首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Describes how a 2?×?2?×?2 factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) affects confidence levels of results. Discussion focuses on uses and limitations of statistical tools such as ANOVAs, as well as the appropriate times to use them. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Discusses multivariate analysis of variance as a general case of familiar multiple regression analysis. A consequence of this approach is a unified treatment of multivariate analysis of variance which can be used by psychologists who are generally familiar with multiple regression approaches to univariate analysis of variance. It is suggested that the generality of the approach permits solutions consistent with any of the several available strategies for dealing with problems of unequal and disproportionate cell frequencies. Inherent in the multiple regression formulation is the otherwise not so obvious fact that univariate analysis of variance results are an integral part of the multivariate solution and that both are important for understanding complex data. Methods of interpreting multivariate analysis of variance results in complex factorial experimental designs are discussed. (32 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Examined the advantage of disadvantages of 5 different exact analyses of variance for nonorthogonal 2-way designs with respect to orthogonality of the analyses, parametric hypotheses tested, and model comparisons made by the analyses. It is proposed that experimenters, when faced with the necessity of performing a 2-way ANOVA, carefully consider these analyses with regard to the a priori information they have about the data, the questions they expect the analysis to help answer, and the questions each analysis is best equipped to answer. It is also suggested that experimenters choose the analysis that best fits their needs rather than depend on one for all situations. (16 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Presents an overview of procedures for calculating power of the F test under 3 models of ANOVA (fixed effects, random effects, and mixed effects). A comparison of power of tests on fixed and random factors shows the latter to have substantially lower power. Consequences for designing experiments and for interpreting experimental results are discussed, and the simplicity with which power calculations are done is emphasized. (15 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Criticizes J. E. Overall and D. K. Spiegel's article (see record 1970-01534-001) discussing 3 methods for performing nonorthogonal analysis of variance (ANOVA). It is observed that the statistics obtained do not provide exact tests for main effects when one is assuming an interaction model. An alternative method is presented for treating nonorthogonal ANOVA which uses an existing general linear model program. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Analyses of variance (ANOVA) with the general linear model (OLM) in many standard statistical packages use an overparameterized model, a model unfamiliar to most behavioral science researchers. Estimates and significance tests with GLM procedures are calculated by computing generalized inverses and estimates of estimable functions. Using simple examples, the authors discuss the concepts that underlie the solutions for 1-way and 2-way ANOVAs with overparameterized models and illustrate how these models allow one to evaluate the research hypotheses. The authors also extend the discussion of overparameterized models to a more general modeling approach than GLM, the general linear mixed model. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Of the many multiple comparisons techniques described by Ryan (see 34: 1416), the procedure, derived from analysis of variance, of partitioning the degrees of freedom attributable to the main effect into n orthogonal components was omitted. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Examines assumptions about the general linear model for interaction terms in the mixed analysis of variance. Some well-known results of S. R. Searle (1971) demonstrate that the inconsistencies between J. H. Dwyer's (see record 1975-02166-001) technique and that of G. M. Vaughn and M. C. Corballis (see record 1969-16617-001) in estimating the magnitude of effect for a mixed interaction are the direct result of specific assumptions made. If it is assumed that the interaction source of variance is a random variable, then the equations obtained by Vaughn and Corballis are correct; however, if an alternative assumption is made (i.e., that the iteraction term is fixed in one direction), then Dwyer's equations are correct. Researchers are called on to be cognizant of these two sets of assumptions and to be aware of the dramatic effects they may have on estimates of magnitude of effect for mixed interactions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Nonorthogonal analysis of variance (ANOVA) is defined as a factorial ANOVA with differing numbers of subjects in the cells. Despite the volume of literature in the psychological journals dealing with the problems of nonorthogonal ANOVA, little attention has been focused on the robustness properties of the corresponding hypothesis tests. Monte Carlo results are presented for the two-way ANOVA design indicating that all of the standard computational routines for the unequal cell size case are nonrobust. This occurs when the assumptions of homogeneity of variance or normality are violated. The user is cautioned against collecting such data. If such data must be analyzed, then alternative or supplemental analysis strategies should be used. Alternative approaches would include simulation, rank transformation, modified ANOVA procedures, and alternative developments in linear models, such as nonparametric factorial ANOVA. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
In J. Overall and D. Spiegel's reply to R. Rawlings's (see record 1972-26084-001) criticism of their previous article, the authors state that Rawlings's alternative nonorthogonal analysis of variance is equivalent to their method, which Rawlings criticized as incorrect. In 2 separate articles (a) Rawlings replies to Overall and Spiegel's present article, and (b) I. Smith contends that there is a statistical error in G. Joe's (see record 1971-25969-001) attempt to clarify the original Overall and Speigel article. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Multivariate analysis versus multiple univariate analyses.   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The argument for preceding multiple analysis of variance ({anovas}) with a multivariate analysis of variance ({manova}) to control for Type I error is challenged. Several situations are discussed in which multiple {anovas} might be conducted without the necessity of a preliminary {manova}. Three reasons for considering multivariate analysis are discussed: to identify outcome variable system constructs, to select variable subsets, and to determine variable relative worth. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Rao's technique of decomposing chi-square into components is modified to derive a distribution-free or nonparametric test of hypotheses involving main effects and interaction examined customarily by the analysis of variance of the two-factor or two-way variety. The proposed nonparametric test of analysis of variance hypotheses is described in terms of six principal steps, illustrated with a computational example, discussed with regard to small expected frequencies, compared with Mood's tests which appear to be disadvantageous in treating interaction effects, and is possible to extend for designs of three or more factors. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Considers that the controversy surrounding dummy variate multiple regression approaches to nonorthogonal analysis of variance would be cleared up if a criterion could be accepted for deciding what constitutes a proper generalization of the classical analysis of variance for orthogonal factorial designs. It is proposed that a general multiple regression solution be interpreted as testing analysis of variance effects only if it results in an estimation of the same parameters and tests of the same hypotheses that might otherwise be estimated and tested in an orthogonal design involving the same factors. A method which satisfies this criterion is identified, and a simple procedure for examining equivalence in orthogonal and nonorthogonal cases is suggested. (19 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Unequal cell frequencies in a factorial design create the problem of nonorthogonality: an intercorrelation among the main and interaction effects. This article presents a nontechnical discussion of the problem introduced by nonorthogonality. Four least-squares approaches to the solution of this problem are presented (the unadjusted main effects, hierarchical, fitting constants, and simultaneous solutions). Each is discussed with reference to its method of dealing with nonorthogonality and the conclusions it permits. Recommendations are provided as to the circumstances under which each solution is appropriate. (French abstract) (14 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Contends that nonorthogonal analysis of variance has been much misunderstood by psychologists, and as a result there has been considerable controversy as to the appropriate methods of analysis. These problems traditionally associated with the nonorthogonal multifactor analysis of variance are rather easily resolved by viewing the analysis of variance (either orthogonal or nonorthogonal) as a series of model comparisons. From this point of view, the analysis of highly confounded designs is seen to yield results that correspond to those that a purely logical analysis would suggest. A logical flow of comparisons and decisions is developed for both the 2- and 3-factor designs that, although more complicated than procedures previously proposed, seems necessary for drawing proper inferences. It is further shown that there is no logical difference between orthogonal and nonorthogonal analysis of variance. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
The power of Wilson's proposed chi-square test for testing n-way analysis of variance designs is very low in comparison with the F test. Several illustrative problems are presented. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Es using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) usually want to analyze effects separately for each response variable after rejecting a null hypothesis of multivariate dispersion. From the standpoint of the multivariate general linear model, 4 measures of importance for response variables are discussed: univariate F statistic for each response, standardized canonical coefficient for each response, contribution to the MANOVA test criterion by each response, and simultaneous confidence intervals on estimates of treatment effects on each response. Artificial data are presented to illustrate problems in using these measures. (17 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
Discusses the biasing effects of nonindependence of observations on the mean squares used to test the effect of some discrete independent variable. Nonindependence of observations is defined, and 3 commonly assumed patterns of nonindependence are identified: nonindependence due to groups, nonindependence due to sequence, and nonindependence due to space. How the bias in both the mean square for treatment and the mean square for error can be derived when each of the 3 patterns of nonindependence is ignored in analyzing the effect of a discrete independent variable is demonstrated. Ways to eliminate the sometimes considerable biases, either by including the source of nonindependence in the analysis, transforming the data to remove it, or modeling it, are discussed. It is concluded that nonindependence of observations should be viewed as a substantive issue central to many areas by psychological research. (28 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Discusses the recommendation by M. R. Leary and E. M. Altmaier (see record 1981-02539-001) that MANOVA should be used with several dependent variables in order to control for overall Type I error rate. This paper calls attention to limitations and dangers in routine application of MANOVA, describes some alternative procedures, and laments the necessarily pervasive character of the problem of statistical error. (20 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Investigated the relationship of new sex role formulations, specifically S. L. Bem's (1974, 1975, 1976) concept of psychological androgyny and the Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI), to personality dimensions. 138 male and 136 female college students filled out the BSRI and the 16 PF. Regression analysis suggested that the BSRI Masculine scale was convergent with personality dimensions characterized as masculine. Results for the BSRI Feminine scale were equivocal. Discriminant analysis suggested that androgynous and masculine-typed Ss shared similar personality dimensions in opposition to feminine and undifferentiated Ss. These results did not fully support Bem's hypothesis of sex roles; however, evidence emerged indicating that masculinity and femininity, as traits, may be qualitatively different phenomena. The implications for trait behavioral measurement are discussed. All results were cross-validated with a 2nd sample of 88 males and 93 females. (21 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号