首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 109 毫秒
1.
《Ergonomics》2012,55(9):1114-1128
The luminance surrounding a computer display can potentially reduce visibility of the display (disability glare), result in sensations of discomfort (discomfort glare) and result in transient adaptation effects from fixating back and forth between the two luminance levels. The study objective was to measure the effects of surround luminance levels upon these functions in younger and older adults to determine recommended surround luminance levels. The younger age group comprised 20 subjects (mean age 27.9 years, range 23 – 39) and the older group 17 subjects (mean age 55.5 years, range 47 – 63). The central task was presented with luminance of 91 cd/M2, tested surround luminance levels were 1.4, 2.4, 8.9, 25.5, 50, 91, 175, 317, and 600 cd/M2. Disability glare was tested with low contrast (20%) visual acuity charts, transient adaptation was tested with a task that required regular fixation between the two luminance levels, discomfort was measured with a questionnaire after reading stories with different surround luminance levels, and preferred luminance was measured by method of adjustment. The surround luminance significantly affected transient adaptation (p < 0.0001), optimal performance occurred at 50 cd/M2 and above for the young group and at 91 cd/M2 and above for the older group. Neither low contrast acuity (disability glare) nor symptoms when reading were significantly affected by surround luminance. There was wide variation in preferred surround luminance; however, average preferred surround luminance was 86.9 cd/M2 for the young group and 62.2 cd/M2 for the older group, slightly below the central luminance of 91 cd/M2. The effects of the surround luminance within the tested range are not large; however, the data show that the lowest surround luminance levels should be avoided and that surround luminance levels at or slightly below that of the central task are preferred.  相似文献   

2.
Sixty young (18-25 years) and 91 older volunteers (60-87 years) were tested for static visual acuity under six different luminance levels ranging from 245.5 cd/m2 (photopic) to 0.2 cd/m2 (mesopic). The results showed significant differences in log decimal acuity and in passing a 20/40 acuity criterion score as a function of age and luminance level. There were no differences, however, in comparisons between young subjects and those aged 60-64. It seems, therefore, that when using visual acuity measures, as is done for driver licensing, 65 years is the critical age after which visual acuity becomes significantly poorer under conditions of degraded illumination. The implications for issuing driver's licenses to individuals over age 65 based on standard visual acuity testing are discussed.  相似文献   

3.
Abstract— High‐intensity light sources illuminating the human eye may create discomfort glare, or at higher intensities even disability glare. In many office lighting conditions, light from overhead luminaires in the ceiling may deliver stray light into human eyes, and as such create discomfort glare, generally referred to as overhead glare. In this paper, overhead glare for a LED luminaire comprising a matrix array of small LED sources using subjective evaluation methodologies and theoretical models, commonly accepted to evaluate glare, were investigated. The perceived overhead glare of the LED luminaire is evaluated at various luminance levels and at different angles (i.e., between 55 and 90°) with respect to the line of sight of the viewer. The results show that a luminaire comprising a matrix of high‐intensity point sources can cause overhead glare and can become glary at lower averaged luminance levels than a luminaire with a uniform light source even at high evaluation angles with respect to the line of sight of the viewer. In addition, the conventional UGR model for predicting discomfort glare needs adaptation for a reliable prediction of perceived overhead glare of complex LED luminaires consisting of a matrix of small‐sized high‐intensity light sources.  相似文献   

4.
In order to provide practical and useful data for display design, border detection with various levels of surround luminance from 0.001 to 500 cd/m2 is investigated. The results indicated that luminance difference for border detection exhibits a monotonically increasing curve against the reference luminance on a logarithmic scale. To compare the luminance difference curves for different surround luminances, the results were converted to contrast threshold curves against the reference luminance that are U‐shaped on a logarithmic scale. These contrast thresholds, however, are higher than those of the grayscale standard display function curve. The causes of the difference between the contrast threshold curves of our study and the grayscale standard display function curve are the adaptation conditions and stimulus size, as well as the difference in criteria between the detection of a border and the detection of non‐uniformity. Finally, the necessary number of gradation levels for border detection under various surround luminances is calculated and found to be at least double the current number of levels (256).  相似文献   

5.
This study investigated drivers' visual perception of information displayed in LED traffic signs under different levels of environmental luminance. Concerning the information displayed, the environmental luminance comprised four luminance contrasts, two arrow types, and three environmental luminances, which were used to identify the visibility of traffic signs at the reading distance. Regarding visible distance, visibility involved reading starting and end points, extent of glare, comfortable distance, and glare distance. According to the study results obtained by using a highly reliable glare assessment method and studying the LED traffic signs, the lower the luminance contrast of traffic signs was the higher glare the participants perceived. A luminance contrast of 6200:2066 cd/m2 provided the farthest comfortable and glare distances, enabling drivers to notice the signs comparatively earlier and have sufficient time to react accordingly. Overall, arrow type “<<<” outperformed “←” because the former was easier for drivers to read and created less glare. Regarding environmental luminance, traffic signs displayed in high and medium luminance environments (i.e., extremely sunny and cloudy days, respectively) each featured their own advantage, whereas those displayed in low luminance environments (nights) have to be further improved.  相似文献   

6.
Liquid crystal displays maintain superior contrast under bright sunlight conditions compared with cathode-ray tube displays. This attribute, along with reduced weight, volume, and power requirements, make liquid crystal displays especially desirable for use in vehicular applications. The present experiment was designed to determine the luminance requirements for transmissive liquid crystal displays viewed under a wide range of lighting conditions typical of many vehicular environments. Both the ambient illumination incident on the display surface and the forward-field-of-view adaptation luminance were parametrically manipulated. The visual task involved speeded spatial discrimination responses for detection of the gap in a modified Landolt-C acuity target. The results indicate that under the worst lighting conditions tested, a display luminance of approximately 180 cd/m2 yielded asymptotic spatial discrimination performance. The results have been incorporated into a predictive visual performance model for spatial discrimination tasks, which describes the present data well and represents a first step toward a device-independent model of display visibility.  相似文献   

7.
Twenty-nine subjects carried out a 3 hour reading task at a simulated CRT. In the apparatus chopper discs generated oscillating luminances of bright characters of 30, 60, 90, 180 and 0 Hz. A further control experiment was conducted with a printed text. The oscillating luminances had an approximate decay time of 4 ms (to 10% of peak luminance). The character luminance was kept constant (75 cd/m2). In the simulated CRT all characters oscillated simultaneously.Before, during and after each reading task, the following parameters were determined: Near point distance, critical flicker frequency (CFF), visual acuity, heterophorias, contrast sensitivity, eye blinking rate, and subjective symptoms of discomfort. The reading speed was continuously recorded. Furthermore the CFF at the simulated CRT was determined.The range of individual CFF at the simulated CRT was found between 40 and 56 Hz. It is concluded that most models of VDTs have today refresh rates lying in a critical range of CFF. In every condition the reading task was found to have an effect on visual functions and caused visual discomfort. Differentiated results among the six conditions were only observed for CFF and visual discomfort. The visible flicker (30 Hz) produced the strongest effects while 180 Hz, 0 Hz and the printed text were associated with relatively small effects.  相似文献   

8.
Computer work is a visually demanding task associated with adverse eye symptoms. Frequent use of digital displays is known to cause a deterioration of the so-called binocular control. Direct glare further reduces the capacity for binocular coordination during computer work, leading to reduced reading ability and increased eye symptoms.The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of different luminance levels of direct glare on binocular eye movement control and reading ability in a computer work environment.Sixteen participants with normal binocular vision performed equal reading tasks in a balanced study. Three controlled lighting conditions of direct glare (2000, 4000 and 6000 cd/m2) were tested, in addition to no glare. After each trial, the participants answered survey questionnaires regarding their understanding of the text, as well as their subjective experience of workload and perceived vision. Horizontal fixation disparity (FD) was measured before and after the reading tasks to evaluate binocular eye movement control.When comparing the responses of visual experience, a significant difference in reported eye symptoms was found between lighting conditions. Based on the variation (SD), a significant difference was found within mean values of repeated measurements of horizontal FD and a significantly higher variation in a comparison of initial FD values measured during lighting conditions of no glare, to final measured values in all three glare conditions. Reading ability was found to be significantly negative affected with the adversity of lighting conditions.This study supports the contention that binocular eye movement control is reduced caused by direct glare. Even lower degree of disability glare caused eye symptoms. The results establish the argument that working with flat screens raises visual demands.  相似文献   

9.
Abstract— To understand actual viewing conditions at home is important for TV design. And the preferred luminance level of LCD TVs under actual viewing conditions is also important in order to obtain both good picture quality and low power consumption. The actual viewing conditions of households and the preferred luminance levels was investigated. In a field test of 83 households, the display luminance, screen illuminance, and viewing locations were measured on site. In laboratory experiments, young and elderly subjects adjusted the luminance of an LCD‐TV screen to their preferred levels under different screen illuminance levels, angular screen sizes, and average luminance levels (ALL) of the images. As a result, two equations, which represent the preferred luminance level of LCD‐TV screens corresponding to different viewing conditions for young and elderly subjects were obtained. When the ALL of the images was 25% and the screen illuminance and angular screen size were set at 100 lx and 20°, respectively, the preferred luminance was 1 60 cd/m2 for the young subjects and 248 cd/m2 for the elderly subjects. By using the setting of the preferred luminance of an LCD TV under actual viewing conditions, it is possible to conserve energy consumption.  相似文献   

10.
We examine the effect of the (luminance) ratio of ambient luminance to tablet surface luminance on text legibility and fatigue over 1 h of long-term reading with a tablet PC in low-ambient-luminance environments limited to 45 cd/m2 (illuminance ~230 lx). Four reading-pattern subject experiments are carried out at ambient-luminance-to-tablet-surface-luminance ratios of 1:1, 1:3, 1:6, and 1:9 at 15 and 45 cd/m2. As per legibility tests based on the semantic differential (SD) method, we find that subjects experience glare at luminance ratios of 1:6 and 1:9, and it is difficult to read text at the luminance ratio of 1:9 under ambient luminances of 15 and 45 cd/m2. Based on subject fatigue (determined by subjective symptom survey), we find that the feeling of fatigue, according to the change in the luminance ratio, most affects the Fuzziness group among the five subjective fatigue groups. Based on physical fatigue determined using the critical flicker fusion frequency (CFF) value, we observe significant CFF differences before and after reading for luminance ratios of 1:1 and 1:9 at the ambient luminance of 45 cd/m2 and for the ratio of 1:9 when the ambient luminance is 15 cd/m2. A larger difference between the ambient and surface luminances corresponds to higher CFF variation rates and more fatigue.  相似文献   

11.
Combined effects of working environmental conditions in VDT work   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
The combined effects of city noise and luminance of the computer display were evaluated from the changes in lymphocytes and mental activities of participants. Healthy male students were tested under the following four experimental conditions: (1) a calculating task on a video display terminal (VDT) with luminance of 90 cd m(-2) without city noise; (2) a calculating task on a VDT with luminance of 20 cd m(-2) without city noise; (3) a calculating task on a VDT with luminance of 90 cd m(-2) with city noise of 70 dB(A); and (4) a calculating task on a VDT with luminance of 20 cd m(-2) with city noise of 70 dB(A). A visual reaction test (VRT) was performed, and critical flicker fusion frequency (CFF), heart rate (HR), numbers of circulating white blood cells (WBCs), lymphocyte subsets and subjective symptoms of fatigue were measured (1) before; (2) just after; and (3) 30 min after each 60 min test. Subjective symptoms of fatigue significantly increased just after experiments conducted under the two noisy conditions. VRT and CFF showed significant changes in the case of the high-luminance display with noise. WBCs and neutrophils showed significant increases in the two quiet conditions. These results suggested that high luminance with noise had the most effect on subjective fatigue and mental activities.  相似文献   

12.
《Ergonomics》2012,55(5):562-570
The combined effects of city noise and luminance of the computer display were evaluated from the changes in lymphocytes and mental activities of participants. Healthy male students were tested under the following four experimental conditions: (1) a calculating task on a video display terminal (VDT) with luminance of 90 cd m-2 without city noise; (2) a calculating task on a VDT with luminance of 20 cd m-2 without city noise; (3) a calculating task on a VDT with luminance of 90 cd m-2 with city noise of 70 dB(A); and (4) a calculating task on a VDT with luminance of 20 cd m-2 with city noise of 70 dB(A). A visual reaction test (VRT) was performed, and critical flicker fusion frequency (CFF), heart rate (HR), numbers of circulating white blood cells (WBCs), lymphocyte subsets and subjective symptoms of fatigue were measured (1) before; (2) just after; and (3) 30 min after each 60 min test. Subjective symptoms of fatigue significantly increased just after experiments conducted under the two noisy conditions. VRT and CFF showed significant changes in the case of the highluminance display with noise. WBCs and neutrophils showed significant increases in the two quiet conditions. These results suggested that high luminance with noise had the most effect on subjective fatigue and mental activities.  相似文献   

13.
《Ergonomics》2012,55(12):1239-1246
Visual acuity of nine subjects was measured with Landolt ‘C’s during 3 hours of exposure to either (i) a neutral control, (ii) a warm humid or (iii) a hot dry atmosphere. The luminance of the background was either 450cd/m2 or 5cd/m2. Simultaneously, rectal temperature and heart rate were recorded as indicators of thermal strain. A significant loss of visual acuity, linearly correlated with thermal strain, was found in both lighting conditions.  相似文献   

14.
Abstract— Recent commercial liquid‐crystal‐display (LCD) televisions are larger and brighter than traditional televisions, thus impacting the viewing conditions in which they are viewed. These changes in viewing conditions may require different electro‐optical transfer functions (EOTFs) for LCD TVs than those for conventional TVs. Here, the way various EOTFs affect the preferred image quality of test images with changes in brightness and surround illumination conditions are examined. The first method used a gain, offset, and gamma (GOG) function with a range of gamma values, and the second method altered the intrinsic EOTFs. Image preference for the simulated EOTFs was determined using a paired‐comparison experiment for ten images. The first experiment took place in a darkened room at two display luminance levels. The results indicated that a gamma of 1.6 was most preferred overall although more so at a lower screen luminance level. In a second experiment, the procedure was repeated with a dim surround of 10% of the display's white point. With this surround, preference for a gamma value of around 1.6 at both screen luminance levels was more enhanced. These results indicated that image preference for different EOTFs is dependent on display luminance and that this dependence is maintained with a dim surround.  相似文献   

15.
Abstract— Intraocular glare and simultaneous contrast control appearance in high‐dynamic‐range (HDR) images. This paper describes unique test targets that simulate real images. These targets change the HDR range by 500 times, without significantly changing the veiling glare on the retina. As well, these targets have nearly constant simultaneous contrast. The range of appearances possible from HDR images with different average luminances were measured. The targets displayed a maximum luminance range of 5.4 log units. Using magnitude estimates (MagEst) of appearances, the relationship between luminance and lightness from white to black was measured. With one exception, only small changes in appearance with large changes in dynamic range were found. It was also found that appearance was scene‐dependent. The same dark grays (MagEst = 10) were observed with luminances of 10, 4.2, 1.1, and 0.063, depending on the percentage of white area in the surround. Glare from more white increases the retinal luminance of the test areas. Simultaneous contrast counteracts glare by making the appearance range (white‐black) with a much smaller range of luminances. Appearance is controlled by both the optical scattered light and the spatial processing. A single tone‐scale function of luminance cannot describe appearance controlled by scatter and spatial processing.  相似文献   

16.
Glare from oncoming headlights is a problem for nighttime drivers because it can decrease visual acuity and cause discomfort. This diminished visual ability and discomfort due can increase drivers’ risk for traffic accidents. Older drivers experience more severe detrimental effects from nighttime glare, and these effects may pose a growing roadway hazard as the number of older drivers increases. The increased brightness of popular high-intensity-discharge (HID) headlights may further exacerbate these visibility problems. In a sample of younger (under 40 years of age) and older (40 years of age and older) drivers, we examined the impact of headlight glare from HID and traditional halogen lights on driving performance in a simulator, as well as the effectiveness of novel polarized glare-reducing eyeglasses for mitigating glare-induced performance deficits. The glare-reducing glasses increased visual awareness in the face of oncoming HID headlights compared to halogen headlights in both age groups. Older drivers performed significantly worse than did younger drivers on several measures of driving and visual detection performance. The glare-reducing glasses mitigated performance deficits, with older drivers performing similarly to younger drivers when exposed to HID headlights while wearing the polarized glasses. Due to the introduction of brighter LED-based headlights to the consumer automotive market and an expanding population of older drivers, automotive manufactures should consider glare-mitigation strategies when designing future headlight systems.  相似文献   

17.
Adaptive curve headlights swivel with steering input and are linked to reduced insurance claims and improved visual performance. This study assessed glare experienced from adaptive curve high-intensity discharge (HID), fixed (non-swiveling) HID and fixed halogen headlights – all tested in low beam mode. Twenty participants rated glare from vehicles’ headlights using the DeBoer visual discomfort scale as a test driver drove towards them from five approaches on a test track. Participants rated the fixed halogen condition as less glaring than the adaptive curve and fixed HID conditions. There was no significant difference in ratings between the HID low-beam conditions. Collapsing across roadway approaches, the mean subjective ratings for the fixed halogen, adaptive curve HID and fixed HID low-beam conditions indicated ‘satisfactory’ levels of glare. Differences between subjective ratings were supported by illuminance data. Differences among the three low-beam systems appear minor, relative to their differences from a benchmark high-beam condition.

Practitioner Summary: Insurance data indicates reduced claims associated with adaptive curve lighting. The current effort was to study how such lighting affects discomfort glare of oncoming drivers relative to conventional headlights. Participants rated halogen headlights as less glaring than fixed and adaptive curve HID low beams. Differences among systems were small and associated with acceptable levels of discomfort glare.  相似文献   


18.
BackgroundGlare can reduce a driver's visual performance, increasing the risk of traffic accidents. The ability of the visual system to process spatial information can be assessed with the contrast sensitivity function (CSF); the CSF is decreased by glare.ObjectiveThe current study had three key aims: to examine whether and how glare-induced contrast sensitivity (CS) loss is modulated by spatial frequency (between 0.5 and 16 cycles per degree) and external noise (zero, low, or high); to evaluate subjective feelings of discomfort under different glare conditions; and to investigate the underlying mechanisms with a perceptual template model (PTM).MethodsParticipants completed a contrast detection task with quick CSF algorithm and reported their subjective feelings under nonglare, steady-glare, and transient-glare conditions.ResultsSteady and transient glare both reduced CS to comparable degrees. When external noise was absent, the CS reduction was largely dependent on spatial frequency, with a larger amount of CS loss at lower spatial frequencies. When external noise was present, the CS reduction was independent of spatial frequency. In addition, the reduction in the area under the log CS function (AULCSF) under steady-glare condition was roughly the same as that under transient-glare condition, regardless of external noise. However, participants reported greater discomfort under transient-glare condition than under steady-glare condition. Moreover, the AULCSF was reduced more under the zero-noise condition than under noise conditions. The PTM suggested that an increase in internal noise and an impaired perceptual template explained the glare-induced CS loss.ConclusionThe glare-induced CS loss was modulated by spatial frequency and external noise. Steady glare and transient glare affect CS to a similar extent, but steady glare produces less discomfort. Glare attenuates signals and weakens noise exclusion ability.ApplicationThis study advances our knowledge on glare-induced CS loss and its underlying mechanisms. The qCSF procedure with external noise should be utilized in future studies to investigate the effect of various light sources on visual function.  相似文献   

19.
Kline DW  Buck K  Sell Y  Bolan TL  Dewar RE 《Human factors》1999,41(3):356-364
To determine if visual aging affects the ability to identify blurred text signs in daytime or nighttime viewing conditions, the photopic and mesopic legibility thresholds of young and old adult observers were compared at three levels of optically induced acuity (Experiment 1). For both age groups, legibility was reduced by nighttime luminance and degraded acuity. Surprisingly, older observers were better than younger ones in identifying defocused (optically blurred) text in both daytime and nighttime conditions. In Experiment 2, older observers were also superior to younger ones in identifying defocused standard and spatially matched novel text signs. These findings indicate that older observers' tolerance of optical blur is a generic ability, rather than one explained by familiarity with the low-pass optical profile of specific signs. Consistent with the notion that factors beyond acuity contribute more importantly to text legibility for older than for younger observers, acuity was a stronger predictor of legibility thresholds in the younger group. Actual or potential applications of this research include the need to consider functional acuity in visual screening protocols and the design of text displays, as well as the utilization of compensatory letter-recognition training for older observers or others with diminished acuity.  相似文献   

20.
The present study investigated the effects of discomfort glare on driving behavior. Participants (old and young; U. S. and Europeans) were exposed to a simulated low-beam light source mounted on the hood of an instrumented vehicle. Participants drove at night in actual traffic along a track consisting of urban, rural, and highway stretches. The results show that the relatively low glare source caused a significant drop in detecting simulated pedestrians along the roadside and made participants drive significantly slower on dark and winding roads. Older participants showed the largest drop in pedestrian detection performance and reduced their driving speed the most. The results indicate that the deBoer rating scale, the most commonly used rating scale for discomfort glare, is practically useless as a predictor of driving performance. Furthermore, the maximum U. S. headlamp intensity (1,380 cd per headlamp) appears to be an acceptable upper limit.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号