首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Examined rates of shuttle box avoidance responding in 3 strains of rats as a function of classical and instrumental contingencies in 2 experiments. Ss were a total of 126 female albino Fischer, Lewis, and Long-Evans rats. In Exp I, during classical conditioned-stimulus-unconditioned-stimulus (CS-UCS) pairings in the absence of an avoidance contingency, there were large differences between the 3 strains in rates of anticipatory responding to the CS. The same pattern of differences was observed in Exp II when the avoidance contingency was added. None of the instrumental contingencies of CS termination, UCS termination, or the avoidance contingency differentially affected the strains. Classically elicited anticipatory responses and their compatibility with the required avoidance response were viewed as central factors in both the acquisition and maintenance of skeletal avoidance responses. (26 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Describes 2 experiments in which, following signaled shuttle box avoidance training, a total of 52 female Fischer344 rats were exposed to the conditioned stimulus (CS) during no-shock treatment trials and subsequently tested during extinction trials in which shock was also absent. In Exp I, Ss that could control the termination of the CS during treatment responded significantly more often during extinction than yoked partners that received the same pattern and duration of CS exposure but could not control its termination. Exp II revealed that the probability of responding during extinction was a decreasing function of the duration of CS exposure during treatment. Thus, in the absence of shock, both lack of control over CS termination and increasing CS exposure each independently facilitated the weakening of well-established avoidance responses. (21 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Presented CS to rats licking sucrose after 1 of several off-the-baseline Pavlovian defense conditioning procedures. 2 random procedures in which there was no CS-UCS contingency were constructed by programing CS and UCS on independent random schedules which had either a long or a short variable inter-CS interval (VITI). Ss were 48 naive male Sprague-Dawley rats in Exp. I and II and 20 rats from Exp. II in the 3rd experiment. Because the CS took up more of the session in the short VITI procedure, it contained more CS-UCS pairings and produced CS-elicited suppression of licking. Suppression occurred with neither the long VITI group nor with shock alone controls. Conditioning was all-or-none after both classical and random procedures. (French summary) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Conducted 2 experiments using 64 and 32 male Long-Evans hooded rats, respectively. Exp. I investigated the extinction of the conditioned avoidance response (CAR) by response prevention and counterconditioning methods. Response prevention was most effective in extinguishing both the CAR and associated conditioned fear, although counterconditioning produced greater extinction than the regular extinction procedure. Exp. II equated the counterconditioning and response-prevention conditions for duration of CS exposure and demonstrated the superiority of the latter in extinguishing the CAR; both methods were equally effective in decreasing conditioned fear as compared to the regular extinction procedure. Extinction of the CAR was facilitated to the extent to which different procedures eliminated response-contingent feedback by reducing escape-avoidance responses. Conditioned fear was a function of the amount of nonreinforced exposure to the CS during extinction. (24 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Examined the notion of conditioned inhibition and suggests a definition in terms of the learned ability of a stimulus to control a response tendency opposed to excitation. 2 techniques of measuring inhibition are outlined: (1) the summation procedure in which an inhibitor reduces the response that would normally be elicited by another stimulus, and (2) the retardation of acquisition procedure in which an inhibitor is retarded in the acquisition of an excitatory CR. Examples of the use of these procedures are given for a variety of UCS modalities. Several possible operations for generating conditioned inhibitors are reviewed: extinction following excitatory conditioning, discriminative conditioning, arrangement of a negative correlation between CS and a UCS, use of an extended CS-UCS interval, and presentation of a stimulus in conjunction with UCS termination. These operations suggest that conditioned inhibitors are not generated either by simple extinction procedures or by pairing a stimulus with UCS termination. By contrast, for both salivary and fear conditioning the other procedures do appear to generate inhibitors. Most of the procedures generating conditioned inhibitors can be described as arranging a negatively correlated CS and UCS. (2 p. ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
The comparator hypothesis posits that conditioned responding is determined by a comparison at the time of testing between the associative strengths of the conditioned stimulus/stimuli (CS) and stimuli proximal to the CS at the time of conditioning. The hypothesis treats all associations as being excitatory and treats conditioned inhibition as the behavioral consequence of a CS that is less excitatory than its comparator stimuli. Conditioned lick suppression by rats was used to differentiate 4 possible sources of retarded responding to an inhibitory CS. These include habituation to the unconditioned stimulus/stimuli (UCS), latent inhibition to the CS, blocking of the CS-UCS association by the conditioning context, and enhanced excitatory associations to the comparator stimuli. Prior research has demonstrated the 1st 3 phenomena. Therefore, we employed parameters expected to highlight the 4th one—the comparator process. In Exp I, our negative contingency training produced a conditioned inhibitor that passed inhibitory summation and retardation tests. In Exp II we found transfer of retardation from an inhibitory CS to a novel stimulus when the location where retardation-test training occurred was excitatory. In Exp III, extinction of the conditioning context attenuated retardation regardless of whether extinction occurred before or after the CS-UCS pairings of the retardation test. Exp IV demonstrated that habituation to the UCS did not contribute to retardation in the present case. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
2 experiments examined escape-avoidance responding in a 1-way jump box following exposure to signaled, inescapable shock. In Exp I with 24 female Long-Evans rats, the occurrence of failures to escape and to avoid was an increasing function of the number of preshocks, over a range of 75-275, with a pronounced interference effect occurring after 225 and 275 preshocks. In Exp II with 10 Long-Evans (noninbred) and 10 Fischer (inbred) female rats, there were large differences between strains in failures both to escape and to avoid following 225 preshocks. Long-Evans Ss were severely retarded, whereas Fischer Ss were disrupted only during the initial trials. Findings demonstrate the importance of strain and number of preshocks in controlling the interference effect in rats. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Second order schedules of IV cocaine reinforcement in rats provide a reliable method for evaluating the effects of conditioned stimuli on cocaine-seeking behaviour, and for measuring the motivational aspects of cocaine reinforcement. In the procedure established here, each infusion of cocaine (0.25 mg/infusion) was initially made contingent on a lever press and was paired with a 20-s light conditioned stimulus (CS). When rats acquired stable rates of cocaine self-administration, the response requirement for cocaine was increased progressively to a second-order schedule of the type FI15 min(FR10:S), whereby the IV cocaine infusion was self-administered following the completion of the first FR10 responses (and CS presentation) after a 15-min fixed interval (FI) had elapsed. Evaluation of the animals' responding during the first, drug-free interval of each daily session provided a measure of cocaine-seeking behaviour, independent of other pharmacological effects of the self-administered drug. Thus, a dose-response study (dose range: 0.083, 0.25 and 0.50 mg/infusion) revealed that responding under this schedule during the initial, drug-free interval changed monotonically with dose, whereas an inverse relationship between cocaine dose and response level tended to appear during the rest of the session, after rats had self-administered the drug. Responding under this schedule was also shown to occur under the control of the CS, which had acquired conditioned reinforcing properties. Thus, a decrease in responding and an increase in the latency to initiate responding followed the omission of the CS for 3 consecutive days. In addition, extinction of cocaine-seeking behaviour was slower when contingent CS presentations occurred compared to extinction when the CS was not present. Furthermore, the reinstatement of responding for cocaine, which followed a brief period of non-contingent CS presentations, was retarded when this conditioned reinforcer had been extinguished together with cocaine. Finally, cocaine-seeking behaviour decreased markedly for the first 6 h that followed a 12-h period of continuous access to cocaine, when compared to responding 6 h after a 90-min session of limited access to the drug. Responding subsequently increased to baseline levels within 72 h. These results emphasise the utility of second-order schedules for studying drug-seeking behaviour and the importance of drug-associated cues in maintaining such responding for cocaine.  相似文献   

9.
Conditioning-specific reflex modification (CRM) occurs when classical conditioning modifies responding to a unconditioned stimulus/stimuli (UCS) in the absence of a conditioned stimulus (CS). Three experiments monitored rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) nictitating membrane unconditioned responses to 5 intensities and 4 durations of periorbital electrical stimulation before and after CS or UCS manipulation. CRM occurred after 12 days of CS-UCS pairings but not following unpaired CS/UCS presentations or restraint. CRM survived CS-alone and CS/UCS-unpaired extinction of the conditioned response (CR) but not presentations of the UCS alone, although CRs remained intact. Thus, CRs could be weakened without eliminating CRM and CRM could be weakened without eliminating CRs. Data indicate CRM is a reliable, associative effect that is more than a generalized CR and may not be explained by habituation, stimulus generalization, contextual conditioning, or bidirectional conditioning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Used 1 or 2 extinction sessions, 2 extinction procedures, and 2 ages of rats (23 and 95-120 days old) in a developmental analysis of the effects of extinction. Ss were 96 male Long-Evans hooded rats. The apparatus was a Y maze with 3 discriminably different arms. After 10 sessions of positive reinforcement, Ss received either 1 or 2 extinction sessions in which responding in 1 arm was no longer reinforced. Following extinction, responding in all 3 arms was again reinforced. Recovery data show that (a) the suppressive effects of extinction were greater for adults than infants and (b) the 2nd extinction session had a greater impact on the adults. Results support the hypothesis that younger rats have greater difficulty than older ones in inhibiting a response. (17 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
The administration of the dopamine antagonists, pimozide and α-flupenthixol, to rats reduced Pavlovian–instrumental transfer when a conditioned stimulus (CS) that had been paired with a noncontingent food reward was tested on instrumental performance. The administration of the antagonists during Pavlovian conditioning and/or testing abolished the enhancement of instrumental performance by the CS. The effect of both antagonists on instrumental incentive learning was then examined. After training in which the rats performed 2 responses for different food rewards, they consumed 1 type food under the antagonists and the other type under vehicle during reexposure. When instrumental responding was subsequently tested in extinction, performance was unaffected by whether the rats had been reexposed to the training reward under the antagonists. These results suggest that Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning are not mediated by a common process. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Conducted 2 experiments to compare the effectiveness of P. L. Brown and H. M. Jenkins's (see record 1968-06958-001) autoshaping procedure with that of instrumental conditioning in the establishment and maintenance of instrumental responding. Ss were 40 male and 32 female Long-Evans rats. In a leverpressing situation, with presentation of the lever signaling trial onset, the 2 procedures were equally effective. When illumination of a key signaled trial onset and the response was pressing the key, the instrumental-training procedure was superior to the autoshaping procedure in establishing and maintaining instrumental responding. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
During Pavlovian fear conditioning a conditioned stimulus (CS) is repeatedly paired with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (UCS). In many studies the CS and UCS are paired on every trial, whereas in others the CS and UCS are paired intermittently. To better understand the influence of the CS-UCS pairing rate on brain activity, the experimenters presented continuously, intermittently, and non-paired CSs during fear conditioning. Amygdala, anterior cingulate, and fusiform gyrus activity increased linearly with the CS-UCS pairing rate. In contrast, insula and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex responses were larger during intermittently paired CS presentations relative to continuously and non-paired CSs. These results demonstrate two distinct patterns of activity in disparate brain regions. Amygdala, anterior cingulate, and fusiform gyrus activity paralleled the CS-UCS pairing rate, whereas the insula and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex appeared to respond to the uncertainty inherent in intermittent CS-UCS pairing procedures. These findings may further clarify the role of these brain regions in Pavlovian fear conditioning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
Four experiments examined the conditioning of sexual arousal in 81 male Long-Evans hooded rats. In each case, the unconditioned response (UCR) was unconsummated arousal after exposure to a female. There was evidence of a substantial conditioned effect, as shown by decreases in the time to complete copulation during postconditioning conditioned stimulus/stimuli (CS) tests. It was also possible to establish a 2nd-order conditioned response (CR), which retained its strength even after extinction of the 1st-order response. Results confirm the power of Pavlovian contingencies in sexual responding. The locus and mode of action of this CR and its function are discussed. (20 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Developed and evaluated an automated procedure for sniff detection in 4 experiments with 21 adult New Zealand white rabbits. The sniff was identified by a brief episode of increased respiratory rate, usually with a well-defined time of onset. It was detected against the background of respiratory activity in Ss simply, reliably, and noninvasively by statistical evaluation of digitized pneumograph records. The basal rate of exploratory sniffing was controlled by familiarization. Upon conditioning to olfactory cues, the rate of sniffing for CS+ increased sharply above the basal rate during the 1st trials and was maintained at high levels by continued reinforcement. During extinction with discrimination between olfactory cues, the rate for CS– fell sharply at first and then more slowly toward the basal rate. With pseudoconditioning, Ss responded to an unpaired odor after several sessions; the rates of response acquisition and extinction and the maintained level of responding were lower than with a paired odor in classical delayed conditioning, and the response was not discriminative in respect to another novel odor given during extinction. The sniff displayed a prominent sensory bias for olfactory cues. The relative frequencies of sniffing and respiratory slowing were measured as CRs by screening procedures with a small computer. (23 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
Gave 48 male Blue Spruce rats shuttle-box training with serial or nonserial CS procedures. The serial CS condition (S1/S1S2), which involved a single stimulus for the 1st 1/2 of a 16-sec CS-UCS interval and 2 stimuli for the latter 1/2, produced shorter avoidance latencies and more avoidance responses when compared with a serial condition (S1/S2), in which the latter 1/2 of the interval involved only 1 stimulus. Both serial conditions resulted in longer avoidance latencies when compared to nonserial conditions. Exp. II with 144 Ss demonstrated that the above latency differences could be eliminated with shorter CS-UCS intervals, and Exp. III with 72 Ss suggested that avoidance-latency differences obtained at longer CS-UCS intervals were independent of the CS duration ratio between serial components. These and other findings were predicted from a generalization-decrement hypothesis. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Investigated the slow reacquisition (RAQ) of responding in rats that occurs when the conditioned stimulus/stimuli (CS) and unconditioned stimulus/stimuli (UCS) are paired again after prolonged extinction training. In Exp 1, an extinguished CS acquired less suppression than a novel CS during a final conditioning phase, but more suppression than CSs that had received comparable nonreinforcement without initial conditioning. In Exp 2, CS–UCS pairings resumed in the context of extinction caused the least RAQ of suppression: Pairings in a neutral context produced better RAQ, while return of the CS to the conditioning context caused an immediate renewal of responding to the CS. In Exp 3, a return of the CS to the extinction context after RAQ training caused renewed extinction performance and interfered with performance appropriate to RAQ. This effect was not due to demonstrable inhibitory conditioning of the extinction context. Results suggest that representations of conditioning and extinction (or CS–UCS and CS–no UCS relations) are both retained through extinction and that performance appropriate to either phase can be cued by the corresponding context. RAQ may thus be slow when the context retrieves an extinction memory. Similar mechanisms may also play a role in other Pavlovian interference paradigms. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
Three experiments, with 118 Sprague-Dawley rats, assessed conditioned analgesia in a Pavlovian 2nd-order conditioning procedure by using inhibition of responding to thermal stimulation as an index of pain sensitivity. In Exp I, Ss receiving 2nd-order conditioning showed longer response latencies during a test of pain sensitivity in the presence of the 2nd-order conditioned stimulus (CS) than Ss receiving appropriate control procedures. Exp II found that extinction of the 1st-order CS had no effect on established 2nd-order conditioned analgesia. Exp III evaluated the effects of post 2nd-order conditioning pairings of subcutaneous morphine sulfate (10–20 mg/kg) and the shock unconditioned stimulus/stimuli (UCS). Ss receiving paired morphine–shock presentations showed significantly shorter response latencies during a hot-plate test of pain sensitivity in the presence of the 2nd-order CS than did Ss receiving various control procedures; 2nd-order analgesia was attenuated. Data extend the associative account of conditioned analgesia to 2nd-order conditioning situations and are discussed in terms of the mediation of both 1st- and 2nd-order analgesia by an association between the CS and a representation or expectancy of the UCS, which may directly activate endogenous pain inhibition systems. (52 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
20.
Five conditioned suppression experiments, with 160 Wistar rats, explored the role of the conditioning history of the conditioned stimulus (CS) in determining the effects of contextual fear on performance to the CS. Contextual fear was produced by postconditioning exposure to unconditioned stimulus/stimuli (UCS) alone in the context of conditioning; it was independently assessed with context-preference tests. When the number of reinforced and nonreinforced trials was equated across extinction, partial reinforcement, and latent inhibition procedures, only the extinction procedure produced a CS whose performance was subsequently affected (i.e., augmented) by contextual fear. Contextual fear's relatively unique augmenting effect on fear of an extinguished CS was abolished by extensive, but not by less extensive, reacquisition training. Results indicate that, depending on the CS's conditioning history, contextual fear either augments or has little effect on fear of the CS. It is suggested that augmentation by context should be viewed as the restoration of fear that is otherwise depressed by extinction. (28 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号