首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The impact of maternal depression and adversity on mother-infant face-to-face interactions at 2 months, and on subsequent infant cognitive development and attachment, was examined in a low-risk sample of primiparous women and their infants. The severe disturbances in mother-infant engagement characteristic of depressed groups in disadvantaged populations were not evident in the context of postpartum mood disorder in the present study. However, compared to well women, depressed mothers were less sensitively attuned to their infants, and were less affirming and more negating of infant experience. Similar difficulties in maternal interactions were also evident in the context of social and personal adversity. Disturbances in early mother-infant interactions were found to be predictive of poorer infant cognitive outcome at 18 months. Infant attachment, by contrast, was not related to the quality of 2-month interactions, but was significantly associated with the occurrence of adversity, as well as postpartum depression.  相似文献   

2.
Three issues were investigated: (a) the regulatory effects of presumed infant and maternal regulation behaviors on infant distress to novelty at 6 months, (b) stability of infant regulatory effects across contexts that vary in maternal involvement, and (c) associations and temporal dynamics between infant and maternal regulation behaviors. Participants were 87 low-risk infants and their mothers, observed at 6 months postpartum during infant exposure to novel toys. Contingencies derived from sequential analyses demonstrate that, by 6 months, some infants reduce their own distress to novelty by looking away from the novel toy or self-soothing, maternal engagement and support have comparable effects, and certain infant and maternal behaviors co-occur. Moreover, infants whose mothers engaged contingently when they looked away from the novel toy expressed less distress than comparable infants whose mothers did not. These findings implicate both infants and mothers in the development of emotion regulation during the infant's first year. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
AIMS: To investigate the transmission of hepatitis C virus from viraemic mothers to infants. METHODS: The study group comprised 54 hepatitis C ribonucleic acid (RNA) positive, human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) negative women attending antenatal clinic, their infants when born, 12 previous children and 44 children of 29 additional nonpregnant, viraemic women. During the study period there were 60 live births (1 set of twins, 5 sequential pregnancies). All infants were tested at birth for hepatitis C virus (HCV) RNA. Thirty infants were retested at 6 months or later. Breast milk from 30 mothers was tested for HCV RNA. The 56 other children were tested for antibody to HCV and HCV RNA. RESULTS: Of the 60 infants tested at birth, 30 failed to attend a 6 month or later followup, 2 infants were HCV viraemic by six months of age, 2 infants had one episode of possible HCV RNA positivity followed by loss of detectable HCV RNA and 26 have shown no evidence of HCV infection. Five of the 30 breast milk samples tested were positive for HCV RNA. Four older children of viraemic mothers were HCV RNA positive. CONCLUSIONS: In this study, 2 of 30 (6.6%) of infants born to HIV negative, HCV viraemic mothers acquired HCV infection. Breast milk remains a possible contributory source of infant HCV infection. Management of babies born to HCV viraemic mothers should include retesting of baby for HCV RNA at 3 to 6 months of age.  相似文献   

4.
5.
6.
We studied the preventive effect against allergies in infants who and whose mothers consumed hypoallergenic formulas until 6 months after birth. Mother and infant pairs were divided into three groups, and the infants were monitored for the development of allergies for the first 2 years. In the MD group (n = 102; n = number of infants), the mothers were given a hypoallergenic formula for mothers (MOM HA), which contained hydrolyzed whey protein as the only protein source, as a substitution for cow's milk during late pregnancy and lactation. In the CD group (n = 127), the mothers were given cow's milk during the corresponding period. All infants in the MD and CD groups were exclusively breast-fed or mixed-fed with breast milk and hypoallergenic infant formula (NAN HA), which contains the same hydrolyzed protein as MOM HA. In the AF group (n = 54), the mothers consumed MOM HA and their infants were mixed-fed with breast milk and a cow's milk-based adopted infant formula during the corresponding period. In the MD group, no infants were positive to cow's milk-specific immunoglobulin E (RAST) at 4 months of age, in contrast to 6% and 3% of infants in the CD and AF groups, respectively. The infants in the MD group showed low incidence of various allergies, especially of eczema, as compared to the CD and AF groups. These results suggest that consumption of cow's milk by mothers and cow's milk-based formula feeding to infants elevate the risk of allergies in infants, and that consumption of hypoallergenic formula for pregnant and lactating women and for infants could be helpful in preventing allergy development in infants.  相似文献   

7.
This study documented the growth of the earliest form of face-to-face communication in 16 mother-infant dyads, videotaped weekly during a naturalistic face-to-face interaction, between 1 and 14 weeks, in 2 conditions: with the infant in the mother's arms and with the infant semi-reclined on a sofa. Results showed a curvilinear development of early face-to-face communication, with a significant increase occurring between Week 4 and Week 9 depending on the dyad. After 2 months, trajectories diverged into 2 groups: 1 whose duration of face-to-face communication continued to increase and 1 whose duration peaked and then began to decrease. After the 1st month, the duration of face-to-face communication was significantly longer when the infant was on the sofa rather than in the mother's arms. In the latter condition, during the 3rd month, girls spent a significantly longer time than boys in face-to-face communication. These findings suggest that context (infant being held vs. not being held) interacts with the infant's age and sex in affecting mother-infant communication. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
A community sample was screened to select three groups of infants and their mothers according to how much the babies cried at 6 weeks of age, the peak age for infant crying. The three groups--of moderate (n = 55), evening (n = 38) and persistent criers (n = 67) and their mothers--were assessed by diary, observation and questionnaire measures of mother and infant characteristics and interactions at 6 weeks and 5 months of infant age. At 6 weeks, mothers of persistent criers spent more time interacting with and physically stimulating their babies. Below-optimum maternal sensitivity/affection was linked to moderately increased crying in the infants overall. However, most mothers of persistent criers showed optimum sensitivity and affection, while no significant links between maternal sensitivity/affection and infant crying were found in the persistent crying group. By 5 months, when infant crying declined, the range and size of differences between mothers of persistent criers and other mothers declined. Home observations and a standard play measure failed to show group differences in maternal sensitivity, affection and intrusiveness at this age. The findings show that persistent infant crying in the early months often occurs in spite of high quality maternal care, so that in most cases the crying is probably not due to inadequate parenting. The need to distinguish general community cases from those at social or medical risk is emphasized and the findings' implications for professionals are discussed.  相似文献   

9.
Mother-infant dyads grouped according to whether the infants had been delivered vaginally (n?=?74) or by planned (n?=?37) or unplanned cesarean (n?=?56) were compared on psychosocial outcomes at 4 and 12 months postpartum. Hypotheses were that unplanned cesarean delivery would be related to less optimal outcomes and that this relationship would be mediated by mother's appraisal of the delivery and would attenuate over time. No delivery-related differences in mother–infant interactions were found at 4 or 12 months postpartum with one exception: Women low in neuroticism who delivered by unplanned cesarean showed less positive affect toward their infants at 4 months than did women high in neuroticism who delivered by unplanned cesarean or women in any other group. There was some evidence of the mediating role of maternal appraisal of the delivery on these effects. In general, the results indicate little cause for concern about the quality of mother–infant interactions following cesarean deliveries. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
OBJECTIVE: To test the hypothesis that a baby's survival is related to the mother's birth weight. DESIGN: Population based dataset for two generations. SETTING: Population registry in Norway. SUBJECTS: All birth records for women born in Norway since 1967 were linked to births during 1981-94, thereby forming 105104 mother-offspring units. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Perinatal mortality specific for weight for offspring in groups of maternal birth weight (with 500 g categories in both). RESULTS: A mother's birth weight was strongly associated with the weight of her baby. Maternal birth weight was associated with perinatal survival of her baby only for mothers with birth weights under 2000 g. These mothers were more likely to lose a baby in the perinatal period (odds ratio 2.3, 95% confidence interval 1.4 to 3.7). Among mothers with a birth weight over 2000 g there was no overall association between mother's weight and infant survival. There was, however, a strong interaction between mother's birth weight, infant birth weight, and infant survival. Mortality among small babies was much higher for those whose mothers had been large at birth. For example, babies weighing 2500-2999 g had a threefold higher mortality if their mother's birth weight had been high (> or = 4000 g) than if the mother had been small (2500-2999 g). CONCLUSION: Mothers who weighed less than 2000 g at birth have a higher risk of losing their own babies. For mothers who weighed > or = 2000 g their birth weight provides a benchmark for judging the growth of their offspring. Babies who are small relative to their mother's birth weight are at increased risk of mortality.  相似文献   

11.
To study the social-emotional development of triplets, 23 sets of triplets, 23 sets of twins, and 23 singleton infants (N=138) were followed from birth to 2 years. Maternal depression and social support were assessed in the postpartum period, mother-infant and father-infant interaction and the home environment were observed at 3 months, a separation-reunion episode and a maternal interview were conducted at 12 months, and infant behavior problems were evaluated at 24 months. Lower parent-infant synchrony was observed for triplets. Triplets showed less distress during maternal separation and less approach at reunion. Mothers reported lower adjustment and differentiation among siblings for triplets than for twins. Higher internalizing problems were reported for triplets, and the triplet with intrauterine growth retardation showed the poorest outcomes. Behavior problems were predicted by medical risk, maternal depression, parent-infant synchrony, infant approach, and mother adjustment. Discussion focuses on developmental risk when the exclusivity of the parent-infant relationship is compromised. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
13.
Accommodative esotropia during the first year of life   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Two infants developed accommodative esotropia during their first six months of life. One infant, whose age at onset was 4 1/2 months, had 4.50 diopters of hyperopia. The second infant, whose age at onset was 5 months, had 3.50 diopters of hyperopia. In both infants, the eyes completely aligned with hyperopic correction. Two points are stressed. First, when the amount of hyperopia exceeds +3.00, consideration should be given to an accommodative element as the cause of the esotropia, even if the child is only 4 to 6 months old. Second, even with small angles of esotropia, an accommodative element should be considered, if there is a substantial amount of hyperopia.  相似文献   

14.
This study investigated (a) stability and change in infant affective responses to the still-face interaction, (b) whether maternal depression affected infant responses, and (c) whether responses to the still-face interaction predicted toddler problem behaviors. Infants (63 girls and 66 boys) of European American mothers (67 depressed and 62 nondepressed) were observed in the still-face interaction at 2, 4, and 6 months. Affect and gaze were coded on a 1-sec time base. There were stable individual differences in gazing away and in rates of negative affect. Developmental change occurred only for gazing away, which increased. At 18 months, infants who failed to smile at 6 months in the still-face interaction showed more externalizing-type behaviors than did other toddlers. Infants who failed to cry at 6 months showed fewer internalizing-type behaviors. Mothers' current depressive symptoms and infants' earlier responses to the still-face interaction made independent, comparable contributions to problem behaviors at 18 months. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
16.
OBJECTIVE: To study the kinetics of Haemophilus influenzae type b (Hib)-specific antibody in infants born to mothers immunized with an Hib polysaccharide or one of two Hib conjugate vaccines. STUDY DESIGN: Serum antibody to the polyribosylribitol (PRP) moiety of Hib was measured by radioimmunoassay and enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay at birth and at 2 and 6 months of age in infants born to women immunized with Hib polysaccharide or conjugate vaccine (PRP-D and HbOC). A subset of infants > or = 6 months of age was immunized with Hib conjugate vaccine after licensure of this vaccine for infants. A comparison group of 18 infants born to unimmunized women received the same Hib conjugate vaccine on a similar schedule. RESULTS: Total PRP antibody concentrations were 1.50, 14.4 and 20.4 microg/ml in 2-month-old infants born to mothers immunized with polysaccharide, PRP-D and HbOC vaccines, respectively, and 2.54, 1.35 and 2.46 microg/ml in 6-month-old infants. Infants born to mothers immunized with polysaccharide vaccine had significantly less PRP antibody at 2 months of age but similar antibody concentrations at 6 months of age. Persistence or increases in total PRP antibody during 6 months were noted in 21 of 47 (44.6%) study infants. A subset of study and comparison infants was immunized with a mean of 2.6 doses of Hib vaccines between 6 months and 2 years of age, and all infants had total PRP antibody concentrations > or = 0.15 microg/ml. CONCLUSION: Conjugate Hib vaccines administered during the last trimester of pregnancy resulted in significantly higher PRP antibody titers in infants at birth and 2 months of age than did polysaccharide vaccine. A subset of infants born to immunized mothers was subsequently immunized with Hib conjugate vaccine and had antibody concentrations similar to those in infants born to nonimmunized women.  相似文献   

17.
Interaction during feeding sessions between preterm infants and their mothers and that between full-term infants and their mothers were compared. (Mothers and babies were from a low-income, inner-city population.) When the children were about 3 years old, they attended a day camp for 3 weeks, during which their cognitive ability (Stanford-Binet) and social ability (both social competence and social participation) were assessed. Early interaction was quite different for preterms and full-terms, but in general it did not predict either social or cognitive ability at age 3. Birth status (preterm/full-term) did predict cognitive (but not social) ability: preterms scored lower. Finally, the children of mothers who were more emotionally and verbally responsive during a home visit at 20 months exhibited more social and cognitive ability at age 3. These results suggest that the baby, within broad normal limits, may be "buffered" against any long-term consequences of interaction during the first few months of life.  相似文献   

18.
Differential emotions theory (DET) proposes that infant facial expressions of emotions are differentiated. To test this hypothesis, the authors examined infant facial expressions longitudinally at 2, 4, and 6 mo of age during face-to-face play and a "still-face" interaction with their mothers. Infant expressions were coded using the Maximally Discriminative Facial Movement Coding System (Max). Consistent with DET, discrete positive expressions occurred more of the time and were of longer duration than blended expressions of positive affect. Contrary to DET, at no age did the proportions or durations of discrete and blended negative expressions differ, and they showed different patterns of developmental change. One is led to either reject or revise DET or else question the adequacy of the Max system. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
In the following article we shall investigate the Second World War with its consequences on nutrition and other living conditions for the German population to try to determine whether lack of nutrition plus other deficiencies, which infants were subjected to in Germany during this time, had any influence on their adult stature. The following data formed the basis of our investigation: (1) nutrition of infants and school-aged children during the years 1939-1948; (2) stature and weight of 6-year-old boys between 1907 and 1948; (3) infant mortality between 1935 and 1950; (4) stature of 19-year-old men who were born between 1938 and 1951. From Figure 5 it can be seen that the mean stature of 19-year-old men born between 1938 and 1951 (except for those born in 1944 and 1945) rose steadily (secular acceleration). The plot of the yearly rate of acceleration (Fig. 6) reveals a minimum in 1944-1945, and follows a general downward, U-shaped trend (P less than 0.05). On the other hand, infant mortality between 1941 and 1949 follows an upward U-shaped trend (P less than 0.05). For the 6- to 8-month-old infants during 1945-1948 nutrition was far worse than that during the preceding years. It is even likely that the nutrition for the infants had already begun to decrease in quality in 1942. We find, therefore, a close correlation between the years of high infant mortality and the growth deficiencies of the men born during these years. Since we must assume that the nutritional situation for babies and infants worsened steadily after 1942 until it is apparent that a number of environmental factors directly influence human growth. The conclusion of our analysis is that a deficit of growth occurring after the end of the fourth year can be compensated fully. We conclude, furthermore, that the growth deficits which arise before the fourth year, on the other hand, cannot be compensated fully even if good or excellent environmental conditions follow.  相似文献   

20.
Investigated the impact of maternal language on infant emotional development during the 1st year. 41 mother-infant dyads participated in 4 episodes of face to face interaction when infants were 1, 2, 6, and 9 mo of age. Analyses of mothers' language indicate that a large proportion of their utterances pertained to infant emotional expressions or states. The referents of maternal language were related to infant behavior at each age level. However, only a few significant correlations were found between mothers' language at 1 and 2 mo and infant emotional expressions at 6 and 9 mo. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号