首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
This research was concerned with tracking a remote target moving in depth. A 3-D television system provided visual feedback, and direct and aided pursuit tracking systems were evaluated as a function of target speed. The stereoscopic display was shown to be generally satisfactory for remote-control operations, although some fatigue or "eyestrain" was reported, probably due to the optics of the system. Contrary to previous tracking studies on nondepth courses, it was shown that on a depth course direct tracking is consistently superior to aided tracking at the 3 target speeds used. Amplitude of error analysis provided answers concerning what Ss do when not on target. Tracking behavior was interpreted in terms of the sensory-feedback mechanisms governing the control of motor patterns. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Curvature and tangential velocity of voluntary hand movements are constrained by an empirical relation known as the Two-Thirds Power Law. It has been argued that the law reflects the working of central control mechanisms, but it is not known whether these mechanisms are specific to the hand or shared also by other types of movement. Three experiments tested whether the power law applies to the smooth pursuit movements of the eye, which are controlled by distinct neural motor structures and a peculiar set of muscles. The first experiment showed that smooth pursuit of elliptic targets with various curvature-velocity relationships was most accurate when targets were compatible with the Two-Thirds Power Law. Tracking errors in all other cases reflected the fact that, irrespective of target kinematics, eye movements tended to comply with the law. Using only compatible targets, the second experiment demonstrated that kinematics per se cannot account for the pattern of pursuit errors. The third experiment showed that two-dimensional performance cannot be fully predicted on the basis of the performance observed when the horizontal and vertical components of the targets used in the first condition were tracked separately. We conclude that the Two-Thirds Power Law, in its various manifestations, reflects neural mechanisms common to otherwise distinct control modules.  相似文献   

3.
A stereotelevision system, capable of presenting binocular cues for remote depth perception, has been developed for research on problems of optical design and for sensory feedback studies in space science. Preliminary experiments evaluated a color-separation system, which was found to be faulty for research. Detailed visual acuity and stereoscopic acuity tests with a binocular-separation system disclosed that a very adequate and reliable 3-dimensional system can be devised for laboratory studies of remote binocular vision. A specific experiment tested the utility of a nondirectional auditory cue in aiding visual pursuit tracking in depth. Results indicated that the effectiveness of the auditory cue varies as a function of the speed of the target course. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
When 2 targets for pursuit eye movements move in different directions, the eye velocity follows the vector average (S. G. Lisberger & V. P. Ferrera, 1997). The present study investigates the mechanisms of target selection when observers are instructed to follow a predefined horizontal target and to ignore a moving distractor stimulus. Results show that at 140 ms after distractor onset, horizontal eye velocity is decreased by about 25%. Vertical eye velocity increases or decreases by 1°/s in the direction opposite from the distractor. This deviation varies in size with distractor direction, velocity, and contrast. The effect was present during the initiation and steady-state tracking phase of pursuit but only when the observer had prior information about target motion. Neither vector averaging nor winner-take-all models could predict the response to a moving to-be-ignored distractor during steady-state tracking of a predefined target. The contributions of perceptual mislocalization and spatial attention to the vertical deviation in pursuit are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
The effect of attention on smooth pursuit and saccadic tracking was studied in infants at 8, 14, 20, and 26 weeks of age. A small rectangle was presented moving in a sinusoidal pattern in either the horizontal or vertical direction. Attention level was distinguished with a recording of heart rate. There was an increase across age in overall tracking, the gain of the smooth pursuit eye movements, and an increase in the amplitude of compensatory saccades at faster tracking speeds. One age change was an increase in the preservation of smooth pursuit tracking ability as stimulus speed increased. A second change was the increasing tendency during attentive tracking to shift from smooth pursuit to saccadic tracking when the stimulus speed increased to the highest velocities. This study shows that the development of smooth pursuit and targeted saccadic eye movements is closely related to the development of sustained attention in this age range. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
The asynchrony of bimanual movements was investigated. Right- and left-handers traced simple geometrical patterns (ellipses) continuously with both hands. All combinations of the direction of rotation in each hand were executed at different rhythms. Geometrically, performances were largely independent of manual dominance. However, by comparing the passage times at homologous positions, the authors found that the dominant hand led the nondominant one by about 25 msec. The asynchrony was affected by neither movement type nor rhythm. The variability of the asynchrony varied along the trajectory, with well-defined maxima and minima. The variability profiles for movements that engaged homologous muscles differed markedly from those that engaged nonhomologous muscles. The authors discuss the hypotheses that bimanual periodic movements are timed by a lateralized functional module and that asynchrony is due to the necessity of transmitting time-keeping information to the other hemisphere. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
When observers tracked moving stripes across a background either of stationary stripes, or of stripes moving in the opposite direction, they saw a clear motion aftereffect when the stripes stopped moving. The direction of this aftereffect was opposite to that of the previously tracked stripes, and was thus the same as the direction of the retinal movement of the non-tracked stripes. This aftereffect of tracking was shown not to depend upon slippage of the tracked contours on the retina during tracking, or upon the saccadic phase of optokinetic nystagmus. The effect showed storage over a period of time with the eyes shut. It appears that the effect is due to induced movement, and arises originally from stimulation of the retina by background contours in the tracking phase. This was shown by confining the view of the moving target to one eye, while permitting both eyes to be exposed to background stimulation during tracking. After such stimulation the magnitude of the aftereffect was equal in the two eyes.  相似文献   

8.
This paper reviews the literature on the effects of luminance and chromatic contrast on reading and object recognition in low vision. The general consensus is that whereas luminance contrast can enhance reading and object recognition for some low vision subjects, chromatic contrast does not aid low vision reading but may be useful in low vision object recognition. Although providing useful theoretical results, the experimental designs have so far been remote from the conditions that low vision patients may experience in the home and work environment. Suggestions are made for an 'ideal' experimental design and details of current research, using conditions more likely to be experienced by low vision subjects in everyday life, are given. This research makes use of a recently developed instrument, the Intuitive Colorimeter.  相似文献   

9.
Antisaccades and smooth pursuit eye tracking and schizotypy   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Although severe combined immunodeficient (SCID) mice are considered useful as an animal model for human hematopoietic diseases, the complete reconstruction of human hematopoietic cells can not be established even in these mice. This appears to be because human cytokines, adhesion molecules and extracellular matrices which support differentiation and growth of human hematopoietic cells differ from those in animals. To improve this animal model, we attempted to produce transgenic (Tg) mice producing human interleukin 3 (hIL-3) and human granulocyte macrophage colony stimulating factor (hGM-CSF) with the homozygote of the scid gene. We established two Tg mouse lines, one releasing both 0.5-1 ng/ml of hIL-3 and 0.05-0.2 ng/ml of hGM-CSF in their sera and another releasing only high (2-10 ng/ml) levels of hGM-CSF. When human cytokine-dependent myeloid cell line, TF-1, was subcutaneously transplanted into these two Tg-SCID mouse lines, TF-1 could be successfully engrafted and grew in all lines of Tg-SCID mice but not in control mice. We also observed that TF-1 grows in GM-CSF Tg-SCID mice in a dose dependent manner in vivo and IL-3 shows an additive effect on its growth. These results indicated that these Tg-SCID mice were an useful in vivo model for investigating human leukemogenesis, especially the role of IL-3 and GM-CSF in leukemogenesis.  相似文献   

10.
White (1976) reported that presentation of a masking stimulus during a pursuit eye movement interfered with the perception of a target stimulus that shared the same spatial, rather than retinal, coordinates as the mask. This finding has been interpreted as evidence for the existence of spatiotopic visual persistence. We doubted White's results because they implied a high degree of position constancy during pursuit eye movements, contrary to previous research, and because White did not monitor Ss' eye position during pursuit; if White's Ss did not make continuous pursuit eye movements, it might appear that masking was spatial when in fact it was retinal. We attempted to replicate White's results and found that when eye position was monitored to ensure that subjects made continuous pursuit movements, masking was retinal rather than spatial. Ss' phenomenal impressions also indicated that retinal, rather than spatial, factors underlay performance in this task. The implications of these and other results regarding the existence of spatiotopic visual persistence are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
In previous studies, smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) have been shown to be disordered in about 70% of schizophrenics and about 45% of their first-degree relatives. In this report, the role of attention in these eye movements is addressed in three experiments (using as subjects schizophrenics, their first-degree relatives, and normals administered chloral hydrate) that recruit focused attention to the task. These studies show that voluntary attention in the form of inattention, "heedless negligence," or failure to cooperate, is not the specific attentional quality that is disordered in SPEM of schizophrenics and their relatives. Rather, the data both indicate that nonvoluntary attending is specifically disordered in these persons, and implicate a neurophysiological substrate that can be described as a failure of inhibitory, synchronizing integrating systems which may be located in the brain stem.  相似文献   

12.
PURPOSE: The authors evaluated the reliability of the coefficients of the (1) amplitude/duration and (2) amplitude/peak velocity relationships of the mean precision values and the mean latency values (saccadic eye movements) and the coefficients of the target velocity/gain relationship (smooth pursuit eye movements). They computed test-retest maximum variability limits for these parameters. METHODS: After a 1-week interval, saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements were recorded twice from 20 healthy subjects; 12 of these subjects underwent a third recording session. The estimate of the intraclass coefficient of reliability, R, was adopted to evaluate the reliability of eye movement quantitative analysis. RESULTS: The data demonstrated that the reliability was fairly good for the amplitude/peak velocity relationship, was good for the precision, and was excellent for the amplitude/duration, the target velocity/gain relationships, and the latency. CONCLUSIONS: Quantitative analysis of both saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements is reliable. One statistic used to estimate reliability, ie, the within-subjects mean square value, also enables the determination of test-retest normal variability values for both the variances and the differences of measurements.  相似文献   

13.
The relative effectiveness of a pursuit and a compensatory tracking display in a simulated one-coordinate aircraft control loop was compared with two experiments. It is concluded that the superiority of the pursuit mode over the compensatory gives clear evidence that the separate display of target-course input, control-system output, and error is essentially as beneficial in a "loose" control arrangement as in a "tight" but unaided tracking system employing position control. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Step-ramp target motion evokes a characteristic sequence of presaccadic smooth eye movement in the direction of the target ramp, catch-up targets to bring eye position close to the position of the moving target, and postsaccadic eye velocities that nearly match target velocity. I have analyzed this sequence of eye movements in monkeys to reveal a strong postsaccadic enhancement of pursuit eye velocity and to document the conditions that lead to that enhancement. Smooth eye velocity was measured in the last 10 ms before and the first 10 ms after the first saccade evoked by step-ramp target motion. Plots of eye velocity as a function of time after the onset of the target ramp revealed that eye velocity at a given time was much higher if measured after versus before the saccade. Postsaccadic enhancement of pursuit was recorded consistently when the target stepped 3 degrees eccentric on the horizontal axis and moved upward, downward, or away from the position of fixation. To determine whether postsaccadic enhancement of pursuit was invoked by smear of the visual scene during a saccade, I recorded the effect of simulated saccades on the presaccadic eye velocity for step-ramp target motion. The 3 degrees simulated saccade, which consisted of motion of a textured background at 150 degrees/s for 20 ms, failed to cause any enhancement of presaccadic eye velocity. By using a strategically selected set of oblique target steps with horizontal ramp target motion, I found clear enhancement for saccades in all directions, even those that were orthogonal to target motion. When the size of the target step was varied by up to 15 degrees along the horizontal meridian, postsaccadic eye velocity did not depend strongly either on the initial target position or on whether the target moved toward or away from the position of fixation. In contrast, earlier studies and data in this paper show that presaccadic eye velocity is much stronger when the target is close to the center of the visual field and when the target moves toward versus away from the position of fixation. I suggest that postsaccadic enhancement of pursuit reflects activation, by saccades, of a switch that regulates the strength of transmission through the visual-motor pathways for pursuit. Targets can cause strong visual motion signals but still evoke low presaccadic eye velocities if they are ineffective at activating the pursuit system.  相似文献   

15.
16.
BACKGROUND: Smooth pursuit eye movement (SPEM) abnormalities are a putative marker of genetic risk for schizophrenia. Accurate SPEM performance requires the subject to activate neural systems responsible for smooth pursuit tracking, while simultaneously suppressing activity of neurons responsible for saccadic movements that would move the eye ahead of the target. This study examined whether specific aspects of SPEM dysfunction cosegregate with genetic risk in parents of schizophrenic probands. METHODS: Eighteen probands and their parents had SPEM recorded. Parents with an ancestral history of schizophrenia were hypothesized to be more likely than their spouses without such a history to carry a genetic risk for schizophrenia. RESULTS: Ten families had a single parent with a positive ancestral history for schizophrenia. The frequency of anticipatory saccades, which were mostly small, and the fraction of total eye movement that they represented were the only measures that differentiated the more likely genetic carrier parents in these families from their spouses and age-matched normals. CONCLUSIONS: Failure to suppress saccadic anticipation of target motion during smooth pursuit appears an aspect of SPEM dysfunction related to presumed genetic risk for schizophrenia.  相似文献   

17.
When viewing a moving object, details may appear blurred if the object's motion is not compensated for by the eyes. Smooth pursuit is a voluntary eye movement that is used to stabilize a moving object. Most studies of smooth pursuit have used small, foveal targets as stimuli (e.g. Lisberger SG and Westbrook LE. J Neurosci 1985;5:1662-1673.). However, in the laboratory, smooth pursuit is poorer when a small object is tracked across a background, presumably due to a conflict between the primitive optokinetic reflex and smooth pursuit. Functionally, this could occur if the motion signal arising from the target and its surroundings were averaged, resulting in a smaller net motion signal. We asked if the smooth pursuit system could spatially summate coherent motion, i.e. if its response would improve when motion in the peripheral retina was in the same direction as motion in the fovea. Observers tracked random-dot cinematograms (RDC) which were devoid of consistent position cues to isolate the motion response. Either the height or the density of the display was systematically varied. Eye speed at the end of the open-loop period was greater for cinematograms than for a single spot. In addition, eye acceleration increased and latency decreased as the size of the aperture increased. Changes in the density produced similar but smaller effects on both acceleration and latency. The improved pursuit for larger motion stimuli suggests that neuronal mechanisms subserving smooth pursuit spatially average motion information to obtain a stronger motion signal.  相似文献   

18.
How can radar operators be helped to do a better job in finding and holding an object seen on a radar picture tube? Ss were 40 persons divided into 4 equal groups. The experiment was divided into 2 parts: finding the object, and holding onto it after it was acquired. The 3 "dimensions" were range, elevation, and azimuth. The movements pertained to the hand control lever on top of which was a little thumbwheel; both are used to control lines and circles on the screen. 2 low frequency generators were used to create the dynamic target and the signals responding to the lever and thumbwheel. When both produced compatible (movements in the same direction) display-control systems during the time when the operator was zeroing in on the target, this worked best. In the second part, an opposite type of relative movement worked best. From Psyc Abstracts 36:04:4LK14M. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
The characteristics of the torsional eye movements during head tilting were investigated. Different conditions were used in the experiments. The subjects were in sitting and lying positions in the light and in the dark. A new method of recording with the suction device of Yarbus and the simple optical system allowed to have records without some redundant components of movement. This made record processing less complex and more accurate. The torsional drift in the slow phase of the eye movement was found present under any conditions. When being recorded simultaneously both eyes in man were in identical and synchronous movement.  相似文献   

20.
A noninvasive system designed for patient tracking during image-guided intranasal sinus surgery is described. It is based on optical digitizing with a custom-made registration and reference system, locatable surgical instruments, and a self-localizing operating microscope. Experimental and clinical results reveal a high degree of accuracy for the system. A mean spatial error of 0.82 +/- 0.31 mm was determined for repositioning of the reference system in a plastic model of the skull. For the positioning of the microscope, a mean error of 2.3 +/- 0.83 mm was calculated. Measurements of repositioning accuracy in 24 patients who received surgery for various sinus diseases had a mean spatial error of 1.56 +/- 0.76 mm. The 95% error interval for locating intranasal structures using the surgical instrument was 2.05 mm, and it was 4.92 mm using the microscope. These results suggest that the use of our noninvasive registration and reference system may be effective, accurate, and useful for noninvasive tracking of patient movements in computer-assisted intranasal surgery.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号