首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 281 毫秒
1.
Exp I examined the tendency of 48 Sprague-Dawley rat pups to approach auditory stimulation. Tests were conducted in a circular maze with a central start area and 8 response areas at the periphery. Stimuli were played back over a loudspeaker behind 1 of 8 response areas. Signals included species calls at high and low intensities. Ss showed a tendency to approach the low-intensity playback of a species "social" vocalization but did not approach the other stimuli. Exp II investigated the importance of binaural cues for localization. 24 pups were tested with 1 or both ears blocked or neither ear blocked. Only the binaurally blocked and normal controls approached the sound source at above chance levels. The failure of Ss in the monaural group to approach the sound probably resulted from a disruption of binaural cues. The ability of Ss to localize sounds and process binaural cues is discussed in relation to the onset of hearing and in terms of physiological responses that are present early in ontogeny. (76 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Assessed the ability of horses to use binaural time and intensity difference cues to localize sound in free-field localization test using pure tones. Ss were required to discriminate the locus of a single tone pip ranging in frequency from 250 Hz to 25 kHz emitted by loudspeakers located 30° to the left and right of the Ss' midline. Three Ss were tested with a 2-choice procedure; 2 additional Ss were tested with a conditioned avoidance procedure. Ss were able to localize 250 Hz, 500 Hz, and 1 kHz but not 2 kHz and above. Because the frequency of ambiguity for the binaural phase-difference cue was calculated to be 1.5 kHz, results indicate that Ss could use binaural time differences but not binaural intensity differences. This finding was supported by an unconditioned orientation test involving 4 additional Ss, who correctly oriented to a 500-Hz tone pip but not to an 8-kHz tone pip. Analysis of the superior olivary complex, the brain-stem nucleus at which binaural interactions first take place, reveals that the lateral superior olive (LSO) is relatively small in the horse and lacks the laminar arrangement of bipolar cells characteristic of the LSO of most mammals that can use binaural intensity differences. (36 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Tested 30 newborn guinea pigs to determine their ability to approach an auditory stimulus early in development. Observations of the behavior of 1–4 day old Ss in a circular 8-choice maze revealed a pronounced tendency to orient toward and approach a tape-recorded signal of guinea pig vocalizations. The occurrence of approach responses was reduced to chance in Ss tested with one ear occluded by wax ear plugs which attenuated but did not totally eliminate sound. The effect of monaural ear blocks was more severe than binaural blocks, which reflects the importance of binaural cues in the maintenance of approach responses to sound. In a 2nd study with 40 Ss the ability of older animals, 11–31 days of age, was examined. Directional approach responses to sound were also evident at this age, and ear plugs disrupted performance only under monaural conditions. Furthermore, in Ss raised from birth with monaural ear blocks but tested without ear plugs, there was a subsequent disruption of performance for at least 21 days. Results indicate the importance of binaural cues in the development of early auditory spatial reponses and suggest the need for appropriate binaural experience for subsequent localization of sounds. (52 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Developmental exposure of Long-Evans rats to 0, 1, 4, or 8 mg/kg/day Aroclor 1254 (A1254) from Gestational Day 6 through Postnatal Day 21 produces an elevated behavioral threshold for a 1-kHz tone. Brainstem auditory evoked responses (BAERs) were assessed in a subset of these animals (about 1 year old) using filtered clicks at 1 (65 and 80 dB SPL), 4 (60 and 80 dB SPL), 16 (40 and 80 dB SPL), and 32 (40 and 80 dB SPL) kHz. Aroclor 1254 decreased BAER amplitudes at 1 and 4 kHz, but not at 16 or 32 kHz. A dose-related decrease in the baseline-to-peak P1A amplitude was observed for the 1-kHz (80-dB) stimulus. Doses of 1, 4, or 8 mg/kg/day A1254 decreased the peak-to-peak amplitude of both P1AN1 and P1BN1 for a 1-kHz (80-dB) stimulus. Doses of 4 and 8 mg/kg/day A1254 decreased the peak-to-peak amplitude of N1P2 and P2N2 for a 4-kHz (60-dB) or 1-kHz (80-dB) stimulus. At 8 mg/kg/day, A1254 also increased the latency of peak P4 at 1 kHz (65 dB). The decreases in peak P1A amplitudes are consistent with a dysfunction of the cochlea and/or auditory nerve. Together, the data confirm that developmental exposure of rats to A1254 produces a permanent low- to mid-frequency auditory dysfunction and suggest a cochlear and/or auditory nerve site of action.  相似文献   

5.
Infant mice (Mus musculus) born to dams housed in isolation throughout pregnancy (IsoPreg) begin differentially approaching homenest bedding over clean bedding on Postnatal Day 6. Offspring of dams housed with 2 other potentially pregnant conspecifics (SocPreg) display such homing behavior on Day 4. Earlier onset of homing reflects facilitated olfactory responsiveness in SocPreg pups, rather than qualitative or quantitative differences in IsoPreg versus SocPreg nest odors, body growth, or motoric capabilities. Exposing pregnant IsoPreg dams to SocPreg bedding also accelerated homing onset in the offspring, though not to the same extent as the full social context. Thus, it appears that the facilitation of homing is mediated through the pregnant dam by a combination of chemical cues and other social stimuli. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Eyeblink conditioned response (CR) timing was assessed in adult and infant rats. In Experiment 1, adult rats were trained with a 150-ms tone conditioned stimulus (CS) paired with a periorbital shock unconditioned stimulus (US; presented at 200- or 500-ms interstimulus intervals [ISIs]). The rats acquired CRs with 2 distinct peaks that occurred just before the US onset times. Experiments 2 and 3 examined developmental changes in CR timing in pups trained on Postnatal Days 24-26 or 32-34. Experiment 3 used a delay conditioning procedure in which the tone CS continued throughout the ISIs. Pups of both ages exhibited robust conditioning. However, there were age-related increases in the percentage of double-peaked CRs and in CR timing precision. Ontogenetic changes in eyeblink CR timing may be related to developmental changes in cerebellar cortical or hippocampal function. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Long-Evans rat pups, 17–18 or 24 days of age, were trained with an eyeblink conditioning (EBC) procedure that was used previously with adult rats (R. W. Skelton, 1988). Pups received 3 sessions of delay conditioning in a single day at about 4-hr intervals (100 trials/session). Trials involved pairings of an auditory CS (2.8-kHz, 82-db tone) and a periocular-shock unconditioned stimulus (UCS; 100 msec, 2mA), which were presented 280 msec apart. EBC was observed at both ages, but older pups learned much more rapidly. Subsequent experiments established that this effect is associative (Exp 2), that age differences in EBC cannot be attributed to differences in ability to respond or in sensitivity to the UCS (Exp 3), and that EBC rate can be modulated by motivational state (Exp 4). This preparation may help elucidate the relation between neural development and the ontogeny of learning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Mother rats (Rattus norvegicus; 6 to 8 days postpartum) approach and maintain proximal orientation to a pup that is emitting ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) far more than do virgin females (W. J. Farrell & J. R. Alberts, 2002). We used a playback regimen to examine the roles of acoustic and nonacoustic cues in regulating maternal proximal orientation toward vocalizing pups. When presented with recorded USVs, mothers of 6- to 8-day-old pups and nulliparous virgin females exhibited equivalent levels of proximal orientation toward the playback speaker. Mothers did show enhanced proximal orientation toward recorded USVs, however, if a silent pup was positioned below the speaker. Pup odors appear to be crucial for the maternal response to vocalizing pups, as peripherally induced anosmia attenuated maternal proximal orientation toward a vocalizing pup. Furthermore, spatial contiguity between olfactory and auditory stimuli was required for a maximal maternal response. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Earlier findings, based on limited behavioral observations, indicate that nursing behavior in rats declines dramatically in duration over time postpartum--despite increasing ingestion of milk by rat pups to meet their growth and metabolic needs--although hungry pups elicit more nursing than do well-nourished pups. The authors compared the nursing pattern in detail for 6 hr on Days 7 and 14 and induced hunger in pups acutely with mammary-duct-ligated dams unable to provide milk. Compared with Day 7, on Day 14, supine nursing and the interval between nursing bouts increased, whereas hovering over pups and kyphotic nursing decreased. When pups were increasingly hungry, these age-related changes were counteracted. Thus, the ingestive motivation of pups largely regulates the nursing pattern over time. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Rats given medial frontal lesions on Postnatal Day 1 or Day 10 were trained on the Morris water task on Days 19–21 or Days 56–58. The operated groups were equally impaired at the water task on Days 19–21, but the Day 10 rats had recovered by 56 days. Dendritic arborization and spine density were analyzed in parietal layer II–III pyramidal cells. At Day 60, but not at Day 22, the Day 10 animals had more dendritic spines per unit dendritic length than did the controls or Day 1 rats. Thus, there was functional recovery rather than sparing after frontal lesions at 10 days, and the recovery was correlated with an increase in dendritic spines. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Analyzed the nursing behavior in the precocial guinea pig. The behaviors of 6 dams with litters of 3 pups were videotaped under undisturbed conditions during both phases of the light–dark cycle and after reunion during the light phase on Days 1, 2, 3, 6, and 13 postpartum. More time was spent nursing in the light than in the dark and in the 1st wk than on Day 13. Pup licking was not required for nipple attachment to occur even on Day 1. Dams displayed a crouched nursing posture accompanied by 120 sec or more of body plus head immobility, similar to the crouched stance in the rat; body plus head immobility was rarely seen in females not in a crouched stance. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Pretraining lesions of rat perirhinal (PR) cortex impair fear conditioning to ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) but have no effect on conditioning to continuous tones. This study attempted to deconstruct USVs into simpler stimulus features that cause fear conditioning to be PR-dependent. Rats were conditioned to one of three cues: a multicall 19-kHz USV, a 19-kHz discontinuous tone, and a 19-kHz continuous tone. The discontinuous tone duplicated the on/off pattern of the individual calls in the USV, but it lacked the characteristic frequency modulations. Well-localized neurotoxic PR lesions impaired conditioning to the USV, the discontinuous tone, and the training context. However, PR lesions had no effect on conditioning to the continuous tone. The authors suggest that the lesion effects on fear conditioning to both cues and contexts reflect the essential role of PR in binding stimulus elements together into unitary representations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Hamsters were trained to go left and right to sounds on their left and right sides, respectively. Silent trials were occasionally given in which no sound was presented. Hamsters exposed to a loud 2- or 10-kHz tone in 1 ear often shifted their responding on the silent trials to the side of the exposed ear, suggesting that they perceived a sound in that ear (i.e., tinnitus). The degree of tinnitus was related to the degree of the accompanying hearing loss (estimated by the auditory brainstem response). However, a conductive hearing loss (plugging 1 ear) did not cause a hamster to test positive for tinnitus. Tinnitus could be demonstrated within minutes following tone exposure, indicating an immediate onset, as occurs in humans. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
A comparison of the ability of mammals to localize sound revealed that among the animals examined to date, none of the rodents have been able to localize as accurately as the carnivores. Because all of these rodents are prey animals, the question arises as to whether their poor localization acuity is a phyletic trait of Rodentia or whether it is a trait common to prey species that may be under less selective pressure than predators to localize sound accurately. To answer this question, sound localization acuity was determined in a species that is both predatory and a rodent, the northern grasshopper mouse. Localization thresholds for a single 100-ms noise burst were determined for three grasshopper mice using a conditioned avoidance procedure. Their 50% discrimination threshold of 19° is larger than that of any of the previously tested carnivores and well within the range of other rodents. However, calculations of the binaural sound localization cues available to rodents (based on their head size) suggest that the grasshopper mouse may make more efficient use of the available locus cues than other rodents. Thus, although the grasshopper mouse cannot localize as accurately as carnivores, it appears to be more accurate than predicted for a nonpredatory rodent of its size. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
A cross-sectional design was used to study the development of acoustic startle behavior in C57BL/6J mice from the approximate onset of hearing (12 days) to 17 days of age. Startle incidence and latency were recorded in response to 5-, 7-, 10-, 15-, and 20-kHz tones each presented at 80, 90, and 100 dB (SPL). From 12 to 17 days of age, higher frequency and lower intensity tones became increasingly effective in eliciting the acoustic startle response. In addition, startle latency decreased substantially, and response incidence became more sensitive to changes in tone intensity and tone frequency. This rapid ontogeny of the acoustic startle response closely parallels previously demonstrated neurophysiological development of the mouse pup auditory system.  相似文献   

16.
Eyeblink conditioning depends critically on an identified brainstem-cerebellar circuit and is modulated under some circumstances by the hippocampus, amygdala, and other forebrain regions. Developmental studies of eyeblink conditioning could help elucidate questions concerning the behavioral expression of plasticity within these brain circuits and regions, and of their functional interactions, as they unfold during ontogeny. Recently, this laboratory has shown that conditioning of the eyeblink reflex develops dramatically between Postnatal Days (PND) 17 and PND 24 in the rat. The present study asked whether the developmental emergence of the eyeblink conditioned response (CR) occurs gradually or abruptly over this age range, and whether it reflects developmental changes in acquisition or expression of the learned eyeblink reflex. In Experiment 1, rat pups received two consecutive days of training beginning on PND 17, 20, or 24. Conditioned responses occurred at low levels on PND 17-18, intermediate levels on PND 20-21, and high levels on PND 24-25. In Experiment 2, 17-day-old rats received 2 days of training, 72 h apart, so that effects of training on PND 17 could be examined at an age, PND 20, when expression of the eyeblink CR was clearly possible. On PND 20, rat pups that had received paired training on PND 17 showed significantly faster conditioning than controls that had received unpaired training or no training on PND 17. These findings suggest that neural plasticity underlying associative learning developmentally precedes its overt expression in behavior. Hypotheses concerning the nature and locus of this learning are discussed.  相似文献   

17.
18.
This study examined the temporal relationship between aminoglycoside ototoxicity and the onset of auditory function in the rat. A single dose of gentamicin sulfate (200 mg/kg) and furosemide (100 mg/kg) was administered on postnatal day 6 (P6), P7, P8, P9, or P10, just before the onset of auditory function. Ototoxicity was assessed by the elevation of auditory brain stem response (ABR) thresholds, recorded once the rats had matured. The ABRs were evoked by acoustic clicks and tone pips. The thresholds of control and P6- and P7-treated animals did not differ significantly from each other. Thresholds of some P8- and all P9-treated animals were elevated. The P10-treated animals were deafened, according to these ABR criteria. These data suggest that the potential for aminoglycoside ototoxicity develops rapidly between P8 and P10, just before the onset of auditory function.  相似文献   

19.
We investigated sensory and behavioral responsiveness of the rat fetus. On Days 19, 20, or 21 of gestation, rat fetuses received intraoral infusions of a biologically important stimulus, milk, or a novel chemical stimulus, lemon. Using a technique to directly observe behavior in utero, we found that rat fetuses discriminate between intraoral infusions of milk and lemon, exhibiting different levels and patterns of overall activity after infusion. Milk was found to evoke a low magnitude, delayed increase in overall fetal activity from Day 19 through Day 21, whereas lemon evoked a high-magnitude, spiked pattern of activity that diminished from Day 19 to Day 21. Late in gestation these two stimuli elicited species-typical action patterns. Milk infusions elicited a stretch response much like the one shown by pups at the nipple; lemon infusions elicited face wiping typical of older pups and adults exposed to aversive gustatory stimuli. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Extracellular recordings were made with microelectrodes from single neurons in the rat's dorsal nucleus of the lateral lemniscus (DNLL) and response characteristics were determined for monaural and binaural acoustic stimulation. The vast majority of DNLL neurons were narrowly tuned to sound frequency and their temporal responses to contralateral tone pulses fell into one of three broad categories: onset (57%), sustained (21%) or onset-pause-sustained (22%). Most DNLL neurons fired multiple action potentials to a single click delivered to the contralateral ear. The majority (77%) of DNLL neurons showed a monotonic increase in the number of spikes elicited by contralateral tone pulses of increasing sound pressure level; the remaining cells were weakly non-monotonic. No obvious tonotopic pattern was found in the distribution of characteristic frequency of neurons in DNLL. Most DNLL neurons exhibited either excitatory/inhibitory (74%) or excitatory/excitatory (9%) binaural response patterns. The remaining cells (17%) were monaural and driven exclusively by stimulation of the contralateral ear. The binaural neurons in DNLL were sensitive to both interaural intensity and interaural time differences as determined by presentation of dichotic tone bursts and clicks respectively. The responses of DNLL neurons could be distinguished on the basis of monaural and binaural response characteristics from those in surrounding areas including the sagulum, paralemniscal zone and the intermediate nucleus of the lateral lemniscus.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号