首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Assessed the development of a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in 60 female Sprague-Dawley rats made hyperphagic with parasagittal knife cuts in the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH). Ss were water deprived and presented with a .1% saccharin solution paired with injections of either LiCl or NaCl. In Exp I, VMH Ss tested at a nonobese weight level did not differ from sham-operated controls in acquisition and extinction of the CTA. In Exp II, moderately obese VMH Ss displayed a stronger CTA than did sham-operated controls as evidenced by slower extinction. A 2nd group of obese VMH Ss given an amount of LiCl equivalent to that given to the controls also displayed retarded extinction of the CTA. Results reflect an obesity-induced suppression in appetitive motivation. (29 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Five experiments explored facilitated taste-aversion conditioning (odor-mediated taste augmentation), using rats that experienced odor (A) and taste (X) in an A+/AX+ design. Augmentation occurred when the stimuli were presented simultaneously during AX+ conditioning, and significantly weaker conditioning occurred after a sequential presentation (Exp 1). Exps 2 and 3 demonstrated that augmented conditioning decreased if the odor aversion was reduced through preexposure or extinction following A+ conditioning. A second-order conditioning explanation was not supported by the results of Exp 4. Exp 5 showed that extinction of the odor aversion after AX + conditioning did not alter the strength of the augmented taste aversion. Odor-mediated taste augmentation is similar to potentiation, in which odor and taste cues operate in a synergistic, not competitive, manner. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Three experiments involving 158 male rats showed that Ss injected with lithium chloride after drinking a saline-lemon solution also reduced their consumption of saline-lemon and sucrose-lemon, but a single exposure to sucrose-lemon decreased the generalized aversion. Exp 1 showed that this decrease in the aversion to sucrose-lemon could not be explained by a decrease either in the aversion conditioned to saline-lemon or in neophobia to sucrose-lemon. Exp 2 ruled out the possibility that generalization between saline-lemon and sucrose-lemon was based on shared novelty. Exp 3 showed that generalization between the 2 solutions was related to the strength of the aversion to the lemon flavor they shared in common and that prior exposure to sucrose-lemon reduced generalization by causing latent inhibition of this common element. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Three experiments exposed rats (Rattus norvegicus) to a discriminative conditioning procedure whereby a specific fluid was followed by lithium in one environment but not in another. This produced context-specific aversion to water, as detected by 2-bottle tests in Experiment 1, and a context-dependent saccharin aversion, which was unaffected by context extinction, in Experiment 2. Experiment 3 found that sucrose preexposure increased contextual control over the aversion established by sucrose-lithium pairings but had no effect on the target context. By contrast, target context exposure during conditioning reduced aversion to this context but did not affect contextual control of the sucrose aversion. In conclusion, depending on the conditioning procedures, contextual control of a taste aversion can be independent of the context's Pavlovian properties. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Examined evidence of latent inhibition in a series of experiments with goldfish. In Exp I, 12 Ss were given nonreinforced preexposure to a color that subsequently predicted shock in an activity conditioning situation; their performance did not differ from that of 12 control Ss preexposed to a markedly different color. In Exp II, 12 Ss given nonreinforced preexposure to a tone and an unstimulated control group of 12 Ss were trained in an appetitive situation, with the tone serving either as a conditioned excitor or as a conditioned inhibitor. Preexposure had significant effect in the conditioned excitation training, but it reduced the level of responding both to the positive stimulus and to the negative compound in the conditioned inhibition training. In Exps III and IV, classical aversive conditioning was studied in the shuttle box. In Exp III, excitatory conditioning to a color was found to be impaired (relative to the performance of nonpreexposed control Ss) as much by nonreinforced preexposure to the training color as by nonreinforced preexposure to a markedly different color; substantial variation in amount of preexposure was without significant effect. In the conditioned inhibition training of Exp IV, 12 Ss with nonreinforced preexposure responded less than did 12 unstimulated control Ss, both to the positive stimulus and to the negative compound. Results demonstrate that the effect of preexposure on goldfish is their reduction of general responsiveness or level of arousal. (47 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
A series of 6 experiments assessed the effects of ACTH and the ACTH analog ACTH4–20 on drinking in conditioned taste aversion and neophobic situations using a total of 168 male Sprague-Dawley rats as Ss. Both substances delayed the extinction of a conditioned taste aversion established by a single pairing of lithium chloride with milk (Exp I). However, in this situation, the ACTH parent peptide was more potent behaviorally. ACTH supressed milk consumption in Ss with no toxicosis experience (Exp II). This effect was apparently not due to the conditioning of a taste aversion (Exp III) with ACTH serving as a weak aversive UCS. Exogenous ACTH (Exp IV) or ACTH4–20 (Exp V) did not enhance neophobia; however, repeated injections of ACTH suppressed drinking. This ACTH suppression was related to the familiarity/novelty of the substance being consumed. The neophobic response to milk was not accompanied by pituitary-adrenal activation (Exp VI). Both neophobic and conditioned taste aversion situations appear to be useful for assessing peptide effects on consummatory behavior. (36 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Conditioned adult male and female cats by pairing a mild electrical stimulus to the superficial peroneal sensory nerve (CS) with a stronger electrical stimulus to the ankle skin (UCS) of the same leg. Subsequent extinction was produced by presenting CS-alone trials. In Exp I (42 Ss), Ss given massed extinction trials showed response decrements to base levels, but Ss that received distributed extinction trials showed no decrements. In Exp II, .5-, 1-, 2-, 3-, or 4-hr intervals between acquisition and extinction produced no significant differences in the extinction data. In Exp III (20 Ss), Ss received extinction trials immediately or 30 min after acquisition trials, followed by 20 additional extinction trials 30 min later. Data indicated significant acquisition and extinction in the 10- and 20-acquisition trial groups. As in Exp II (35 Ss), varying the interval between acquisition and extinction did not produce any group differences in the extinction data. These results demonstrate that response increases produced by paired trials in the spinal preparation do not decay spontaneously over time and are not caused by sensitization effects. (23 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Results from 3 experiments indicate that severing the subdiaphragmatic vagus in male Sprague-Dawley rats increased the rate of extinction of learned taste aversions. In Exp I, when the illness-inducing agent was the blood-borne toxin apomorphine, vagotomized Ss tended to consume more saccharin than controls during repeated extinction tests. In Exp II, vagotomy disrupted retention and increased extinction of a preoperatively acquired saccharin aversion. Disruptions were found when the taste aversion was induced by copper sulfate, a local gastric irritant, or apomorphine. Exp III demonstrated that vagotomy did not affect retention or extinction of a shock-induced conditioned emotional response to noise. It is concluded that integrity of the vagus is not necessary for acquisition of a learned taste aversion when a blood-borne toxin is used as the illness-inducing agent. However, the vagus apparently mediates an integral portion of the CR following taste–illness acquisition. (24 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Three experiments with 48 cats investigated memory for CR as a function of proactive inhibition. The proactive operation was the preexposure to quasi-random presentations of the potential CS and UCS. The possible CSs were light and tone, and the UCSs were brief mild shocks to either the right or left paw, which produced a brisk leg jerk. In Exp I, all possible combinations of CS and UCS components of the eventual CR were present in the preexposure period for one or another group as in the traditional interference paradigms of human paired-associate memory research. Exp II demonstrated that the decline cannot be attributed to a strategy type of interpretation that asserts that when the retention–extinction situation occurs, Ss "backward scan" and judge themselves to be once again in the preexposure period. Performance immediately after reaching the conditioning criterion did not differ between the controls that experienced no preexposure and the experimentals, but it did so after the 10-wk retention interval. Exp III investigated the role of context in the memory deficits by maintaining the same context in the preexposure, conditioning, and memory test situations or giving the preexposure experience in an environment different from the other 2 situations. Context change greatly reduced but did not eliminate the proactive inhibition. It is concluded that the CR is readily forgotten given appropriate interference and does not differ from other kinds of learning in this respect. (27 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Three experiments examined the endocrine mechanisms responsible for sex differences in prepubertal play behavior of ferrets. In Exp I, 6 gonadally intact adolescent males exhibited higher levels of "stand-over" behavior than 6 females did in tests between 63 and 123 days of age with gonadally intact female partners of the same age. In Exp II, with 69 Ss, those Ss exposed to androgen or to ovarian steroids over Days 5–20 of postnatal life subsequently exhibited significantly higher levels of stand-over behavior in tests with females than did control females gonadectomized on Day 5 and not given steroids. None of the Ss in Exp II exhibited levels of stand-over behavior comparable to those of the gonadally intact males in Exp I. In Exp III, with 36 Ss, males gonadectomized and implanted subcutaneously with testosterone capsules on Day 70 and tested with females at 84–96 days of age exhibited levels of stand-over behavior comparable to those observed in Exp I in gonadally intact males of the same age (Weeks 12–24). Males gonadectomized on Day 70 and given no hormone at testing exhibited significantly lower levels of this behavior. Significantly lower levels were also exhibited by males gonadectomized on Day 35 and females gonadectomized on Day 70, regardless of whether they were tested with testosterone present after Day 70. Sex differences in the expression of prepubertal play behavior of ferrets apparently result from differential exposure of males and females to androgen over an extended postnatal period. (25 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Conducted 2 experiments on the ability of different sex steroids to stimulate crowing, strutting, and copulation. Exp I with 32 male Japanese quail was designed to maximize crowing. Intact untreated males and castrated males treated with 2 dosages of testosterone propionate (TP), with 2 dosages of dihydrotestosterone propionate (DHTP), or with oil were tested. The DHTP-treated males crowed extensively; TP-treated males crowed, but to a lesser extent than DHTP-treated males. In Exp II (30 Ss), which was designed to maximize strutting and copulation, males with photically regressed testes were treated with DHTP, DHTP?+?estradiol benzoate (EB), or EB alone and were tested with female partners. The DHTP-treated males did not copulate, but 2 birds strutted. The EB-treated males copulated but did not strut. Males receiving DHTP?+?EB strutted and copulated. These results suggest that (a) copulation in quail may involve conversion of testosterone to estrogen by the brain; (b) crowing and strutting may involve conversion of testosterone to dihydrotestosterone by the brain; and (c) in quail, the different components of male reproductive behavior show divergent patterns of hormone responsiveness, and thus the neural tissues underlying these behaviors have different molecular requirements for activation by steroids. (42 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
In 2 experiments, 265 Swiss albino Rb-3 9-wk-old untreated mice, when placed in a novel environment composed of a central cage surrounded by 4 dark boxes in which different sounds were emitted, explored the boxes for 1 or 2 hrs. In most cases, Ss settled into 1 of the 4 boxes and built their nest. Most of the controls chose the silent compartment, whatever the sound delivered in the other boxes. An acoustic choice test was then carried out. Four groups of mice were exposed to a particular sound for 10 days: Group 1: Gestation Day 10 to birth; Group 2: birth to Day 10; Group 3: Day 10 to Day 20; Group 4: Day 20 to Day 30. Ss were then tested for box preference on Day 63 in the 4-box choice, one of which contained the sound of the earlier exposure. Ss stimulated before the 10th day after birth behaved in the same way as controls, whereas Ss stimulated between the 10th and the 20th days after birth exhibited a strong preference for the box delivering the sound of the previous treatment. This choice was significant, whatever the harmonic (Exp I) or impulsive (Exp II) nature of the sound. Ss stimulated between the 20th and the 30th days showed a preference for the silent box, but among the others, 75% chose the sound received during infancy. The behavior of Ss stimulated when adult was the same as that of the controls. (38 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Investigated the ability of animals to form taste aversions following neural manipulations. In Exp 1, 10 rats received intraoral infusions of sucrose every 5 min starting immediately after the injection of LiCl. 12 controls were injected with NaCl. Oromotor and somatic taste reactivity behaviors were videotaped and analyzed. Lithium-injected Ss decreased their ingestive taste reactivity over time; aversive behavior increased. Controls maintained high levels of ingestive responding and demonstrated virtually no aversive behavior following sodium injection. Ss were tested several days later for a conditioned taste aversion (CTA). Rats previously injected with lithium demonstrated significantly more aversive behavior than controls. Exp 3 revealed that when similarly treated rats were tested for a CTA while in a lithium-induced state, difference in the ingestive behavior was observed. In Exp 2, naive rats were injected with NaCl or LiCl but did not receive their 1st sucrose infusion for 20 min. Ss also received infusions at 25 and 30 min postinjection. There were no differences in the task reactivity behavior displayed. Rats dramatically changed their oromotor responses to sucrose during the period following LiCl administration, provided the infusions started immediately after injection, a change attributable to associative processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Preconditioning experience with the UCS retards subsequent excitatory conditioning. Three experiments demonstrated that the UCS retardation effect is attenuated by associative manipulations of contextual stimuli of the UCS preexposure environment. The UCS retardation effect was reduced by (a) altering contextual stimuli between preexposure and conditioning (Exp I, 49 New Zealand male rabbits; Exp II, 28 Ss); and (b) latently inhibiting contextual stimuli subsequent to UCS (Exp III, 36 Ss). Although UCS preexposure retarded excitatory conditioning, results of Exp IV (48 Ss) demonstrated that UCS preexposure facilitated inhibitory conditioning. Overall findings indicate that an association between contextual stimuli and preexposed UCS contributes to the effects of preconditioning UCS experience on subsequent learning. (48 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Conducted 2 experiments with 33 and 60 female hooded rats to determine ways of decreasing or increasing the efficacy of response prevention (flooding). In Exp. I, Ss were trained to avoid electric shock and following learning some groups were given the response-prevention treatment. (Response prevention consisted of thwarting the avoidance response while forcing Ss to remain in the presence of the feared stimuli.) Mechanically confining the Ss during response prevention (interfering with the occurrence of spontaneous exploration and locomotion) decreased the efficacy of the treatment in leading to extinction of the avoidance response. In Exp. II, Ss were trained to avoid intense shock and then were given response prevention in either 1 single, long-duration session or in several shorter sessions distributed over days (massed vs. distributed flooding). Results indicate that distributed response prevention was more effective than massed response prevention. (French summary) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
During a single 1-hr feeding test, adult male Norway rats were offered intact carcasses of adult male rodents, sacrificed by CO? asphyxiation. In Exp I, 79 Ss fed more readily on conspecifics dead 24–220 hrs than on conspecifics dead 10 min or 6 hrs. In Exp II, 22 Ss fed readily on roof rats (Rattus rattus) whether they had been dead 10 min or 24 hrs. In Exp III, 15 Ss rendered anosmic by zinc sulfate treatment fed more readily on conspecifics dead for 10 min than did 17 controls. In Exp IV, 34 Ss consumed more flesh from house mice coated with roof rat urine than from mice coated with Norway rat urine. It is concluded that Norway rats exhibit a natural aversion to feed on the intact carcasses of freshly sacrificed adult conspecifics in the sense that they are more likely to feed on other species of rodent. The aversion diminishes with the dissipation of the chemoreceptive cue(s) identifying the carcass as a conspecific. (64 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Four experiments with 202 rats investigated spontaneous configuring, using the conditioned flavor-aversion paradigm. In Exp I, extended training of a 2-flavor compound stimulus did not produce spontaneous differentiation of conditioned responding to that compound and its elements. In Exp II, it was found that extended nonreinforced exposure to a compound stimulus generated spontaneous element–compound differentiation when the elements were later conditioned. Ss that received extended preexposure to the compound showed less conditioned responding to the compound than to either of its elements. However, Ss that had not received preexposure to the compound showed greater conditioned responding to the compound than to either of its elements (summation). In Exp III, nonreinforced preexposure to a compound stimulus prior to minimal reinforced compound training produced spontaneous compound–element differentiation, but extended reinforced compound training eliminated that differentiation. In Exp IV, extended partial reinforcement training with a compound produced differentiation of the compound from its elements. Implications of these data for the mechanisms responsible for spontaneous configuring, and for the summation assumptions common to most learning theories, are discussed. (38 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
Conducted 2 experiments to investigate a possible antagonism between progesterone (P) and testosterone propionate (TP) with regard to the latter's role as a mediator of aggression-promoting pheromone. Ss were a total of 18 male BALB/cJ inbred mice trained as fighters and 64 castrated A/J male mice. Exp I showed that castrated Ss injected with TP were attacked by fighters faster, more often, and for a longer duration than castrates treated with TP and P. Exp II revealed that castrates coated with urine collected from castrated urine donors treated with TP were attacked more often and for longer periods than castrates coated with the urine of donors given TP and P. Thus, P appears to antagonize the effect of TP on aggression pheromone through a urinary pathway. The 2nd part of Exp II replicated and confirmed the results of Exp I. Results may be explained in terms of a peripheral biochemical antagonism with TP or, more likely, by the elicitation of inhibitory cues by P. (34 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
In Phase 1 of Exp I, with 60 male Sprague-Dawley rats, Ss given access to water while under the influence of scopolamine developed adipsic tolerance, but Ss denied access to water in the drug state did not. In Phase 2, Ss displayed adipsic tolerance only when scopolamine was administered with cues associated with previous drug injections. In Phase 3, adipsic-tolerant Ss showed a polydipsic response to an injection of phenobarbital relative to nontolerant Ss with the same pharmacological history. In Exp II, with 48 male Sprague-Dawley rats, the effect of deprivation level on the Pavlovian extinction of adipsic tolerance was assessed. Ss satiated with water during extinction showed a loss of adipsic tolerance, but water-deprived Ss did not. The proposed model is discussed in relation to (1) tolerance in other response systems such as morphine analgesia, (2) theories of extinction for nonpharmacological Pavlovian conditioning, and (3) homeostatic regulation and incentive motivation. (35 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
In Exp I, 24 intact male Lister rats were given either lactose or sucrose solutions. Although on 1st exposure they readily consumed lactose, its ingestion produced a conditioned taste avoidance (CTA) that was partly extinguished by repeated sucrose exposure after lactose conditioning. In Exp II, 8 Ss with large bilateral electrolytic lesions of the basolateral amygdala and 10 with either sham or no operations were given 2 pairings of saline with LiCl injections (upper gastrointestinal tract discomfort) and, in a separate condition, access to high levels of lactose (lower gastrointestinal tract discomfort). CTAs were measured both by 2-bottle tests and by video recordings of orofacial and somatic responses. The lesions attenuated LiCl-induced but not lactose-induced CTA, results demonstrating that a CTA can occur without the basolateral amygdala. Findings suggest that aversions based on distaste can be distinguished from avoidances based on danger, not only in terms of orofacial responses but also in terms of their neuroanatomical substrate. (31 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号