首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Male-induced estrus was examined in montane (Microtus montanus), meadow (M. pennsylvanicus), prairie (M. ochrogaster), and pine (M. pinetorum) voles. Duration of male contact needed for receptivity, effects of parity, and vaginal cytology were assessed. Among nulliparous females, montane voles attained receptivity with less male contact than prairie voles. Meadow and pine voles showed very low receptivity rates. Among parous females, montane and meadow voles did not differ in duration of male contact needed for receptivity and required less than prairie voles. Overall, parous females had higher receptivity rates than nulliparous females. When isolated from males, prairie and pine voles had more leukocytes and fewer cornified cells in vaginal smears than montane or meadow voles. Species differences in estrus induction are discussed in relation to species differences in social organization. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Examined male preference for unmated vs mated females in 2 species of voles, using 73 prairie voles and 78 montane voles in 2 testing situations each. In Exp I, conducted in a tether test situation, prairie voles spent significantly more time and copulated more with unmated than with mated females. In Exp II, male prairie voles spent significantly more time visiting and investigating anesthetized unmated females than anesthetized mated females. In Exp III, male montane voles showed no significant visitation or copulatory preference for unmated vs mated females in the tether situation. In Exp IV, male montane voles spent more time with unmated, anesthetized females than mated females but displayed no other significant differences. In general, male prairie voles appeared more discriminating in their mate choice than male montane voles. These differences are consistent with differences in male parental effort in the field. (29 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
12 male prairie voles and 12 male mountain voles were given test of copulatory behavior with 1, 2, or 4 mating partners. Changes in the number of available partners produced minimal changes in the parameters of copulatory behavior in males of the 2 species. Male montane voles generally copulated with more of the females, showed a lesser tendency to concentrate copulations on a single female, and changed females more often than did prairie voles. These differences are consistent with differences in social structure reported in field studies and may reflect processes underlying species differences in social organization. (33 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Parental behavior and pup development in meadow and prairie voles were examined. Social units were manipulated for the presence of fathers and of juveniles. Meadow vole mothers alone with pups showed less maternal care and spent more time resting than did mothers with juveniles or with fathers and juveniles. Pups reared only with mothers developed faster than did pups under other conditions. Number of animals in the social unit was negatively correlated with the rate of pup development. Meadow vole fathers and juveniles showed no parental care. In contrast, prairie vole mothers spent less time in the nest when both fathers and juveniles were present. Fathers assisted in rearing pups, and litters developed more rapidly when fathers were present. Juveniles remained in the natal nest and engaged in parentlike behavior. The findings relate to differences in the life history strategy for the 2 vole species. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Female meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) are territorial during warm months but demonstrate social tolerance under low temperatures. In spring, females nest together and some pairs participate in communal nursing and rearing of young. Because communal nursing involves significant cooperation, selective pair-bonds may develop between 2 nestmates. Using a choice apparatus, the authors determined that (a) captive females demonstrated partner preferences for a nestmate; (b) partner preferences were enduring and persisted after dyadic separation; and (c) following the loss of a nestmate, females did not develop preferences for a new nestmate, even after extended cohabitation. Data support the hypothesis that captive meadow voles develop selective and enduring same-sex social bonds that may, under free-living conditions, facilitate communal nesting and cooperative rearing of young. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Examined whether dithering behavior is the result of sexual imprinting on 2 species. When young zebra finch males are exposed to their own species, as well as to Bengalese finches later on, they may court both species; the stability as well as the exclusiveness of the preference was examined. In Exp I, dithering males that had been tested for their preference when adults were subsequently either isolated or given experience with conspecifics. Afterward, the males were tested again. Although preferences appeared to have become more zebra finch directed, especially in males with experience with conspecifics, most males in both groups still courted both species, results indicating that the dithering phenomenon is persistent. In Exp II, whether dithering indicates a double preference or the lack of a specific preference was examined. Dithering males preferred both zebra finches and Bengalese finches over (unfamiliar) white zebra finches and over silverbills. Results suggest that dithering indicates imprinting on both zebra and Bengalese finches. (15 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have been known to exhibit rudimentary abilities in analogical reasoning (Flemming, Beran, Thompson, Kleider, & Washburn, 2008; Gillian, Premack, & Woodruff, 1981; Haun & Call, 2009; Thompson & Oden, 2000; Thompson, Oden, & Boysen, 1997). With a wide array of individual differences, little can be concluded about the species' capacity for analogies, much less their strategies employed for solving such problems. In this study, we examined analogical strategies in 3 chimpanzees using a 3-dimensional search task (e.g., Kennedy & Fragaszy, 2008). Food items were hidden under 1 of 2 or 3 plastic cups of varying sizes. Subsequently, chimpanzees searched for food under the cup of the same relative size in their own set of cups—reasoning by analogy. Two chimpanzees initially appeared to fail the first relational phase of the task. Meta-analyses revealed, however, that they were instead using a secondary strategy not rewarded by the contingencies of the task—choosing on the basis of the same relative position in the sample. Although this was not the intended strategy of the task, it was nonetheless analogical. In subsequent phases of the task, chimpanzees eventually learned to shift their analogical reasoning strategy to match the reward contingencies of the task and successfully choose on the basis of relative size. This evidence not only provides support for the analogical ape hypothesis (Thompson & Oden, 2000), but also exemplifies how foundational conceptually mediated analogical behavior may be for the chimpanzee. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Analogical reasoning is a corner stone of human cognition, but the phylogenetic origins of this skill are still unknown. Recent animal studies have suggested that only apes can solve the 2- by 2-item relational matching (RMTS) analogy problem, with potential benefits of language- (Premack, 1983) or token-training procedures (Thompson, Oden, & Boysen, 1997). In this study, 6 baboons were initially trained in an RMTS task in which the same and different relations were exemplified by compound stimuli made of 2 adjacent patches of colors. Learning occurred in this task with a first set of colors and transferred to probe trials with new colors (Experiment 1). Manipulation of the size of the sample or comparison stimuli (Experiment 2) showed that the performance was not merely controlled by the surface of the color patches, suggesting cognitive flexibility. Performance collapsed to chance level when a gap was introduced between the 2 elemental features composing the same or different displays (Experiment 3). Nevertheless, this effect of gap size was abolished by training (Experiment 4). It is suggested that monkeys share the ability to judge relations between relations with humans and apes, even in the absence of language or token training. However, this ability has been previously masked by a local mode of processing that hinders the processing of the stimuli as pairs rather than as independent objects. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Vasopressin-immunoreactive (AVP-ir) cells in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BST) and medial amygdaloid nucleus (MAN) and their AVP-ir projections to the lateral septum were studied in monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and promiscuous meadow voles (M. pennsylvanicus). A sexually dimorphic AVP-ir pathway was found in both species; males had more AVP-ir cells in the BST and MAN, as well as denser AVP-ir fibers in the lateral septum, than did females. A significant species difference was also found. Overall, meadow voles had more AVP-ir cells in the BST and MAN than did prairie voles. Male prairie voles, however, had a higher density of AVP-ir fibers in the lateral septum than male meadow voles. The species difference in the sexually dimorphic AVP-ir projections in the BST and MAN is implicated in the rodents' different life strategy and behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Four experiments examined the ability of quokkas (Setonix brachyurus) and fat-tailed dunnarts (Sminthopsis crassicaudata) to solve 2 configural tasks: transverse and negative patterning. Transverse patterning requires the simultaneous solution of 3 overlapping discrimination problems (A+B-, B+C-, C+A-). Both species could solve the nonoverlapping (elemental) version of this task (U+V-, W+X-, Y+Z-), but only dunnarts solved the transverse patterning task. Negative patterning requires conditioned responses to 2 stimuli when presented separately but not together (A+, B+, AB-). Both species formed a selective conditioned response to A+ and B+ stimuli and inhibited responding to a simple nonreinforced stimulus (C-), but only dunnarts successfully inhibited responding to the AB- compound to solve the negative patterning task. These experiments are the first to demonstrate configural learning in a marsupial. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
The hypothesis that social learning is an adaptive specialization for social living predicts that social species should learn better socially than they do individually, but that nonsocial species should not exhibit a similar enhancement of performance under social learning conditions. The authors compared individual and social learning abilities in 2 corvid species: the highly social pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the less social Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana). The birds were tested on 2 different tasks under individual and social learning conditions. Half learned a motor task individually and a discrimination task socially; the other half learned the motor task socially and the discrimination task individually. Pinyon jays learned faster socially than they did individually, but nutcrackers performed equally well under both learning conditions. Results support the hypothesis that social learning is an adaptive specialization for social living in pinyon jays. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Examined social grooming in groups of bonnet and pigtail macaques to test the hypothesis that the physical aspects of grooming (body sites, postures, methods) evolved in more aggressive species of primates to serve social functions (proximity maintenance and tension reduction). Both species used social presents to direct grooming to particular sites, and used grooming to other sites to terminate interactions, thus regulating proximity with grooming to certain body sites. The 2 species differed in method of grooming: Pigtails primarily stroked (method used for tension reduction); bonnets picked or pick-stroked (method used for hygiene). Pigtail but not bonnet grooming method regulates tension reduction. Pigtails groomed in a social context, whereas bonnets groomed in solitary context. The pigtail results are similar to those found with rhesus monkeys. Different aspects of grooming serve different functions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Same–different judgments of familiar objects and animals were investigated in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in a task based on category matches rather than identity matches. 18 categories of familiar animals and objects were each composed of 12 color slides and were presented as pairs of slides. Ss indicated "same" or "different" on a response lever for reinforcement. On Same trials, 2 different views of the same object were presented, typically with differences in perspective, lighting, and background. On Different trials, 2 pictures of different objects were presented. Ss acquired the category discriminations and transferred their response judgments accurately to novel pictures from the categories. Transfer was better to objects with which the monkeys had actually interacted rather than those with which they did not interact. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
The hypothesis that sex differences in maze learning result from sex differences in activity was tested with wild-caught prairie (Microtus ochrogaster) and meadow (M. pennsylvanicus) voles. For 38 voles error production and activity were simultaneously measured in a series of 7 symmetrical mazes. Repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) examined species, sex, maze, and interaction effects for 3 dependent variables: errors, activity, and errors/activity. The pattern of significant effects was very different for the errors and activity ANOVAs, which suggests that differential activity cannot explain differential error rates. In contrast, the pattern of effects was very similar for errors and errors/activity ANOVAs, which suggests that controls for activity do not remove differences in error production. These results fail to support the activity hypothesis. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
In this study, a multivariate analysis of the locomotor activity of adult, breeding male and female meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) was conducted. Overall, male voles made more movements and spent more time in the center of the activity chambers than did female voles. The authors further investigated the effects of brief exposure (3 min) to predator (red fox [Vulpes vulpes]) odor and various control odors (butyric acid, extract of orange) on subsequent activity. Control odors had no effects. Immediately following exposure to the fox odor, male voles exhibited significantly lower levels of activity and decreased center time. No significant changes in any activity variable were observed in the female voles following exposure to fox odor. This study provides evidence for sex differences in both basal activity levels of meadow voles and activity following exposure to a predator odor. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
Collected data on the behavioral patterns displayed by 55 male and 32 female voles in tests of 10 categories of behavior: home cage activity, wheel running, open-field behavior, sexual dimorphism for body mass, copulatory behavior, tonic and dorsal immobility, climbing, swimming, digging, and nest building. These data were used to construct a 13-scale adaptive profile so that Ss' behavior could be easily and meaningfully assessed with respect to the full range of scores observed in other species tested under similar conditions. Their behavioral patterns emerged as a complex, reflecting both their microtine classification and forest-dwelling ecology. It is suggested that the profile provides a quantitative and ecologically meaningful basis for understanding the behavioral tendencies that adapt a species for life in a particular habitat. Also, rodents of this superfamily provide an ideal group for behavioral study because of the large number of native species and their ease of adaptability in the laboratory. (48 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
In this investigation, the authors used habituation techniques to explore similarities and differences in the qualities of individual odors from hamsters. In Experiment 1, male Turkish hamsters (Mesocricetus brandti) treated flank-gland odors of 2 males from 1 litter as similar compared with the odor of a male from another litter, whether the odor donors were familiar or unfamiliar. At the same time, the Turkish hamsters discriminated between the subtle differences in the individual odors of their familiar brothers. In Experiment 2, male Turkish and golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) treated the flank-gland odors of 2 unfamiliar, unrelated conspecifics as similar compared with the flank odor of a heterospecific individual. The results suggest that similarities in individual odors are related to genetic similarity of the odor donors. These similarities could provide a basis for different types of social recognition, including kin and species recognition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
Octopus macropus and Octopus vulgaris have overlapping habitats and are exposed to similar temporal changes. Whereas the former species is described as nocturnal in the field, there are conflicting reports about the activity time of the latter one. To compare activity patterns, the authors tested both species in the laboratory. Octopuses were exposed to a light-dark cycle and held under constant dim light for 7 days each. O. macropus showed nocturnal and light-cued activity. According to casual observations, O. vulgaris started out nocturnal but had switched to mostly diurnal when the experiment began. Individual variation of its activity was found. The different activity patterns of O. macropus and O. vulgaris might reflect their lifestyles, the latter species being more generalist. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Five experiments were conducted with 196 collared and 243 brown male lemmings. When tested in a Y-maze olfactometer, sexually experienced and naive Ss of both species preferred the odor of conspecific females to the odor of females of another species. Both experienced and naive males also preferred the odor of conspecific females to that of conspecific males. Only sexually experienced males showed a significant preference for estrous over nonestrous conspecific females. Sexually experienced males also discriminated between estrous and nonestrous heterospecific females of a familiar (lemming) and unfamiliar (meadow vole, Microtus pennsylvanicus) species. In conjunction with the results of earlier studies of the development of species-specific olfactory preferences, findings suggest that 2 olfactory cues mediate a male's attraction to receptive females: a species-specific odor that is learned during early development and a sexual attractant whose saliency is established as a result of adult experience with a receptive female. (30 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Unlike humans, not all mammals use both of the binaural cues for sound localization. Whether an animal uses these cues can be determined by testing its ability to localize pure tones; specifically, low frequencies are localized using time-difference cues, and high frequencies are localized using intensity-difference cues. We determined the ability to use binaural cues in 2 New World bats, Phyllostomus hastatus, large omnivores, and Carollia perspicillata, small frugivores, by testing their tone-localization ability using a conditioned avoidance procedure. Both species easily localized high-frequency tones, indicating that they could use the interaural intensity-difference cue. However, neither species was able to use the phase-difference cue to localize either low-frequency pure tones or amplitude-modulated tones (which provided an envelope for additional time analysis). We now know of 3 bat species that cannot use binaural time cues and 2 that can. Further exploration of localization in bats may provide insight into the neural analysis of time cues in species that do not hear low frequencies. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号