首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The bidirectional control procedure was used to determine whether pigeons (Columba livia) would imitate a demonstrator that pushed a sliding screen for food. One group of observers saw a trained demonstrator push a sliding screen door with its beak (imitation group), whereas 2 other groups watched the screen move independently (possibly learning how the environment works) with a conspecific either present (affordance learning with social facilitation) or absent (affordance learning alone). A 4th group could not see the screen being pushed (sound and odor control). Imitation was evidenced by the finding that pigeons that saw a demonstrator push the screen made a higher proportion of matching screen pushes than observers in 2 appropriate control conditions. Further, observers that watched a screen move without a demonstrator present made a significantly higher proportion of matching screen pushes than would be expected by chance. Thus, these pigeons were capable of affordance learning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
The present study aimed at showing a spatial compatibility effect (and, by implication, a right–left subdivision of space) solely attributable to the orienting of attention. Five groups of 8 normal Ss were required to give right–left discriminative responses to stimuli presented within one of 6 empty boxes arranged in a horizontal row. Reaction times and errors were recorded. A right–left grouping of the boxes occurred regardless of whether Ss' fixation was kept at the intermediate position (Exp 1) or at one extremity (Exp 2) of the row. In Exps 3 and 4, Ss' attention was not aligned with a fixed position but was moved, through peripheral cues, from trial to trial and positioned between different pairs of adjacent boxes. Results showed that the display was again subdivided into 2 regions and that the reference point for the right–left subdivision was the focus of attention. In Exp 5, eye position was instrumentally monitored, and Ss' attention was directed by central cues. The results confirmed that the focusing of attention leads to a right–left partitioning of space. Directing attention to a position in space brings about a right–left perceptual organization that predominates over that provided by the other egocentric reference axes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
An artificial fruit (AF) was used to test for social learning in pig-tailed macaques (Macaca nemestrina) and adult humans (Homo sapiens). A monkey demonstrator opened the AF, showing alternative methods to 2 groups of cage mates. Video films of the monkey demonstrations were presented to adult humans. Compared with chimpanzees and children, the macaques watched the demonstrations significantly less and in a much more sporadic manner. They also produced only very weak and transitory evidence of social learning. In contrast, the adult humans performed as one might expect of optimum imitators, even producing evidence of components of a "ratchet effect." (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
After separation of symblepharons in ocular pemphigoid, transplantation of nasal or bucal mucosa is recommended to prevent recurrent lid fusion; however, it is not always successful. BACKGROUND: An 86-year-old patient was referred to our eye clinic because of trichiasis in ocular pemphigoid. In the right eye, the only functional eye, fusion of the lower lid and the globe was present. There was severe trichiasis with multiple lesions of the cornea and beginning neovascularization. The left eye showed complete fusion of the lid fissure and neovascularization of the cornea. To prevent the right eye from suffering the fate of the left, we tried to find a simple surgical method that could easily be performed on the multimorbid patient. METHOD: Under parabulbar anesthesia we separated the lower lid from the globe until free passive motility of the lower lid was achieved. A cut-to-size piece of Gore-Tex surgical membrane was prepared and fixated with resorbable u-sutures on the inner lid. The membrane was left in place for 4 weeks until suture lysis. A silicone tube, which was transitorily fixed to the outer lower lid, had an additional ectropionizing effect. Three months later, lower-lid ectropion surgery was performed, combined with tarsectomy to obtain a long-term effect. RESULT: Six months later the lower-lid fornix had stabilized, and the lid and globe had good motility. The corneal lesions had healed. CONCLUSION: The use of 0.1 mm membrane prevents penetration of cell because of the microstructure. Similar to the "bare sclera" technique, which is used in selected cases in strabismus or pterygium surgery, the inner lid and sclera were epithelialized separately from the remaining conjunctiva. The surgical membrane prevented recurrence of the symblepharon. We think the presented technique is an easy, quick method of preventing recurrence of lid fusion after separation of symblepharons.  相似文献   

5.
In the last two decades, it became largely accepted that monkeys show little, if any, copying fidelity. However, some recent studies have begun to challenge this notion. To explore reasons for such contrary findings, we designed a foraging apparatus so that in each of two experiments with capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), a model would demonstrate one of two alternative methods to obtain food. The apparatus had a V-shaped track on which a panel could be slid up left or right from the center to reveal food. In Experiment 1, food was located in a cup directly behind the center panel. In Experiment 2, sliding the panel left or right revealed food either in left or right ends of the V-track. Since this sliding movement led directly to one food location exclusive of the other, we predicted capuchins would show greater copying fidelity in this second Experiment. Instead, subjects were significantly more faithful to the model’s method in Experiment 1, which provided strong evidence of capuchins copying what they had observed. We suggest that the contrasting results of Experiment 1 may have occurred because capuchins prioritize exploratory behavior when alternative foraging locations are accessible. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
A confidence interval (CI) for a population predictor weight for use with N. Cliff's (1994) method of ordinal multiple regression (OMR) is presented. The OMR CI is based on an estimated standard error of a weight derived from a fixed-effects model. A simulation was performed to examine the sampling properties of the OMR CI. The results show that the OMR CI had good Type I error rate and coverage. The OMR CI had lower power than the least-squares multiple regression (LSMR) CI when predictors were not correlated but had higher power when predictor correlations were moderate to high. In addition to discussing the simulation results, it is pointed out that the OMR CI can have superior sampling properties when the fixed-effects assumptions are violated. The OMR CI is recommended when a researcher wants to consider only ordinal information in multivariate prediction, when predictor correlations are moderate to high, and when the assumptions of fixed-effects LSMR are violated. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Sixteen domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) were tested in a familiar context in a series of 1-min trials on how well they obeyed after being told by their owner to lie down. Food was used in 1/3 of all trials, and during the trial the owner engaged in 1 of 5 activities. The dogs behaved differently depending on the owner's attention to them. When being watched by the owner, the dogs stayed lying down most often and/or for the longest time compared with when the owner read a book, watched TV, turned his or her back on them, or left the room. These results indicate that the dogs sensed the attentional state of their owners by judging observable behavioral cues such as eye contact and eye, head, and body orientation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
55 9-mo-olds watched while an object was hidden under a cloth to their left or a cloth to their right. They were then moved to the opposite side of the table and allowed to search. Since no trials training a reach in one direction were included, the child's choice of the incorrect cloth from the new position was interpreted as reliance on a spatial reference system based on the child's body, rather than on repetition of a learned motor habit. Striking environmental effects were found. Infants tested in a landmark-free laboratory and an unfamiliar landmark-filled office behaved egocentrically; those tested in their homes did not. (6 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Recent studies have shown that when an object is hidden in a location A and then in a location B, 8-month-old infants tend to search in A if forced to wait 3 s before retrieving the object, and to search randomly in A or B if forced to wait 6 s before retrieving the object (e.g., Diamond, 1985). A non-search method was devised to examine 8-month-olds' ability to remember the location of a hidden object. The infants saw an object standing on one of two placemats located on either side of the midline. Next, screens were pushed in front of the placemats, hiding the object from view. After 15 s, a hand reached behind one of the screens and reappeared holding the object. The infants looked reliably longer when the hand retrieved the object from behind the "wrong" as opposed to the "right" screen (where the object was actually hidden). This result suggests that the infants (a) remembered the object's location during the 15-s delay and (b) were surprised to see the object retrieved from behind the right (left) screen when they had last seen it on the left (right) placemat. These results indicate that 8-month-old infants' ability to remember the location of a hidden object is far better than their performance in the AB search task suggests. As such, the present results cast serious doubts on accounts that attribute infants' perseverative and/or random search errors to limited memory mechanisms. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
To compare cognitive complexity in wolves and their direct descendents, 4 10-wk-old Eastern timber wolves and 4 10-wk-old malamutes were presented with a series of puzzle boxes that required them to perform increasingly complex manipulations to extract a food dish. Wolves averaged 5.8 successes in 8 trials, and malamutes averaged 1.5 successes. This difference was significant at the .05 level, which supports H. Frank's (see record 1981-32176-001) theoretical model of wolf and dog information processing. The cognitive demands of each task and consistent group differences in problem-solving strategy are discussed in terms of Piaget's stages of human sensorimotor development. (24 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
A single left coronary artery was found in an asymptomatic 21-year-old man who initially had electrocardiographic and vectorcardiographic evidence of anterolateral myocardial infarction. The single left coronary artery, which supplied the distribution of both the left and right coronary arteries, was free of disease at catheterization. There has been no previous association of a normal single left coronary artery and anterior myocardial infarction. Patients with the finding of a single coronary artery should be watched closely, as this may represent a potentially fatal condition.  相似文献   

12.
Subjected 86 3-day-old White Leghorn chicks, reared either socially or in isolation in large or small boxes, to tonic immobility by a ventral restraint induction procedure. Test boxes were identical to rearing boxes or differed only in size. Incidence and duration of immobility were reliably enhanced by social and high-density rearing, but similarity of test and rearing boxes had no effect. A high negative correlation between vocalization rate and duration of eye closure was obtained. Results are discussed in terms of the optimizing induction procedure in relation to early righting behavior. Qualitatively different stages of immobility are defined. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Studied 51 male Long-Evans rats with unilateral ablation of the left or right cerebral neocortex or left or right hippocampus using a battery of tests of spatial orientation, motor coordination, and social behavior, including Morris water task, radial arm maze, feeding, narrow beam traversing, puzzle latches, hoarding, grooming, nest building, running wheel activity, male–male interaction, and shock-induced aggression. Comparison of the brains of operated and control Ss confirmed previous suggestions that the right hemisphere of the rat is bigger and may have different connections than the left hemisphere. Despite the morphological asymmetries, comparison of the behavior of Ss with right and left hemidecortication and right and left hippocampal lesions failed to show a single instance of functional asymmetry in the rat brain. These behavioral results contrast with previous reports of functional asymmetry in the control of activity, orientation, and rotation. (32 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
This study explored 14- and 18-month-old infants' ability to identify the target of an emotional display. In the visual task, infants were presented with 2 boxes. Each box contained an object that could be identified by opening the box lid and looking inside. In the tactile task, the objects had to be pulled out of the boxes before they could be seen. An experimenter expressed happiness as she looked or put her hand inside one box, and disgust as she repeated this action with the other box. Infants were then allowed to explore the boxes. Infants touched both boxes but preferred to search for the happy object. Thus, regardless of age or task, infants identified the target of each emotional display as something inside a box and not the box itself. Infants appeared to use the experimenter's attentional cues (gaze and action) to interpret her emotional signals and behaved as if they understood that she was communicating about the objects.  相似文献   

15.
Readers' eye movements were monitored as they read sentences containing noun-noun compounds that varied in frequency (e.g., elevator mechanic, mountain lion). The left constituent of the compound was either plausible or implausible as a head noun at the point at which it appeared, whereas the compound as a whole was always plausible. When the head noun analysis of the left constituent was implausible, reading times on this word were inflated, beginning with the first fixation. This finding is consistent with previous demonstrations of very rapid effects of plausibility on eye movements. Compound frequency did not modulate the plausibility effect, and all disruption was resolved by the time readers' eyes moved to the next word. These findings suggest (contra Kennison, 2005) that the parser initially analyzes a singular noun as a head instead of a modifier. In addition, the findings confirm that the very rapid effect of plausibility on eye movements is not due to strategic factors, because in the present experiment, unlike in previous demonstrations, this effect appeared in sentences that were globally plausible. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
Trained chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus) to discriminate between two identical boxes containing food on the basis of the positions of the boxes. Once learning was accomplished, the visual characteristics (color or size) of the boxes were changed and chicks were retrained to criterion. Results showed that chicks remembered more about visual characteristics of the boxes than about their position. The same results did not occur, however, if the change was limited only to parts of the boxes rather than to the entire object. Further experiments indicated that it was not the amount of physical change as such that produced these different results but rather the way in which animals select meaningful objects in their perceptual organization of the environment. We argue that chicks define spatial locations in terms of the relative positions of objects and that in so doing they also encode those visual characteristics that make possible perceptual segregation of distinct objects. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
We developed an in vitro motility assay system, in which myosin-coated polystyrene beads were made to slide on actin filament arrays (actin cables) in giant algal cells and subjected to centrifugal forces, which were parallel to the direction of bead movement to serve as external loads on actin-myosin sliding (Oiwa et al. (1990) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 87: 7893-7897), and succeeded in determining the steady-state force-velocity relation of ATP-dependent actin-myosin sliding. To give further information about the properties of actin-myosin sliding, we have applied centrifugal forces, in parallel with the plane of actin-myosin sliding but at right angles with the direction of bead movement, and have found that such "lateral" centrifugal forces reduced the velocity of bead movement. In addition, we have also found that the velocity of bead movement is reduced more markedly with lateral forces applied from the left side of the bead ("left" lateral forces) than those applied from the right side of the bead ("right" lateral forces). These results are discussed in connection with the direction of sliding force generated by the myosin heads on the bead which interact with the right-handed double helix of actin monomers constituting actin filaments.  相似文献   

18.
Coronary arteries anomalies may be part of complex congenital malformations of the heart or be an isolated defect. In our anatomic collection of congenital heart disease, an isolated anomalous origin of coronary arteries was observed in 27 of 1,200 specimens (2.2%): left coronary artery from pulmonary trunk in five, origin from the wrong aortic sinus in 12 (both right and left coronary artery from the right sinus in four and from the left sinus in seven, left coronary artery from the posterior sinus in one), left circumflex branch from right aortic sinus or from very proximal right coronary artery in three, high takeoff of right coronary artery in three, stenosis of the coronary ostia attributable to valvelike ridge in four. In 16 (59%) patients (12 males and 4 females, age ranging from 2 months to 53 years; median, 14), the final outcome was sudden death; it occurred in all cases of left coronary artery origin from right aortic sinus, in 43% of right coronary artery origin from left aortic sinus, and in 40% of the left coronary artery from the pulmonary trunk. Sudden death was precipitated by effort in eight (50%) and was the first manifestation of the disease in eight (50%); previous symptoms consisted of recurrent syncope in four, palpitations in three, and chest pain in one. Five patients who died suddenly during effort were athletes. In conclusion, (1) more than half of our postmortem cases with anomalous origin of coronary arteries died suddenly, (2) all but two patients with sudden death had anomalous coronary artery origin from the aorta itself, (3) the fatal event was frequently precipitated by effort, (4) palpitations, syncope, and ventricular arrhythmias were the only prodromic symptoms and signs. Recognition during life of these coronary anomalies, by the use of noninvasive procedures, is mandatory to prevent the risk of sudden death and to plan surgical correction if clinically indicated.  相似文献   

19.
In order to examine the status of G-proteins in congestive heart failure due to myocardial infarction, the left coronary artery in rats was ligated and animals assessed after 4, 8 and 16 weeks. Sham-operated control and experimental animals were used for the preparation of membranes from the viable (uninfarcted) left and right ventricles. Adenylyl cyclase activities in the presence of pertussis toxin and cholera toxin were increased and decreased in left ventricles from all groups, respectively. On the other hand, adenylyl cyclase activities in 8 and 16-week experimental right ventricles were unaltered in the presence of pertussis toxin and increased in the presence of cholera toxin. Depression of adenylyl cyclase activities in left ventricles from all groups as well as in the right ventricle at 4 weeks were not evident when enzyme activity was determined in the pertussis toxin-treated membranes in the absence or presence of Gpp(NH)p. Cholera toxin-catalyzed ADP ribosylation was decreased in left ventricles from all infarcted groups and increased in the right ventricles at 8 and 16 weeks whereas the pertussis toxin-catalyzed ADP ribosylation was increased in all experimental tissues except in the right ventricles at 8 and 16 weeks. G(s alpha)-protein content was decreased in the left ventricle at 16 weeks and increased in the right ventricles at 8 and 16 weeks of myocardial infarction. On the other hand, G(i alpha)-protein content was increased in left ventricles from all infarcted groups and the 4-week right ventricle but was unaltered in 8 and 16-week right ventricles. An increase in mRNA abundance for G(i alpha)-protein was seen in both left and right ventricles following myocardial infarction. A significant increase in mRNA level for G(s alpha)-protein was observed in all left ventricles and 8-week right ventricle following the coronary occlusion. These results suggest that changes in Gs- and Gi-proteins in the failing heart due to myocardial infarction are chamber-specific and are dependent upon the stage of congestive heart failure.  相似文献   

20.
The hippocampus, objects, and their contexts.   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Rats with hippocampal aspiration lesions and controls were trained on delayed nonmatching to sample with small complex goal boxes, presented trial uniquely. A series of experiments then used pairs of large or small boxes, presented repeatedly. The lesions impaired choice accuracy when the rats were tested with large empty boxes but not when small boxes containing 3-dimensional objects were used. There was a comparable impairment when the rats were tested with pairs of large complex boxes, which contained arrays of objects, identical to those used in the smaller boxes but necessarily spaced further apart. Subsequent experiments revealed that the lesion deficit with large boxes was reduced by insertion of a continuous line of distinctive objects and eliminated by trial-unique presentation of large boxes. The results are discussed in terms of (non) spatial accounts of hippocampal function and the compensatory effects of novel object cues. We conclude that, for hippocampal rats, spatial cues, although useless, can nonetheless be profoundly disruptive. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号