首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
This study examined the extent to which chronic d-amphetamine administration sensitizes animals to some behavioral and neurochemical effects of foot shock stress. Rats received daily injections of saline for 14 days or d-amphetamine (2 mg/kg 7 days and 4 mg/kg 7 days). After a 7 day drug abstinent period, extracellular dopamine, 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid and homovanillic acid concentrations were measured in the medial prefrontal cortex using in vivo microdialysis in freely moving rats. The behavioral responses to mild foot shock stress were enhanced in the d-amphetamine-pretreated subjects. Concomitant with this behavioral sensitization, d-amphetamine-pretreated subjects showed greater stress-induced increases in extracellular dopamine in the medial prefrontal cortex than in controls. d-Amphetamine (2 mg/kg)-induced stereotyped behavior was also enhanced in the amphetamine-pretreated animals compared to controls; however, d-amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine in the medial prefrontal cortex were not enhanced in the amphetamine-pretreated group. These results suggest that the mesocortical dopaminergic system is involved in cross-sensitization between d-amphetamine and stress, but not in d-amphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization.  相似文献   

2.
The effects of d-amphetamine on the bar-pressing of rats maintained under a variable-interval schedule of water reinforcement were examined as a function of the operant history of the subjects. One group of rats initially received 51 sessions of exposure to a fixed-ratio 20 schedule, while a second group received equivalent exposure to an interresponse-time-greater-than-12-sec schedule. Mean group response rate when stable was over ten times as high under the fixed-ratio schedule as under the interresponse-time-greater-than-12-sec schedule. Response rates of the two groups largely converged across 47 sessions of exposure to a variable-interval 60-second schedule, at which time response rates for both groups appeared stable. Acute administration of d-amphetamine sulfate similary affected mean response rates of both groups: A 0.25 mg/kg dose did not obviously affect rate, while doses of 0.5, 1.0, and 2.0 mg/kg produced dose-dependent rate decreases. These results indicate that the efficacy of operant history as a determinant of drug effects may be limited to circumstances where current contingencies do not exercise powerful and direct control over behavior.  相似文献   

3.
Three experiments were concerned with tolerance to anorexia induced by d-amphetamine. In experiment 1, one group of rats on a 2 hr food deprivation schedule received 2 mg/kg of d-amphetamine 15 min before eating every other day for a month. A second group of rats on a similar schedule received the same dose of d-amphetamine immediately after eating. When compared to a saline-treated control group, the former group showed significant decreases in weight and food intake; tolerance to the amphetamine-induced anorexia began to occur toward the end of the experiment. The latter group showed a significant decrease in food intake on the non-drug days and an overall weight loss when compared to the control group. Experiment 2 demonstrated that tolerance to d-amphetamine-anorexia was related to the duration of drug administration per se. Experiment 3 showed that taste can be a factor in influencing the rate of tolerance to d-amphetamine-induced anorexia. These results indicate that both pharmacological and experiential factors play an important role in determining the rate of tolerance to this action of d-amphetamine.  相似文献   

4.
Amphetamine- and apomorphine-induced changes in the activity of neurons in the caudate-putamen of paralyzed, locally anesthetized rats were recorded in animals pretreated with 2.5 mg/kg d-amphetamine sulphate for 6, 18 or 36 days, or in animals pretreated with saline for 36 consecutive days. In saline-pretreated animals, 2.5 mg/kg d-amphetamine sulphate (IP) produced an initial, brief potentiation of neuronal firing that was followed by a marked depression of neuronal activity lasting for approximately 35 to 110 min after injection. In amphetamine-pretreated animals, this depression of neuronal activity to the same dose of the drug was markedly prolonged, especially in animals given 36 consecutive days of d-amphetamine pretreatment. A similar enhancement occurred in response to 0.25 mg/kg apomorphine (IP) in animals pretreated with amphetamine for 36 days compared to saline-pretreated control animals. These results are discussed in relation to the known behavioral and biochemical effects of acute and long-term amphetamine administration.  相似文献   

5.
Response rate can influence the behavioral effects of many drugs. Reinforcement magnitude may also influence drug effects. Further, reinforcement magnitude can influence rate-dependent effects. For example, in an earlier report, we showed that rate-dependent effects of two antidepressants depended on reinforcement magnitude. The ability of reinforcement magnitude to interact with rate-dependency has not been well characterized. It is not known whether our previous results are specific to antidepressants or generalize to other drug classes. Here, we further examine rate-magnitude interactions by studying effects of two stimulants (d-amphetamine [0.32–5.6 mg/kg] and cocaine [0.32–10 mg/kg]) and two sedatives (chlordiazepoxide [1.78–32 mg/kg] and pentobarbital [1.0–17.8 mg/kg]) in pigeons responding under a 3-component multiple fixed-interval (FI) 300-s schedule maintained by 2-, 4-, or 8-s of food access. We also examine the effects of d-amphetamine [0.32–3.2 mg/kg] and pentobarbital [1.8–10 mg/kg] in rats responding under a similar multiple FI300-s schedule maintained by 2- or 10- food pellet (45 mg) delivery. In pigeons, cocaine and, to a lesser extent, chlordiazepoxide exerted rate-dependent effects that were diminished by increasing durations of food access. The relationship was less apparent for pentobarbital, and not present for d-amphetamine. In rats, rate-dependent effects of pentobarbital and d-amphetamine were not modulated by reinforcement magnitude. In conclusion, some drugs appear to exert rate-dependent effect which are diminished when reinforcement magnitude is relatively high. Subsequent analysis of the rate-dependency data suggest the effects of reinforcement magnitude may be due to a diminution of drug-induced increases in low-rate behavior that occurs early in the fixed-interval. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Relapse prevention in abstinent cocaine addicts remains a major focus of drug addiction therapy. We used a rat model of cocaine addiction that focused on cocaine-seeking behavior elicited interoceptively and by conditioned stimuli. Each of 18 rats could self-administer a maximum of 20 intravenous cocaine injections (1.5 mg/kg) per session per day. To prevent initiation of responding by cocaine itself priming injections were never administered. Although cocaine was available beginning every session the rats displayed a self-imposed period of abstinence followed by a period of rapid consumption. The abstinence period was variable among rats but consistent for individual rats. In experiment 1 we studied the contribution of a CS+ (stimulus light and lever retraction) to the motivation to initiate and maintain a cocaine self-administration episode. We compared the number of responses the rats emitted to receive the first and subsequent injections of the day between a group responding on a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule (n = 6) and a group responding on a second-order (SO) schedule (n = 5) of reinforcement. For all rats the number of responses per injection was raised daily until a rat failed to consume more than four injections. The SO group was able to emit approximately four times as many responses as the FR group to obtain their first and subsequent injections. In experiment 2 (n = 7) responses during extinction were counted with and without the CS+. Responding was greater in the presence of the CS+ than in its absence. The present model demonstrates that the motivation to self-administer cocaine is variable and greatly enhanced by conditioned stimuli.  相似文献   

7.
Albino rats were given extensive training in spaced responding, using a DRL 30 sec schedule of food reinforcement (only lever presses more than 30 sec apart were reinforced). All rats then went 12 days without behavioral testing. During this period half the rats received daily intragastric doses of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) and the rest equal volumes of the THC vehicle. On day 13, some rats received THC 3 hr before behavioral testing while others received only vehicle. The former showed a sharp increase in lever press rate over baseline levels, but the vehicle control rats were unaffected. The rats with 12 prior THC doses were no less affected than those with no previous drug history. Continued testing resulted in recovery of baseline performance within 5 sessions, again with no effect of previous drug history. Similar results were obtained with doses of 4 mg/kg and 16 mg/kg, though the drug's effects were more pronounced at the higher dose. These results demonstrate that performance in the drug state can be a far more important determinant of tolerance than mere exposure to THC. Drug administration was then suspended for 1 week. Rats that had become tolerant to 4 mg/kg THC were then redivided into 3 new groups. One group received daily doses of vehicle and DRL sessions, a second received DRL sessions without vehicle, and 1 group received neither vehicle nor DRL sessions for this week. Subsequent DRL testing after THC administration showed that only the groups receiving DRL sessions in the intervening week lost their previously acquired tolerance. Experience thus appears to play an important role in loss of tolerance to THC as well as in acquisition of tolerance.  相似文献   

8.
Partial reinforcement is known to increase resistance to extinction (Rn) relative to training with continuous reinforcement. This phenomenon, referred to as the partial reinforcement extinction effect, is one of the most robust in learning and conditioning studies. Experiment 1 investigated manipulations known to affect the partial reinforcement extinction effect and determined their possible relevance for drug use patterns. Male rats received intravenous cocaine self-administration training under partial reinforcement (FR-10) training or continuous reinforcement (FR-1) conditions with either a low (0.25 mg/kg infusion) or a high cocaine dose (1.00 mg/kg infusion). Animals were placed on an extinction (recurrent nonreward) schedule for 10 days (1-hr sessions) prior to being tested for cue-induced reinstatement (single 2-hr session). Experiment 2 involved acquisition of cocaine self-administration under FR-1 conditions of short training (15 days) or extended training (30 days) with a low dose (0.25 mg/kg infusion) or a medium dose (0.50 mg/kg infusion) of cocaine reward prior to extinction or reinstatement. Experiment 1 showed that rats trained with FR-10-high dose outcomes exhibited greater Rn than the remaining groups. Additionally, FR-10-high dose and FR-10-low dose rats were more likely to return to active drug seeking during the reinstatement test. In Experiment 2, rats trained under FR-1-medium dose conditions were more persistent during extinction following short acquisition training than comparable rats experiencing extended acquisition training. The reinstatement test was conducted following extinction, in which it was observed that overtraining under FR-1-medium dose reward schedules resulted in a decrease in the tendency to return to active drug seeking. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Researchers examined whether exposure to novel environmental stimuli reduces drug self-administration. Rats were trained to self-administer amphetamine on a fixed ratio (FR) 5 schedule of reinforcement and then were exposed to novel stimuli during the session. Responding was significantly decreased with exposure to novelty but returned to baseline levels on intervening nonexposure sessions. In 2 subsequent experiments, rats were exposed to novel plastic objects prior to the session. Immediately following exposure, rats were allowed to self-administer amphetamine on an FR 1 schedule, which was increased gradually to an FR 5 either using predetermined increments or on the basis of performance criteria. Exposure to the novel objects significantly decreased acquisition of amphetamine self-administration in both situations. Results suggest that exposure to novel environmental stimuli may be effective at reducing drug self-administration. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
We have established a rat model that reflects the course of development of alcohol and opiate addiction. The present study with d-amphetamine aimed to define general principles in the development of an addiction. Male rats had a continuous free choice between d-amphetamine solutions (100, 200 and 400 mg/l) and water for 47 weeks. An initial intake of high doses of d-amphetamine during the first weeks of drug choice was followed by an individually stable pattern of drug consumption of moderate drug doses. During this period of controlled consumption (from week 10 to week 40), the voluntary intake of d-amphetamine depended on individual factors (dominant rats: 0.37+/-0.02 mg/kg per day, subordinate rats: 0.57+/-0.05 mg/kg per day) and environmental variables (group housing: 0.21+/-0.02 mg/kg per day, single housing: 0.41+/-0.03 mg/kg per day). Beginning with week 41, voluntary d-amphetamine consumption progressively increased (1.9+/-0.2 mg/kg per day in week 47), although the experimental conditions remained unchanged. Drug intake during a retest (free choice as before) after 6 months of drug deprivation revealed that the rats had persistently lost their control over drug intake and were no longer able to adjust drug taking to internal and external conditions. These addicted rats took very high drug doses, even when all d-amphetamine solutions but not water were adulterated with bitter tasting quinine (6.6+/-0.6 mg/kg per day; age-matched controls: 0.37+/-0.04 mg/kg per day). Forced intake of d-amphetamine for 47 weeks (7.1+/-0.3 mg/kg per day) via the drinking fluid caused physical dependence (hyperreactivity during withdrawal) but did not lead to drug addiction (voluntary intake in the retest with adulteration: 0.42+/-0.04 mg/kg per day). Both the temporal development and the prerequisites of psychostimulant addiction were in principle the same as for alcohol and opiates.  相似文献   

11.
Rats were trained to self-administer cocaine (0.5 mg/kg/infusion) and were then pretreated with the 5-HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT (0.125, 0.25 or 0.5 mg/kg, SC). 8-OH-DPAT pretreatment produced a decrease in reinforced response rates. When the effect of 8-OH-DPAT (0.5 mg/kg, SC) on responding for a range of cocaine doses was assessed, the drug produced a decrease in response rates when lower doses of cocaine served as the reinforcer. Fluoxetine (10 mg/kg, IV), an indirect 5-HT agonist, also reduced reinforced response rates for a low dose infusion of cocaine. Rates of responding for infusions of higher cocaine doses were not affected by fluoxetine pretreatment during an FR1 schedule of reinforcement. When an FR10 schedule of reinforcement was imposed, reinforced response rates for infusions of higher doses of cocaine were also reduced. Thus, under conditions that produce high rates of responding (low dose infusion or high ratio requirements for an infusion) fluoxetine reduced responding. This effect may be due to the effects at the 5-HT1A receptor, since 8-OH-DPAT produced a similar effect on cocaine self-administration. Given that the effects of these 5-HT agonists are observed only when low doses of cocaine serve as the reinforcer or when task demands are high, it is possible that the suppression of responding reflects an effect that is not specific to the reinforcing impact of cocaine. An alternative explanation for these effects incorporates a concept of unit cost/cocaine infusion that allows for direct comparison across studies that employ different reinforcement schedules.  相似文献   

12.
Intravenous self-administration of GBR 12909, an indirect dopamine agonist, was examined on a Fixed Ratio (FR 1) and a Progressive Ratio (PR) schedule of reinforcement in rats. Subjects were first trained to self-administer cocaine (1.5 mg/kg/inj) during daily 5 h sessions, after which GBR 12909 (0.187-1.5 mg/kg/inj) was substituted. On the FR 1 schedule, the inter-infusion interval for GBR 12909 self-administration was directly related to dose and was approximately three times longer than that established for equivalent doses of cocaine. Breaking points on the PR schedule were comparable for GBR 12909 and cocaine self-administration. The data indicate that, compared to cocaine, GBR 12909 has a longer duration of action and a similar reinforcing efficacy.  相似文献   

13.
Various dosages of d-amphetamine (0.1, 0.5, 2.5 mg/kg) and of cocaine (5.0, 20, 40 mg/kg) were administered i.p. to each of 7 rats trained in an experimentally induced conflict procedure. Sessions were 1 hr in duration and consisted of five 12 min periods; responding was reinforced with food on a F124 sec schedule of reinforcement during each period; however, in periods 2 and 4 each response was followed by the application of footshock. Significant increase in responding did not occur in any period following any of the pretreatments. Cocaine (5.0, 20 mg/kg) and d-amphetamine (0.5, 2.5 mg/kg) significantly decreased responding in both punished and unpunished periods. Following these treatments the rate of responding in punished and unpunished components was not significantly different. This suggest that psychomotor stimulants may not selectively increase anxiety, at least at dosages which are not at the same time anorexic.  相似文献   

14.
The immediate and carry-over effects of scopolamine and d-amphetamine were evaluated in a free running Y-maze spontaneous alternation task. The immediate effect of scopolamine (1.0 mg/kg) or d-amphetamine (5.0 mg/kg) was to reduce alternation to chance or to levels significantly below chance (perseveration), respectively. On a second, non-drug test day alteration decreased in saline treated animals, but increased among mice which received scopolamine on Day 1. In contrast, upon retesting in the non-drug state, the performance of animals initially treated with d-amphetamine resembled that of saline treated mice. Subsequent experiments revealed that these effects could not be attributed to drug effects on peripheral mechanisms, consolidation, residual drug action or drug dissociated learning. It was concluded that the behavioral effects of scopolamine and d-amphetamine are qualitatively different. Whereas scopolamine disrupts habituation, d-amphetamine induces perseveration independently of any effects on habituation.  相似文献   

15.
Chronic administration of high doses of d-amphetamine produced time-limited, surmountable tolerance to stimulus effects of d-amphetamine. 23 male Sprague-Dawley rats discriminated saline and 0.80 mg/kg d-amphetamine under fixed ratio (FR) schedules of food delivery. Suspending training and administering saline did not alter sensitivity to d-amphetamine, indicating that neither tolerance nor sensitization developed during regular training. Acute pretreatment with 3.2 mg/kg d-amphetamine did not alter the ED50 for stimulus effects of d-amphetamine. In contrast, administration of 3.2 or 6.4 mg/kg d-amphetamine, b.i.d., for 3 days or 2 weeks increased the ED50 for stimulus effects 3- to 4-fold but did not produce consistent tolerance to rate-altering effects. Tolerance to stimulus effects was surmountable, as higher doses of d-amphetamine produced full drug-lever selection in tolerant rats. Sensitivity recovered after chronic administration ended. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
The effects of oral d-amphetamine, 5--20 mg were studied in isolated humans who produced speech monologues during experimental sessions. Drug effects were studied under double-blind conditions by making repeated observations within each subject after placebo or active drug. In the first experiment, d-amphetamine 15 mg was studied in 4 isolated subjects who had received instructions that they should talk some of the time during experimental sessions. All subjects spoke more after active drug than after placebo. In the second experiment, d-amphetamine 5--20 mg was studied in 4 subjects who were instructed to talk, but who also earned points under a fixed interval 5 min schedule by speaking (i.e. by closure of a voice operated relay). Point delivery did not generally influence patterns of speech over time. Reliable drug produced increases in amount of talking were observed in 3 of 4 subjects. Adjective checklist self report scores indicating a stimulant drug effect were also sensitive to effects of d-amphetamine. Under controlled laboratory conditions, an increase in speaking is a reliable behavioral effect of d-amphetamine in isolated humans producing speech monologues.  相似文献   

17.
Methylphenidate (MPH) is one of the most widely prescribed drugs for treating attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Previous research suggested that MPH is a reinforcer for rats, but not all of the manipulations to show that lever pressing is controlled by the contingency to obtain MPH have been examined. In Experiment 1, responding for MPH on a progressive ratio (PR) schedule was assessed. Rats self-administered varying doses of MPH (0.056–1.0 mg/kg/infusion) on a PR schedule of reinforcement, and self-administered more MPH than saline, with maximal responding occurring at a unit dose of 0.56 mg/kg/infusion. Experiment 2 examined if there were differences in responding between contingent and noncontingent MPH (0.56 mg/kg/infusion) on a fixed ratio schedule of reinforcement. Results showed that rats responded for contingent MPH, and that responding was not maintained when MPH was delivered noncontingently. Experiment 3 examined self-administration of MPH (0.1 or 0.3 mg/kg/infusion) during long access (6 hr) compared to short access sessions (1 hr). Results showed that rats given long access to MPH showed an escalation of intake across sessions, with this escalation being more pronounced at the lower unit dose (0.1 mg/kg/infusion); in contrast, rats given short access to MPH did not show an increase in MPH self-administration across sessions at either MPH dose tested. Taken together, these results indicate that MPH is an effective intravenous reinforcer for rats and that, similar to other stimulants such as cocaine, amphetamine and methamphetamine, MPH is subject to abuse as reflected by dysregulated intake across repeated long access sessions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
Estrogen increases and progesterone decreases the acquisition and reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in female rats. Here estrogen and progesterone were studied for their effects on the escalation of cocaine self-administration in female rats. The rats received ovariectomy (OVX) or sham (SH) surgery and were treated with estradiol benzoate (0.05 mg/kg sc) and/or progesterone (0.5 mg/kg) or vehicle (indicated by E, P, and V), resulting in 5 groups: SH+V, SH+P, OVX+V, OVX+E, OVX+E+P. Rats self-administered intravenous cocaine (0.4 mg/kg) under a fixed ratio 1 (FR 1) schedule during 2-hr sessions and were then given 6-hr sessions (long access; LgA) (FR 1) for 21 days. After LgA, self-administration was reassessed with 2-hr sessions under the FR 1 and a progressive ratio schedule with 4 cocaine doses. There were no differences among the 5 groups in cocaine self-administration during initial 2-hr sessions. During LgA, the SH+V, OVX+E, and OVX+V groups escalated their cocaine self-administration, whereas the OVX+E+P and SH+P groups did not. Estradiol increased escalation in the OVX+E group compared with the OVX+V group, and progesterone (SH+P) reduced escalation compared with the SH+V group. When estrogen and progesterone were both administered in OVX rats (OVX+E+P), escalation was significantly lower than in the OVX+E group. Cocaine infusions during the 2-hr sessions were significantly higher after escalation than before in all groups except the progesterone-treated groups (SH+P and OVX+E+P). Estrogen promoted and progesterone inhibited escalation of cocaine self-administration, illustrating the importance of female gonadal hormones in drug-seeking behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Adult male rats (Rattus norvegicus) were exposed to a water supply in the home cage containing 100 ppm cadmium chloride and sodium saccharin (.65% wt/vol; cadmium group) or water containing only the saccharin amendment (group control). On Day 65 of exposure, animals from each group received jugular catheter implants and were subsequently trained over the course of 15 daily 2-hr sessions to self-administer a .25 mg/kg/infusion of cocaine HCl under a fixed ratio 1 schedule. Immediately following acquisition training, the full dose-effect function was determined for all animals by using cocaine doses of .03, .06, .125, .25, .50, and 1.0 mg/kg. Cadmium-exposed animals executed more active (cocaine) lever responses during acquisition training but were not different from controls in depressing a pharmacologically inactive lever. For dose-effect testing, cadmium exposed animals exhibited greater self-administration than controls at the higher doses of cocaine, and there was evidence that the cocaine dose that produced maximum responding was higher in cadmium-exposed than control animals. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Here we report that a single administration of naloxone to conscious rats produces no significant increase in oxytocin release, but when repeated 3-4 days later results in a large release of oxytocin. Plasma oxytocin concentrations were measured in conscious and urethane-anaesthetized rats pretreated with naloxone or isotonic saline on Day 1. On Days 2, 3 or 4, a second dose of naloxone was given, producing an increase in oxytocin secretion in naloxone-pretreated groups (P < 0.05 vs. controls) on Day 3 and 4, but not on Day 2. The specificity of the opioid antagonist supersensitivity was determined by injection of the kappa-antagonist nor-binaltorphimine (nor-BNI). Pretreated rats (naloxone, saline or nor-BNI, Day 1) received an additional acute nor-BNI injection (Day 4) which increased plasma oxytocin concentration in the three groups. However, this increase was higher in naloxone-pretreated rats with no differences between the nor-BNI- and saline-pretreated animals. Measurements of electrical activity of single supraoptic nucleus oxytocin neurons and of plasma oxytocin concentration (Day 4) showed that naloxone modestly enhanced the responsiveness of oxytocin neurons to cholecystokinin (CCK) in naloxone-pretreated rats (by comparison with saline-pretreated rats), but had only a small effect on basal firing rate that did not differ between naloxone-pretreated rats and saline-pretreated rats. To investigate whether naloxone-pretreatment modified the effect of morphine on CCK-induced oxytocin release, on Day 4 CCK was injected i.v. with or without morphine. Morphine at a dose of 0.1 mg/kg did not affect CCK-induced oxytocin release, whereas 1 mg/kg of morphine blocked this release in both saline- and naloxone-pretreated rats. The results suggest that naloxone induces opioid antagonist supersensitivity on oxytocin secretion, mainly by up-regulating kappa-opioid mechanisms on oxytocin nerve terminals in the posterior pituitary.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号