首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Previous studies show that if acoustic startle stimuli are presented in pairs, then the reaction to the 2nd stimulus (S2) is reduced, with the size of the refractory decrement determined by the interstimulus interval and the relative intensity of the 1st stimulus (S1). If a neutral stimulus (p) is presented just prior to S1, then the reaction to S1 is similarly inhibited, revealing the phenomenon of prestimulus inhibition. In 2 experiments with male albino Holtzman rats (N = 24) it was found that suppression of the reflex to S2 by S1 was unaffected by prestimulus inhibition of S1 (i.e., reflex amplitudes associated with S2 were identical in pS1-S2 series and S1-S2 series). In contrast, a reduction in the intensity of S1 relative to S2 did reduce the effect of S1 on S2. These data indicate that prestimulus inhibition of the reflex to S1 does not result because the preliminary stimulus attenuates the sensory impact of S1. The inhibitory process may be presumed to have a central locus. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Previous results show that the acoustic startle reflex in the rat is inhibited if a relatively weak stimulus precedes the startle-eliciting tone burst. The present 5 experiments explored the effect of brief silent periods (gaps) in white noise on the startle reflex in order to describe the limits of temporal resolution in the auditory system of 12 Long-Evans hooded rats. Brief silent periods did depress reflex behavior, and 2 responsible processes were identified. One was most evident at a 190-msec lead time between gap and startle tone. It yielded a linear decrement in reflex expression over a dynamic range of 0–7 msec and an estimate for the threshold of temporal acuity of 3.5 msec. The 2nd was evident primarily at a 40-msec interstimulus interval and had a linear effect over a dynamic range of at least 40 msec. Brief gaps had a greater inhibitory effect at the 190-msec interval between gap and startle stimulus; prolonged gaps had their greater effect at the 40-msec interval. The 1st process was identified as reflex inhibition, which is sensitive to the sensory properties of the lead stimulus. The 2nd process was identified as sensory adaptation, produced by noise exposure but unmasked by silence. (47 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Fear-potentiated startle is defined as an increase in the magnitude of the startle reflex in the presence of a stimulus that was previously paired with an aversive event. It has been proposed that a subject's awareness of the contingencies in the experiment may affect fear-potentiated startle. The authors adapted a conditional discrimination procedure (AX+/BX-), previously validated in animals, to a human fear-potentiated startle paradigm in 50 healthy volunteers. This paradigm allows for an assessment of fear-potentiated startle during threat conditions as well as inhibition of fear-potentiated startle during safety conditions. A response keypad was used to assess contingency awareness on a trial-by-trial basis. Both aware and unaware subjects showed fear-potentiated startle. However, awareness was related to stimulus discrimination and fear inhibition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Conducted 4 experiments with 24 male albino rats relating inhibition and facilitation of the startle response, elicited by an intense auditory stimulus, to the offset of a 70-db continuous acoustic signal. Data indicate that if the antecedent signal terminated 10-1000 msec before the startle-eliciting stimulus, the amplitude of the startle response was reduced. If offset occurred less than 10 msec prior to the startle-eliciting stimulus, the response latency was reduced. Results are consistent with previous research employing weak antecedent acoustic signal onset. The overall configuration of the results suggests that activity in brain centers mediating the startle reflex may be an early component of the orienting reflex arc. (19 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Two experiments, with 16 Ss, examined developmental changes in visual function in the Royal College of Surgeons (RCS) rat with inherited retinal degeneration (retinal dystrophy) by studying the inhibition of acoustic startle reflexes by visual prestimuli. Compared with a congenic strain of nondystrophic rat, RCS Ss showed an increase in the interstimulus interval between the inhibitory prestimulus and the eliciting stimulus that produced maximal inhibition, a result suggesting a decrease in the speed of processing. The amount of inhibition also decreased over time, which suggests a progressive loss of visual function. Simultaneous presentation of auditory and visual prestimuli was used to demonstrate that the changes in inhibition were related to alterations in visual function and that auditory function was not impaired. Results show that reflex modification is a suitable test for evaluating visual dysfunction in rats. ( 27 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Potentiation of blink startle during aversive and nonaversive Pavlovian single-cue conditioning was assessed in human Ss. In Exp 1 (N?=?89), the conditioning group received paired presentations of a visual CS and an unconditioned stimulus/stimuli (UCS), whereas the control group was presented with a random sequence. The UCS was an electric shock for half the Ss and a nonaversive reaction time (RT) task for the other half. Electrodermal conditioning was evident regardless of the nature of the UCS, but blink potentiation was found only in the conditioning group that had been trained with the aversive UCS. These results were replicated in Exp 2 (N?=?65), in which a nonaversive UCS of increased motivational significance was used. Thus, only aversive conditioning seems to affect the affective valence of the CS, at least as reflected by changes in a skeletal reflex. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Conducted 3 experiments with 6 male albino rats each in which inhibition and facilitation of the startle response, elicited by an intense auditory signal, was related to a change of the frequency characteristic of a 70-db continuous acoustic signal. Data indicated that if a frequency change occurred in the acoustic environment 64 msec before the startle-eliciting stimulus, the amplitude of the startle response was reduced; if frequency change occurred 4 msec prior to the startle-eliciting stimulus, the response latency was reduced. Results extend the generality of previous research employing weak antecedent acoustic signal onset and offset. Results indicate that neural mechanisms mediating the startle reflex may be activated by any change in the acoustic environment and that these mechanisms may be a component of the orienting reflex arc. (21 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
The relationship between stimulus intensity and startle response magnitude (SIRM) can assess the startle reflex and prepulse inhibition (PPI) with advantages over more commonly used methods. The current study used the SIRM relationships in mice to determine differences between white noise and pure tone (5 kHz) stimuli. Similarly to rats, the SIRM relationship showed a sigmoid pattern. The SIRM-derived reflex capacity (RMAX) and response efficacy (slope) of the white noise and pure tone stimuli in the absence of prepulses were equivalent. However, the pure tone startle response threshold (DMIN) was increased whereas the stimulus potency (1/ES??) was decreased when compared to white noise. Prepulses of both stimulus types inhibited RMAX and increased DMIN, but the white noise prepulses were more effective. Both stimulus intensity gating and motor capacity gating processes are shown to occur, dependent on prepulse intensity and stimulus onset asynchrony. Prepulse intensities greater than 10 dB below the startle threshold appear to produce PPI via stimulus intensity gating, whereas a motor capacity gating component appears at prepulse intensities near to the startle threshold. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
We used affective modulation of the eye-blink component of the startle reflex to examine effects of three levels of alcohol intoxication and a no-intoxication control on emotional responses to pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant pictures. Non-problematic student drinkers (n = 101; 48 female) were randomly assigned to intoxication groups. Normal inhibition of startle during exposure to pleasant pictures was intact across groups. In contrast, potentiation of startle during viewing of unpleasant pictures was evident in the no- and low-intoxication groups, compared to the intermediate- and high-intoxication groups, in which it was significantly reduced. This pattern suggests that a direct and selective anxiolytic effect of alcohol can occur at higher levels of intoxication without an analogous impact on response to emotionally positive stimuli at similar levels. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
This study extended prior work showing abnormal affect–startle modulation in psychopaths. Male prisoners viewed specific categories of pleasant (erotic or thrilling) and unpleasant (victim or direct threat) slide pictures, along with neutral pictures. Acoustic startle probes were presented early (300 and 800 ms) and late (1,800, 3,000, and 4,500 ms) in the viewing interval. At later times, nonpsychopaths showed moderate and strong reflex potentiation for victim and threat scenes, respectively. For psychopaths, startle was inhibited during victim scenes and only weakly potentiated during threat. Psychopaths also showed more reliable blink inhibition across pleasant contents than nonpsychopaths and greater heart rate orienting to affective pictures overall. These results indicate a heightened aversion threshold in psychopaths. In addition, deficient reflex modulation at early times suggested a weakness in initial stimulus evaluation among psychopaths. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
In Exp I, 97 male Sprague-Dawley albino rats were given 10 light–shock pairings on 2 successive days. At 24–48 hrs following training, groups of Ss received bilateral transection of the cerebellar peduncles, bilateral lesions of the red nucleus (which receives most of the cerebellar efferents), or bilateral lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala. Controls were sham operated. At 3–4 days after surgery, Ss were tested for potentiated startle (PS [increased acoustic startle in the presence of the light previously paired with shock]). PS was blocked by lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala but not by transection of the cerebellar peduncles or lesions of the red nucleus. Exp II, in which a visual prepulse test was used with 14 Ss, indicated that the blockade of PS observed in Ss with amygdala lesions could not be attributed to optic tract damage. Exp III, with 20 Ss, demonstrated that the absence of potentiation in Ss with amygdala lesions was not simply due to a lowered startle level ceiling, because these Ss could show increased startle with increased stimulus intensity and with administration of intraperitoneal strychnine, (0.75 mg/kg), a drug that increases startle. Results are consistent with the hypothesis that the amygdala is involved in fear conditioning. (64 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Previous research with both animal and human Ss has shown that startle reflex magnitude is potentiated in an aversive stimulus context, relative to responses elicited in a neutral or appetitive context. In the present experiment, the same pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral picture stimuli were repeatedly presented to human Ss. Startle reflex habituation was assessed in each stimulus context and was compared with the habituation patterns of heart rate, electrodermal, and facial corrugator muscle responses. All systems showed initial differentiation among affective picture contents and general habituation over trials. The startle reflex alone, however, continued to differentiate among pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant pictures throughout the presentation series. These results suggest that (1) the startle probe reflex is relatively uninfluenced by stimulus novelty, (2) the startle modulatory circuit (identified with amygdala-reticular connections in animals) varies systematically with affective valence, and (3) the modulatory influence is less subject to habituation than is the obligatory startle pathway or responses in other somatic and autonomic systems. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Seven experiments related amplitude and latency of 20 racing pigeons' startle response, elicited by an intense visual stimulus, to antecedent auditory and visual events in the sensory environment. Data indicate that (a) within broad limits the amplitude of the reflex was a positive function of the intensity of the sensory background prevailing at the time of startle elicitation; (b) a change in the sensory environment occurring 15-2,000 msec prior to the startle-eliciting stimulus inhibited the amplitude of the response; and (c) a change in the sensory environment less than 10 msec prior to the startle-eliciting stimulus reduced the latency of the response. Findings are consistent with previous research on acoustic elicited startle in the rat. The overall configuration of the results suggests that a pathway including the reticulospinal tract and the bulbopontine reticular nuclei could be the major mediator of startle. In these terms, latency-reduction effects would occur because of partial activation of this pathway, amplitude inhibition would occur because of cerebellar influence, and amplitude facilitation would reflect cerebral or striatal influences. (49 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Four conditioned suppression experiments with 98 male albino rats compared the inhibitory strength of a Pavlovian conditioned inhibitory stimulus (CS–) and a differential CS– and identified some postconditioning manipulations that modulate the measured effectiveness of the CS–. In Exp I, more inhibition was detected to a differential inhibitor than to a Pavlovian inhibitor in summation and retardation tests. Exps II–IV provided evidence that some inhibition conditioned to the Pavlovian CS–, but not to the differential CS–, was masked by a within-compound association. In Exp II, postconditioning extinction presentations of the Pavlovian conditioned excitatory stimulus (CS+) increased the inhibition observed to its CS–. In Exp III, postconditioning pairings of the Pavlovian CS+ with a more powerful UCS than that used for conditioning reduced the inhibition observed to its CS–. In Exp IV, nonreinforced postconditioning presentations of the Pavlovian CS– increased the inhibition observed to that CS–. The unmasking and masking of inhibition conditioned to the Pavlovian CS– by operations that modulate the strength of the within-compound association also changed the relative effectiveness of the Pavlovian and differential procedures. (28 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Examined the developmental emergence of fear-potentiated startle in rats ranging in age from 16 to 75 days. In Exp 1, a pure tone served as the CS and an acoustic startle pulse served as the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) for fear conditioning. Fear-potentiated startle by the tone CS was observed in rats 23 days of age and older but not in rats 16 days of age. In Exp 2, a light served as the CS. Rats 30 days of age and older showed fear-potentiated startle, whereas 23-day-old rats did not. The final experiment demonstrated that another behavioral index of fear, stimulus-elicited freezing, was observed earlier in development than fear-potentiated startle, confirming the effectiveness of the training procedure for conditioning fear. Results suggest that fear-potentiated startle is a relatively late-emerging response system, parallelling the development of conditioned autonomic changes (e.g., heart rate) rather than that of freezing. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
Startle reflexes to intense sound bursts are inhibited by weak stimuli that briefly precede their elicitation. In 3 experiments, with 36 Long-Evans hooded rats, the startle stimulus (a 110-db tone burst) was presented 100 msec after the final link in a train of stimuli, the length of the train varying from 1 to 1,000, its repetition rate varying from 1 to 10 per sec, and its constituents being 40 db or 50 db white noise bursts of 25 msec duration. Inhibition was invariant across train length and repetition rate. In Exp IV, the startle stimulus was presented a variable interval after the final link, from 40 to 1,280 msec with 1 or 100 noise bursts (50 db) in the train. Inhibition developed more rapidly following the last member of the 100-stimulus train, suggesting a "priming" or sensitization effect of stimulus repetition, but its overall strength and subsequent rate of decay were not different in the 2 conditions. The general persistence of inhibition following these extended series of stimuli reveals that reflex inhibition must be the outcome of a fixed and obligatory process associated with sensory input. (29 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Two experiments with 47 albino rats measured acoustic startle response and freezing in a potentiated startle paradigm in which a startle stimulus was presented either alone or in the presence of a light conditioned stimulus/stimuli (CS) that had been paired previously with either 1-mA or 3-mA footshock. During the CS, the 1-mA group had higher startle amplitudes and a higher percentage of freezing than the 3-mA group. Startle amplitude was positively correlated with freezing under all conditions. The nonmonotonic relation between potentiated startle and shock intensity replicated the study of M. Davis and D. I. Astrachan (see record 1979-00353-001). However, rather than suppressing startle, as they suggested, freezing facilitated startle and, like startle amplitude, was nonmonotonically related to shock intensity. In Exp II, these results were replicated and showed a regularly decreasing monotonic extinction function or potentiated startle and shock-associated freezing for both shock-level groups. (19 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
The size of the rat's acoustic startle reflex was augmented by brief acoustic clicks (which did not themselves elicit startle) presented several milliseconds before the reflex-eliciting stimulus (RS). The same clicks presented after the RS gave relatively weak augmentation that was present in the 1st, but not the 2nd, testing session. Brief footshocks set to 75% of each animal's flinch threshold augmented startle when presented both before and after the RS in both testing sessions. Augmentation by a leading footshock increased with shock intensity and also with the intensity of the RS. Augmentation by a trailing footshock increased with shock intensity and also with the intensity of the RS. Reflex size is not fixed at the time of reflex elicitation but can be augmented by a later nonreflexogenic stimulus. Reflex augmentation may be caused by the 2nd member of a stimulus pair discharging elements of the reflex pathway that were partially activated by the 1st. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues was examined in Gulf War veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) by use of the startle reflex methodology. Veterans participated in a differential aversive conditioning experiment consisting of 2 sessions separated by 4 or 5 days. Each session comprised two startle habituation periods, a preconditioning phase, a conditioning phase, and a postconditioning extinction test. In contrast to the non-PTSD group, the PTSD group showed a lack of differential startle response in the presence of a conditioned stimulus with or without an unconditioned stimulus in Session 1 and an increase in the baseline startle response during Session 2. The PTSD group also exhibited normal differential conditioning following reconditioning in Session 2. These data suggest that individuals with PTSD tend to generalize fear across stimuli and are sensitized by stress. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
This study explored the time course of conditioned fear response expression. Two neutral male facial expressions served as conditioned stimuli (CS) in a differential trace conditioning that involved either an aversive (n=14) or a nonaversive (n=12) unconditioned stimulus (UCS) in a between-subjects design. Skin conductance response (SCR) to the CSs and startle response magnitudes to acoustic probes presented at early (250 ms) or late (1,750 ms) probe times after CS onset were measured. As expected, conditioned SCR discrimination was observed in both aversive and nonaversive learning, whereas the conditioned potentiation of the startle response was only observed for the aversive UCS condition. Interestingly, conditioned startle discrimination was specific for the later probe time. In contrast, conditioned fear potentiation of the startle response at the early probe time was equally pronounced for CS+ and CS-. These findings suggest that fear-eliciting neural structures are rapidly activated in fear learning, whereas the expression of inhibitory conditioning requires more time, presumably reflecting the involvement of cortical top-down control processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号