce taste avoidance may vary on the basis of the ability of the species to vomit. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
The effect of microinjection of a GABAB receptor agonist, baclofen, into the ventral tegmental area on the rewarding effect of morphine was investigated using the conditioned place preference paradigm in rats. Morphine (1-8 mg/kg, s.c.) caused a dose-related place preference for the drug-associated place. In contrast, microinjection of baclofen (0.1-1 nmol/side) into the ventral tegmental area did not produce a significant preference for either compartment of the test box. Pretreatment with baclofen (0.1-1 nmol/side) into the ventral tegmental area dose dependently suppressed the morphine (8 mg/kg, s.c)-induced place preference. This suppression of the morphine (8 mg/kg, s.c.)-induced place preference by baclofen (1 nmol/side), but not with the GABAA receptor antagonist bicuculline (1 nmol/side). The present results suggest that a decrease in GABAB neurotransmission in the ventral tegmental area, which may be produced via inhibition of a tonic GABAergic input by morphine, may be involved in the expression of the rewarding effect of morphine.  相似文献   

6.
In this study, the authors investigated the role of enkephalins in morphine-induced conditioned place preference, locomotor sensitization, and analgesic tolerance. Both preproenkephalin wild type (ppENK [+/+]) and knockout (ppENK [-/-]) mice showed similar preference for the morphine-paired chamber over the vehicle-paired chamber, indicating morphine induced comparable conditioned place preference in ppENK (+/+) and ppENK (-/-) mice. Sensitization developed to the motor stimulatory action of morphine after its repeated administration, but the magnitude of this response was not altered in ppENK (-/-) mice. However, as shown previously, ppENK (-/-) mice displayed blunted morphine analgesic tolerance. Taken together, the results suggest that enkephalins may be important for the development of analgesic tolerance but not for conditioned place preference or behavioral sensitization induced by morphine. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
The motivational properties of morphine and nicotine were investigated in an automated conditioned place preference (CPP) procedure using a two-compartment apparatus. The accuracy of the photocell recording system was assessed by correlation with direct observation. In a counterbalanced conditioning design, graded doses of morphine (0.1-3.2 mg/kg SC) produced dose-related CPP. Under similar conditions, a dose of nicotine (0.6 mg/kg SC) previously reported to produce CPP failed to show an effect. Increasing the number of conditioning trials from 4 to 12 did not facilitate CPP with nicotine. After pretreatment with nicotine (0.4 mg/kg SC) daily for 7 days prior to conditioning, nicotine (0.4-0.8 mg/kg) produced increasing magnitudes of CPP. Locomotor activity was assessed during both conditioning and extinction tests. During conditioning, nicotine but not morphine decreased activity in the first conditioning trial, but by the fourth trial, marked stimulation was apparent following administration of either drug. Activity in the drug-paired compartment was not increased during tests for CPP carried out in the undrugged state following 4 conditioning trials with either morphine or nicotine, but there was evidence for conditioned hyperactivity after 12 conditioning trials with nicotine. The results suggest that motivational properties of nicotine can be detected in counterbalanced CPP procedures, but only in subjects with a history of nicotine exposure. The CPP produced by morphine or nicotine does not appear to be an artefact associated with conditioned changes in locomotor activity.  相似文献   

8.
The influence of thyrotropin-releasing hormone (TRH) on morphine-induced analgesic and reinforced responses was studied in two inbred strains of rats, Fischer-344 (F344) and Wistar Albino Glaxo/GSto (WAG). Conditioned place preference, voluntary consumption of morphine solution and analgesic action of morphine in tail immersion test were studied. There were interstrain differences in pain sensitivity, i.e., F344 rats had longer latency of tail immersion and deeper analgesic effect of morphine (5 mg/kg, ip) than WAG rats. TRH (1 mg/kg, ip) produced a stronger analgesic effect in WAG rats, while F344 rats demonstrated only slight increase in pain threshold. Administration of TRH in combination with morphine significantly stronger potentiated the effect of the latter in WAG than in F344 rats. F344 rats preferred morphine in the two-bottle choice test and consumed relatively larger amount of morphine solution in the drinking paradigm than WAG rats. Morphine in the dose of 5 mg/kg (ip) induced place preference in both rat strains. Intraventricular administration of TRH (1 mcg) produced a slight effect of place preference only in F344 rats. Preceded by morphine, such injection reduced the effect of place preference. It is suggested that WAG and F344 rats have different sensitivity of brain structures to TRH. This is probably determined by genetic differences in dissociation of analgesic and reinforcing effects of morphine.  相似文献   

9.
Bilateral electrolytic lesions of the nucleus accumbens in rats eliminated the capacity of 10 mg/kg morphine to produce a conditioned place preference (Experiment 1). However, these lesions did not alter the capacity to establish context-specific tolerance to the analgesic effects of 5 mg/kg of morphine (Experiment 2). This latter finding indicates that rats with nucleus accumbens lesions are not impaired in associating the effects of morphine with a particular location. Thus, the failure of morphine to produce a conditioned place preference in these lesioned rats probably cannot be attributed to an inability to associate the effects of morphine with a particular chamber, i.e., the initially nonpreferred chamber. Rather, morphine may fail to establish a conditioned place preference in these rats because nucleus accumbens lesions disrupt a pathway that is critical in mediating the rewarding effects of opiates. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
In 5 experiments with 164 male Wistar rats, Ss administered morphine (5 mg/kg, sc) during each of several daily sessions in an open field showed an increase in locomotor activity. Since increases were not observed in Ss given morphine in a different environment (home cage) and saline in the open field, it is concluded that they were due to conditioning. Increases in activity were retained over a 7-day rest period; they were also produced when a 2nd opiate (5 μg/kg etorphine) was substituted for morphine, were not seen when 2 mg/kg naloxone (ip) was administered during treatment, and were present in Ss showing tolerance to opiate-produced hypoactivity. Morphine's direct effect on activity is believed to have a biphasic dose–response curve; therefore, the relation of dose to conditioning was also studied. Increases in activity were the only conditioned behaviors observed; they were present only at the higher doses (16, 4, and 1 vs .25, .065, and 0 mg/kg), and they occurred whether or not the dose was associated with unconditioned hypoactivity. Discussion deals with the relation of conditioning and morphine tolerance, the question of whether the UCS of morphine conditioning is a compensatory or a direct effect of morphine, and the similarity of conditioned increases in activity produced by morphine and by other reinforcing stimuli. (40 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
When a multisensory environment was reliably paired with morphine (2 mg/kg) in rats, that environment, in a drug-free test, evoked a hyperactive conditioned response (CR). When an olfactory cue (banana odor) was the only stimulus element reliably paired with morphine, it also elicited a hyperactive CR. However, a gustatory cue (saccharin solution) evoked a hypoactive CR. This taste-elicited decrease in activity was dose dependent; morphine at 2 and 4 mg/kg conditioned hypoactivity, whereas a higher dose (8 mg/kg) did not. A robust conditioned saccharin aversion occurred only at the highest dose of morphine, suggesting disassociation between the hypoactive CR and taste aversion. A taste cue present during context conditioning also prevented either acquisition or expression of the hyperactive CR to the context. The modality of the conditioned stimulus is a critical determinant of the form of the CR in a morphine locomotor conditioning paradigm. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Ejaculation-induced reward in the male rat was evaluated by the conditioned place-preference paradigm. It was supposed that ejaculation induces a reward state such that it can be conditioned to environmental stimuli. Males were allowed to ejaculate once and were then immediately transferred to a place-preference cage. One ejaculation produced place preference. Naloxone (16 mg/kg) not only blocked this place preference but also induced a place aversion. Naloxone by itself had no effect on place preference. It is suggested that release of endogenous opioids renders ejaculation rewarding. Pimozide, in a dose of 1 mg/kg, had no effect on ejaculation-induced reward. Dopamine thus seems to be of slight importance for that effect of copulation. Perhaps compulsive sexual activity obeys the same mechanisms as compulsive drug use in opiate addicts. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
The effects of diabetes on morphine-induced place preference in mice were examined. Morphine caused dose-related place preference in both diabetic and non-diabetic mice. This morphine-induced place preference in diabetic mice was greater than that in non-diabetic mice. The morphine (5 mg/kg)-induced place preference in both diabetic and non-diabetic mice was significantly antagonized by pretreatment with beta-funaltrexamine, a selective mu-opioid receptor antagonist, but not with naloxonazine, a selective mu1-opioid receptor antagonist. The morphine (5 mg/kg)-induced place preference in non-diabetic mice was attenuated by pretreatment with either naltriben, a selective delta2-opioid receptor antagonist, or 7-benzylidenenaltrexone. a selective delta1-opioid receptor antagonist. Moreover, the morphine (10 mg/kg)-induced place preference in non-diabetic mice was antagonized by pretreatment with 7-benzylidenenaltrexone (0.7 mg/kg). Although 7-benzylidenenaltrexone had no effect on the place preference induced by 5 mg/kg morphine in diabetic mice, it reduced the place preference induced by 3 mg/kg morphine. Furthermore, the morphine (5 mg/kg)-induced place preference in diabetic mice was significantly antagonized by co-pretreatment with beta-funaltrexamine (10 mg/kg) and 7-benzylidenenaltrexone (0.7 mg/kg). 2-Methyl-4a alpha-(3-hydroxyphenyl)- 1,2,3,4,4a,5,12,12a alpha-octahydroquinolino[2,3,3-g]isoquinoline (TAN-67), a non-peptide delta-opioid receptor agonist, produced place preference in diabetic, but not in non-diabetic mice. These results support the hypothesis that the morphine-induced place preference is mainly mediated through the activation of the mu2-opioid receptor. Furthermore, the enhancement of the morphine-induced place preference in diabetic mice may be due to the up-regulation of delta-opioid receptor-mediated functions.  相似文献   

14.
As a part of the mesocorticolimbic system, the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is thought to participate in the regulation of locomotor activity, motivation and reward. The mPFC consists of at least three different subareas. In previous lesion studies examining this region usually large parts of the mPFC were destroyed, with little discrimination between the different subareas. Therefore, this study was designed to selectively lesion the prelimbic area of the mPFC using a relatively low dose of quinolinic acid. In a conditioned place preference (CPP) experiment, lesioned and control animals were treated with cocaine (15 mg/kg), amphetamine (4 mg/kg), morphine (10 mg/kg) or MK-801 (0.3 mg/kg) to induce CPP. The lesion blocked the development of CPP only in animals receiving cocaine, but not in animals receiving amphetamine or morphine. MK-801 failed to produce a CPP in both lesioned and unlesioned animals. During the conditioning experiment, the acute locomotor response to the different drugs was also measured. Only the response (in terms of locomotion and rearing) to cocaine and MK-801 was reduced to a significant extent by the lesion, while the response to amphetamine and morphine was not affected. Also, the lesions did not cause any changes in the spontaneous activity of the animals when tested without drug. These results show that even small lesions of the prelimbic subarea of the mPFC are sufficient to produce behavioral effects. However, these are apparent only when the animals are challenged with cocaine or MK-801, but not with amphetamine or morphine, or when drug-free. This suggests that the mPFC might have a special role in mediating cocaine and MK-801 effects.  相似文献   

15.
[Correction Notice: An erratum for this article was reported in Vol 107(2) of Behavioral Neuroscience (see record 2008-10474-001). Table 1, on page 119, contains two errors. In the first section, the dose/route for the agent nicotine should read as follows: 1.2-2.0 mg/kg sc. In the second section, the dose/route for the agent morphine should read as follows: 2-80 mg/kg ip. Also, on page 121, paragraph 3, line 14, the parenthetical information after 40 mg/kg cocaine should read (40C; 2 × 20 mg/kg/3 cc).] The nature of flavor–drug associations produced by a range of doses of the reinforcing agents cocaine (5, 10, 15, 20, or 40 mg/kg, sc), phencyclidine (0.5, 2, 10, or 20 mg/kg, sc), and methamphetamine (2, 5, or 10 mg/kg, ip) were assessed by the taste reactivity (TR) test and the conditioned taste avoidance (CTA) test. Even at the highest doses tested, none of the agents produced aversive TR responding. At doses that produced equivalent-strength CTA, lithium did establish aversive TR responding. Results provide evidence that drugs that serve as reinforcers in other paradigms produce conditioned flavor avoidance that is not motivated by a conditioned dislike for the flavor. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
The question of whether a common mechanism mediates both aversive and rewarding drug-paired cues is still unclear. In this study, we used a place preference conditioning paradigm to train rats to associate 1 chamber with morphine and the other chamber with saline. On the test day, rats were divided into those displaying conditioned place preferences (CPP) versus conditioned place aversion (CPA). After the test, all rats were killed and c-Fos immunocytochemistry was performed. For the control group, rats were treated with the same procedure except that the injections of morphine or saline had no association with the chambers. Compared with the control group, the CPP and CPA groups showed a significant increase of c-Fos expression in the dorsomedial striatum, central medial nucleus of the thalamus, and the basolateral amygdala. However, we saw no difference between CPP and CPA rats in any brain region examined. These results suggest that a morphine-paired environment can elicit neural activity in brain regions that are involved in emotional learning. Morphine-conditioned place preference and aversion may share a common neural circuitry elicited by a morphine-paired environment. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Four experiments examined the effect of naloxone pretreatment on the expression and extinction of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference (experiments 1, 2, 4) or conditioned place aversion (experiments 1, 3). DBA/2 J mice received four pairings of a distinctive tactile (floor) stimulus (CS) with injection of ethanol (2 g/kg) given either immediately before or after 5-min exposure to the CS. A different stimulus was paired with injection of saline. Pre-CS injection of ethanol produced conditioned place preference, whereas post-CS injection of ethanol produced conditioned place aversion. Both behaviors extinguished partially during repeated choice testing after vehicle injection. Naloxone (10 mg/kg) had little effect on the initial expression of conditioned place preference, but facilitated its extinction. Moreover, repeated naloxone testing resulted in the expression of a weak conditioned place aversion to the CS that initially elicited a place preference. In contrast, naloxone (1.5 or 10 mg/kg) enhanced expression of conditioned place aversion, thereby increasing its resistance to extinction. A control experiment (experiment 4) indicated that repeated testing with a different aversive drug, lithium chloride, did not affect rate of extinction or produce an aversion to the CS previously paired with ethanol. These findings do not support the suggestion that naloxone facilitates the general processes that underlie extinction of associative learning. Also, these data are not readily explained by the conditioning of place aversion at the time of testing. Rather, naloxone's effects appear to reflect a selective influence on maintenance of ethanol's conditioned rewarding effect, an effect that may be mediated by release of endogenous opioids. Overall, these findings encourage further consideration of the use of opiate antagonists in the treatment of alcoholism.  相似文献   

18.
Reports results of 3 experiments with a total of 228 male Wistar rats. In Exp I it was shown that chronic prior exposure to amphetamine attenuated the conditioned avoidance of saccharin that was produced by both amphetamine and morphine during gustatory conditioning trials; the relationship between morphine and amphetamine was somewhat anomalous because of their pharmacological dissimilarity. The relationship was also asymmetrical, since in Exp II chronic prior exposure to morphine failed to mitigate avoidance conditioning by amphetamine but was effective in attenuating conditioning by morphine itself. In Exp III it was found that prior treatment with chlordiazepoxide attenuated saccharin avoidance conditioned by chlordiazepoxide but not by amphetamine or morphine. The findings were related to the L. Parker et al (1973) hypothesis, based on findings with morphine, concerning the development of conditioned preferences for substances associated with the repletion of artifically induced biological needs. It is suggested that findings are best interpreted as a reflection of drug tolerance rather than conditioned preference. (18 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
(+)-5-Methyl-10,11-dihydro-5H-dibenzo[a,d]cyclohepten-5,10-imine maleate, (MK-801) a potent noncompetitive antagonist of central NMDA receptors, has been hypothesized to have rewarding properties indicative of abuse potential. To test this hypothesis, the effects of MK-801 on the acquisition of a conditioned place preference and on locomotor activity were assessed and compared with d-amphetamine. Both MK-801 (0.03 and 0.1 mg/kg, SC) and d-amphetamine (1.0 mg/kg, SC) administration resulted in the acquisition of a conditioned place preference. However, while both amphetamine and the higher dose of MK-801 produced a behavioral activation during the training period the lower dose of MK-801 did not. These results suggest that MK-801, at doses that produce behavioral activation and below, is rewarding and therefore may have abuse potential.  相似文献   

20.
Adult male rats were exposed ad libitum for 40 days to 100 ppm cadmium chloride through their diet, or an identical diet with no added cadmium. Conditioned place preference (CPP) was conducted in a 2-chamber apparatus in which all drugs were paired with the least-preferred side as determined on a pre-test. In Experiment 1. control and cadmium-exposed rats received 0, 0.6, 1.25, 2.5, or 5 mg/kg morphine sulfate (i.p.) for 4 days, and vehicle only for 4 days. Control animals showed a preference for the drug-paired side at 1.25, 2.5, and 5 mg/kg while the cadmium-exposed rats showed a preference at 5 mg/kg only. In Experiment 2, rats were implanted with cannulae into the lateral ventricles and 0, 2, 5 micrograms morphine sulfate was administered intracerebroventricularly (i.c.v.). An attenuation by cadmium again was observed, as control animals showed a place preference at 2 and 5 micrograms and cadmium-exposed animals showed preference at 5 micrograms only. In Experiment 3, increasing doses of the mu-opioid receptor agonist fentanyl (0, 0.0004, 0.004, and 0.04 mg/kg) were systemically administered (s.c.) and rats tested for CPP. While cadmium animals showed place preference only at 0.04 mg/kg, control animals showed preference at 0.0004, 0.004, and 0.04 mg/kg. These findings are discussed within the framework of metal-induced disturbance of neurochemical function and/or associative processing, and the implications that such disturbances may have for drug seeking and taking.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号