共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The first experiment investigated the effects of mild to moderate sensorineural hearing impairment on temporal analysis for noise stimuli of varying bandwidth. Tasks of temporal gap detection, amplitude modulation (AM) detection, and AM discrimination were examined. Relatively high levels of stimulation were used in order to reduce the possibility that the results of the listeners with hearing impairment would be influenced strongly by audibility. A general summary of results was that there was relatively great interlistener variation among the listeners with hearing impairment, with most listeners showing normal performance and some showing degraded performance, regardless of the bandwidth of the stimulus carrying the temporal information. A second experiment investigated the hypothesis that listeners with sensorineural hearing impairment might have poor gap detection due to loudness recruitment. Here, gap markers were presented at levels where loudness growth was steeper for the listeners with hearing impairment than for the listeners with normal hearing. Although gap detection was sometimes poorer in listeners with hearing impairment than in listeners with normal hearing, there was no clear relation between gap detection performance and loudness recruitment in listeners with mild to moderate sensorineural hearing impairment. 相似文献
2.
This paper examines the possibility of estimating basilar-membrane (BM) nonlinearity using a psychophysical technique. The level of a forward masker required to mask a brief signal was measured for conditions where the masker was either at, or one octave below, the signal frequency. The level of the forward masker at masked threshold provided an indirect measure of the BM response to the signal, as follows. Consistent with physiological studies, it was assumed that the BM responds linearly to frequencies well below the characteristic frequency (CF). Thus the ratio of the slopes of the masking functions between a masker at the signal frequency and a masker well below the signal frequency should provide an estimate of BM compression at CF. Results obtained from normally hearing listeners were in quantitative agreement with physiological estimates of BM compression. Furthermore, differences between normally hearing listeners and listeners with cochlear hearing impairment were consistent with the physiological effects of damage to the cochlea. The results support the hypothesis that BM nonlinearity governs the nonlinear growth of the upward spread of masking, and suggest that this technique provides a straightforward method for estimating BM nonlinearity in humans. 相似文献
3.
Some masking effects of synthetic vowels on adjacent noise bursts are reported for hearing-impaired listeners. The 200-ms vowels were similar to [a] and [i]; the 50-ms noise bursts were in one of three frequency bands: .5 to 1.5, 1.5 to 4, or 4 to 6 kHz. With voiceless-stop-like temporal intervals between the stimuli, there was little backward or forward masking of the noise bursts by either vowel. Some forward masking occurred under proximate conditions of vowel and burst in time and frequency. 相似文献
4.
Listeners with sensorineural hearing impairment typically exhibit auditory processing deficits such as reduced frequency and/or temporal resolution. Such deficits may represent separate sequela of auditory pathology or may result directly from the sensitivity loss and the requirement to listen at high levels. To assess the impact of increased thresholds on frequency resolution, auditory filter characteristics were determined for hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners at 500 and 2000 Hz in the presence of continuous broadband noise meant as a rough simulation of hearing loss. In the fitting procedure, the low-frequency skirt of the derived auditory filter was allowed to vary as a function of signal level, permitting different filter shapes to be estimated at high versus low signal levels. Listeners with moderate hearing losses at 2000 Hz demonstrated near-normal auditory filter shapes for lower signal levels, but increasingly broad and asymmetric filters as signal level was raised. At 500 Hz, where hearing losses were mild, filter bandwidths increased little at the higher signal levels. The presence of broadband noise had essentially no effect on filter shapes of either listener group. The filter shape abnormalities demonstrated by listeners with moderate hearing loss, which were not observed in normal-hearing listeners at the same signal levels, indicate that poor frequency resolution in these patients for high-intensity stimuli does not follow directly from decreased sensitivity, but instead reflects an independent pathology. 相似文献
5.
6.
Bannerman D. M.; Deacon R. M. J.; Seeburg P. H.; Rawlins J. N. P. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2003,117(4):866
Acquisition and reversal of a spatial discrimination were assessed in an appetitive, elevated plus-maze task in 4 groups of mice: knockout mice lacking the AMPA receptor subunit GluR-A (GluR1), wild-type controls, mice with cytotoxic hippocampal lesions, and controls that had undergone sham surgery. In agreement with previous studies using tasks such as the water maze, GluR-A-/- mice were unimpaired during acquisition of the spatial discrimination task, whereas performance in the hippocampal group remained at chance levels. In contrast to their performance during acquisition, the GluR-A-/- mice displayed a mild deficit during reversal of the spatial discrimination and were profoundly impaired during discrete trial, rewarded-alternation testing on the elevated T maze. The latter result suggests a short-term. flexible spatial working memory impairment in GluR-A-/- mice, which might also underlie their mild deficit during spatial reversal. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
10.
Rats with medial prefrontal cortical lesions were tested in a modified water maze navigation task. In Stage 1, the rats were trained to locate a hidden platform from a single start location. They were then subjected to a series of trials during which a second start position was used (Stage 2). In Stage 3, the rats had to navigate to a new goal location from the 2 experienced start positions. Stage 4 required the rats to navigate to the same goal as in Stage 3, starting from 4 distinct positions. Finally, a single probe trial with no platform was conducted. Rats with prefrontal lesions were impaired only during Stage 4. This deficit was specific to the 2 start positions newly introduced during this stage, suggesting a dysfunction of planning processes. This impairment might result from a working memory deficit, precluding the animal from forming an adequate representation of the whole course of movements required to reach the platform. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
11.
The perceived lateral position of narrow-band noise (NBN) was studied in a graphic pointer task as a function of the method of compensation for interaural threshold asymmetries in hearing-impaired and normal-hearing subjects. The method of compensation consisted of equal sensation level (EqSL) or equal sound-pressure level (EqSPL) at the two ears within the same subject. The NBN signals were presented at 11 center frequencies with interaural intensity differences (IIDs) that varied from -20 to +20 dB. When equalizing by SL, the perceived lateral position is essentially linearly dependent on the degree and direction of asymmetry in asymmetric normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. Equalizing by SPL shows no such dependency but produces images that are lateralized close to the midline. These results reveal that subjects may have adapted to their threshold asymmetries. These results will be discussed in terms of the fitting of binaural hearing aids. 相似文献
12.
The spatial distribution of outer hair cells that participate in generating transient-EOE frequency-components has been investigated in man. According to several models (e.g. Wilson (1990) Hear. Res. 2, 527-532; Zwicker (1986) J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 80, 154-162; Wilson and Kemp (Eds.), Cochlear Mechanisms, Structures, Functions and Models, Plenum Press, NY), EOEs result from interferences between broadly distributed contributions, responsible for their long frequency-dependent delay. This work presents an analysis of the temporal patterns of click- and tone-burst-EOEs in human ears when contributions to EOEs are reduced by noise-induced lesions with audiometric notches centred around 4 kHz (N = 46). Although the auditory thresholds at the frequencies of the studied EOE-components were always normal, these components exhibited drastic and predictable changes compared to normal control ears (N = 40). (1) Their temporal pattern at the highest EOE frequency fmax just below the audiometric notch appeared to be determined by the cochlear state at high frequencies (6 to 8 kHz). Either it was normal and the EOE exhibited a complicated beat-structure, or it was impaired and the time envelope of the EOE was simple. In contrast, any type of time pattern could be observed in normal ears. (2) The temporal patterns of EOEs one octave below fmax always presented many beats and short delays. The proposed interpretation is that contributions to a transient-EOE component at frequency f can come from distant basal cochlear areas, i.e. more than 1 and sometimes 1.5 octaves from the place tuned to f. Therefore, the possible relationships between transient-EOEs and tuning mechanisms which presumably involve only a small number of OHC need further investigations. 相似文献
13.
The pharmacokinetics of 3-hydroxymethyl-7-[2-(4-pyridyl-thio)acetamido]-2-cephem-2-carboxylic acid acetate (cefapirin) was evaluated after 1.0 g i.m. to 14 patients with different glomerular and tubular functions. Peaks in normal renal function ranged between 9--17 micrograms/ml and was 3--4 times higher in patients with glomerular filtration rates below 30 ml/min. The serum half-life in normal renal function was 0.8 h and was somewhat increased in renal impairment. The distribution volume was little influenced by renal capacity. Recommendations for dosage are given. 相似文献
14.
Detection thresholds for either 500-Hz tones or 4-kHz tones were measured for a group of 19 listeners utilizing the interaural configurations NoSo and NoS pi. Both broadband (100-8500 Hz) noises and narrow-band (50-Hz-wide) noises served as maskers. In addition, direct measures of the listeners' sensitivities to changes in interaural temporal differences (ITDs) and interaural intensitive differences (IIDs) were measured using 400-Hz-wide noises centered at 500 Hz or 4 kHz. A rather large range of inter-individual differences in threshold was observed for 4-kHz tonal signals masked by narrow-band noise in the NoS pi configuration. This result is consistent with several sets of data from our previous experiments conducted over more than a decade. A broad range of thresholds was also obtained for 500-Hz tonal signals masked by narrow-band noise in the NoS pi configuration. This outcome, coupled with the fact that the use of a broadband masker did not yield a large distribution of thresholds for the detection of a 500-Hz tone masked by a broad band of noise, suggests that it is the use of a narrow-band masker, per se, that results in a large range of thresholds. Statistical analyses revealed that thresholds in the NoS pi detection tasks were not highly correlated with thresholds measured in the ITD- and the IID-discrimination tasks. Nevertheless, the five listeners who were the most sensitive in the narrow-band NoS pi detection and the five listeners who were the least sensitive in the narrow-band NoS pi detection tasks were those who were the most and least sensitive, respectively, to changes in ITDs and to changes in IIDs. 相似文献
15.
Using gratings with and without spatial noise we measured r.m.s. contrast sensitivity as a function of (i) retinal illuminance, (ii) amount of light added onto the screen, and (iii) exposure time. Our experiments showed that contrast sensitivity in external spatial noise was independent of decreasing retinal illuminance and exposure time as long as contrast sensitivity was lower with noise than without. Thereafter the decrease of contrast sensitivity was identical with and without noise. Although contrast sensitivity without external spatial noise was independent of the amount of added light, contrast sensitivity in external spatial noise was found to increase with the amount of added light until it reached the sensitivity measured without noise. Thereafter contrast sensitivities with and without noise were identical. Our results (i) provide experimental evidence for the validity of the generally accepted hypothesis that in the human contrast detection mechanism signal-to-noise ratio is constant at threshold and (ii) indicate that external spatial noise is the principal source of noise when it reduces contrast sensitivity. 相似文献
16.
M Bj?rklund J Sirvi? J Puoliv?li J Sallinen P J?k?l? M Scheinin BK Kobilka P Riekkinen 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1998,54(3):569-576
Blood flow in response to bradykinin (BK, B2 receptor agonist) and desArg9 BK (B1 receptor agonist) was measured by laser Doppler flowmetry, as a reversal of noradrenaline (50 nmol)-induced decreased blood flow, in the synovium of the anaesthetised rabbit. Either a pretreatment (-6 h) of the cytokines IL-1beta (10 pmol) plus TNFalpha (10 pmol) or saline was injected intra-articularly. BK increased blood flow irrespective of pretreatment, whereas desArg9BK increased blood flow only in the cytokine-pretreated joints. The B2 antagonist HOE 140 reversed (p < 0.01) only the BK responses, and the B1 antagonist desArg9Leu8BK only reversed desArg9BK responses (p < 0.001). A nitric oxide synthase inhibitor, (L-NAME, 10 micromol kg(-1)), reversed the effects of the kinins (p < 0.05), but not sodium nitroprusside-stimulated responses. The results suggest that the B2 receptor is constitutively expressed and that the B1 receptor can mediate responses in inflamed tissues. The results, in addition, indicate that the responses, mediated via both receptors, are nitric oxide-dependent. 相似文献
17.
Sentences were processed through simulations of cochlear-implant signal processors with 6, 8, 12, 16, and 20 channels and were presented to normal-hearing listeners at +2 db S/N and at -2 db S/N. The signal-processing operations included bandpass filtering, rectification, and smoothing of the signal in each band, estimation of the rms energy of the signal in each band (computed every 4 ms), and generation of sinusoids with frequencies equal to the center frequencies of the bands and amplitudes equal to the rms levels in each band. The sinusoids were summed and presented to listeners for identification. At issue was the number of channels necessary to reach maximum performance on tests of sentence understanding. At +2 dB S/N, the performance maximum was reached with 12 channels of stimulation. At -2 dB S/N, the performance maximum was reached with 20 channels of stimulation. These results, in combination with the outcome that in quiet, asymptotic performance is reached with five channels of stimulation, demonstrate that more channels are needed in noise than in quiet to reach a high level of sentence understanding and that, as the S/N becomes poorer, more channels are needed to achieve a given level of performance. 相似文献
18.
Serum digoxin levels were examined in 55 patients with varying degrees of renal function impairment who were receiving chronic oral digoxin therapy. Three methods of predicting digoxin serum levels were investigated. Each method may be applied using serum creatinine values and does not require urinary data. Correlations between calculated and actual digoxin levels in combined male and female patients were improved when changes in digoxin distribution volumes in renal impairment were considered. Correlations between calculated and actual digoxin levels were poor in male patients but were again improved by incorporating changes in drug distribution volume. Correlations obtained in female patients were superior to those obtained in male patients and appeared to be independent of the method of calculation employed. 相似文献
19.
20.
Normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners were tested to determine F0 difference limens for synthetic tokens of 5 steady-state vowels. The same stimuli were then used in a concurrent-vowel labeling task with the F0 difference between concurrent vowels ranging between 0 and 4 semitones. Finally, speech recognition was tested for synthetic sentences in the presence of a competing synthetic voice with the same, a higher, or a lower F0. Normal-hearing listeners and hearing-impaired listeners with small F0-discrimination (deltaF0) thresholds showed improvements in vowel labeling when there were differences in F0 between vowels on the concurrent-vowel task. Impaired listeners with high deltaF0 thresholds did not benefit from F0 differences between vowels. At the group level, normal-hearing listeners benefited more than hearing-impaired listeners from F0 differences between competing signals on both the concurrent-vowel and sentence tasks. However, for individual listeners, deltaF0 thresholds and improvements in concurrent-vowel labeling based on F0 differences were only weakly associated with F0-based improvements in performance on the sentence task. For both the concurrent-vowel and sentence tasks, there was evidence that the ability to benefit from F0 differences between competing signals decreases with age. 相似文献