首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Tested a multiple resources approach to time-sharing performance which assumes that each cerebral hemisphere controls its own set of processing resources that it cannot share with the other hemisphere. Right-handed men performed a verbal memory task while concurrently tapping the index finger of either hand as rapidly as possible. Task priority was manipulated with a payoff scheme. Ss remembered more on the verbal task when concurrently tapping with their left hands than when tapping with their right hands, and their memory performance was much better when the memory task was emphasized than when the tapping task was emphasized, regardless of hand. For the tapping task, decrements from baseline tapping rates and trade-offs between tasks were equal for both hands when Ss were reading the to-be-remembered words aloud. In contrast, during the retention interval, decrements were larger for the right hand than the left, and there were no task trade-offs. On right-hand trials, both tasks required exclusively left-hemisphere resources, whereas on left-hand trials, right-hemisphere resources were required to execute the tapping movements per se, but left-hemisphere resources were necessary to coordinate those movements with the movements required for overt speech. The data underscore the importance of manipulating task priority to obtain an accurate picture of a task's resource requirements. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
1. We used conventional gradient echo magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) at high field strength (4 Tesla) to functionally image the right motor cortex in six normal human subjects during the performance of a sequence of self-paced thumb to digit oppositions with the left hand (contralateral task), the right hand (ipsilateral task), and both hands (bilateral task). 2. A localized increase in activity in the lateral motor cortex was observed in all subjects during the task. The area of activation was similar in the contralateral and bilateral tasks but 20 times smaller in the ipsilateral task. The intensity of activation was 2.3 times greater in the contralateral than the ipsilateral task.  相似文献   

3.
We used fMRI to compare the ipsilateral activation in the sensorimotor region (SMR) during dominant and non-dominant hand motor tasks between right and left handers. In right handers, the ipsilateral activation was significantly greater during non-dominant (left) hand task than dominant (right) hand task, while in left handers, it showed no significant difference. The ipsilateral activation was most pronounced in the precentral subregion (presumably corresponding to the premotor area) during either hand task in both groups. We conclude that the different patterns of ipsilateral activation might be mainly explained by the hemispheric dominance. The skill of the hand and complexity of tasks may be related to the predominant activation of the premotor area.  相似文献   

4.
Investigated right-hemispheric specialization for tactual processing in 96 2–5 yr old right-handed children. Cross-modal transfer from touch to vision was assessed under conditions where Ss palpated shapes with either their left or right hand while music was simultaneously played to the left ear, right ear, or neither ear. This task pitted music against palpation such that both tasks involved the same or different cerebral hemispheres (if music and haptic perception are both lateralized in the right hemisphere as is thought). Results show that in the absence of music, Ss at each age showed a left-hand (right-hemisphere) advantage. The adultlike patterns of ipsilateral interference from music were evident among 4- and 5-yr-olds, in that music to the left ear selectively disrupted left-hand performance. The adultlike pattern was absent at ages 2 and 3 yrs, and music to the right ear disrupted left-hand performance at these ages. Music had an overall, nonselective interference on right-hand performance at all ages. The early appearing left-hand advantage and the lateralized nature of the interference observed among older Ss support the idea of a right-hemispheric specialization for tactual processing of form in young children. (29 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
In a recent article MacNeilage, Studdert-Kennedy, and Lindblom (1987) proposed that nonhuman primate handedness may be contingent on the specific task requirements with visual–spatial tasks yielding left-hand preferences and fine motor tasks producing right-hand preferences. This study reports hand preferences in the manipulation of joysticks by 2 rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and 3 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Reach data were collected on these same subjects and served as a basis for comparison with preference data for manipulation of the joystick. The data indicated that all 5 subjects demonstrated significant right-hand preferences in manipulating the joystick. In contrast, no significant hand preferences were found for the reach data. Reaction time (RT) data also indicated that the right could perform a perceptual–motor task better than the left hand in all 5 subjects. Overall, the data indicate that reach tasks may not be sensitive enough measures to produce reliable hand preferences, whereas tasks that assess fine motor control produce significant hand preferences. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Women show menstrual phase-related cognitive changes that suggest altered hemispheric activation for a particular task, such that they demonstrate the greatest lateral performance differences on prototypical left hemisphere tasks during the luteal phase and on prototypical right hemisphere tasks during menstruation. Additionally, menstrual phase may alter total cerebral responsiveness, such that response times and performance accuracy for many tasks are best during the luteal phase and most impaired during the menstrual phase. We evaluated the effect of menstrual phase on spatial bisection (a perceptuospatial task) to help further understand hormonally-mediated changes in interhemispheric dynamics. Healthy young adult women and men blindly pointed to their midsagittal plane with either hand. Women were repeatedly tested according to menstrual phase, and men were tested at similar intervals. The mean pointing error in the luteal phase differed significantly from that of all other phases and did not differ significantly from those of men, who pointed significantly to the left across test sessions. These findings suggest that, in space bisection tasks, women are more likely to have asymmetric hemispheric activation during the luteal phase than during the menstrual phase. Thus, space bisection did not resemble other prototypical right hemisphere behaviors. The luteal phase may have nonspecifically activated both hemispheres on this task instead of suppressing right hemisphere function, and a slight functional asymmetry favoring the right hemisphere may have been promoted. In addition, intermanual pointing discrepancies in both subject groups decreased over repeated sessions. This suggests that, while practice alters an internal kinesthetic reference, it does not influence an imaginal extrapersonal spatial reference.  相似文献   

7.
Subjects with partial or complete defects of the corpus callosum, either congenital or acquired, performed a choice reaction time (RT) task involving a right or left key-press response to a light presented at random in the right or left hemifield. Like normal subjects, all of them exhibited two additive effects typical of these tasks: the spatial stimulus-response compatibility effect (faster RT for stimuli and responses matched for side), and the hand placement effect (longer RT for responses performed with crossed hands). Two subjects with a complete callosal defect, one acquired and the other congenital, showed a third effect, not present in normal subjects, consisting of a marked advantage for RT of responses with hand anatomically ipsilateral to the stimulus, independent of both stimulus-response compatibility and hand placement. These findings can be interpreted according to a hierarchical model of information processing assuming that, in the absence of the corpus callosum, the matching of the mental codes for the stimulus and response sets takes place solely in the hemisphere receiving the stimulus, with a subsequent rapid-intrahemispheric or slow-interhemispheric transmission of the response command to the appropriate motor centers.  相似文献   

8.
BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Functional recovery after cerebral infarction is a complex phenomenon that depends on various factors. The aim of this study was to investigate changes in cerebral perfusion during motor activity in stroke patients with very early recovery of motor function. METHODS: We included 9 consecutive patients hospitalized for acute-onset hemiparesis who showed complete functional recovery within 24 hours. CT of the brain showed an ischemic or hemorrhagic cerebral lesion in areas compatible with the symptomatology. Within 36 hours (range, 28 to 36) all patients were examined for the effects of a thumb-to-finger opposition task on cerebral blood flow in the middle cerebral arteries, evaluated by means of bilateral transcranial Doppler ultrasonography. Data were compared with those of 9 healthy subjects matched for age and sex. In patients, the evaluation was repeated 2 to 4 months later. RESULTS: A comparable increase in flow velocity (% mean+/-SD) was observed with respect to baseline in the contralateral middle cerebral artery during motor activity with patients' normal (8.8+/-2.0%) and recovered hand (9.7+/-4.1%) and with both hands of control subjects (10.6+/-1.4%). In the middle cerebral artery ipsilateral to the hand performing the motor task, the increase in flow velocity was significantly higher (P<0.0001) during movement of the recovered hand in patients (8.6+/-2.7%) than during movement of the normal hand in both patients (2.6+/-1.6%) and control subjects (1.4+/-0.7%). In patients, pattern of changes in flow velocity during motor performance remained the same in the second evaluation. CONCLUSIONS: These observations suggest that areas of the healthy hemisphere can be activated soon after a focal injury and contribute to the positive evolution of a functional deficit in some patients. This phenomenon of ipsilateral activation cannot be considered transient because it is evident months after stroke onset.  相似文献   

9.
Right-handed subjects (N?=?120) participated in four different laterality tasks designed to measure aspects of cerebral hemisphere asymmetry: identification of dichotically presented consonant-vowel syllables (CVs), examination of the effects of concurrent repetition of CVs and concurrent anagram solution on finger-tapping by the right and left hands, lateralized identification of CVs presented tachistoscopically to the left and right visual fields, and left/right biases on a free-vision face task involving judgments of emotion. Ear differences in the dichotic listening task were related to the pattern of lateralized interference in the dual-task finger-tapping paradigm. There were no other significant relations between pairs of tasks, but when the present results are considered in the light of other recent experiments, there appears to be a relation between lateral bias on the free-vision face task and visual field differences in tachistoscopic identification. The pattern of results has implications for hypothesized individual differences among right-handers in cerebral dominance for verbal processes, input pathway dominance, and asymmetric arousal of the two cerebral hemispheres. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) with a right-hand preference displayed shorter latencies to enter a novel room containing novel structures and objects, touched more objects, and performed more touches and more parallax movements than marmosets with a left-hand preference. These results are consistent with specialization of the right hemisphere (left hand) for fear and negative emotional states and specialization of the left hemisphere (right hand) for approach and positive emotional states. There were no effects of age or sex on any of these behaviors. This relationship between exploration and hand preference did not occur when the marmosets were tested in the home cage with a novel problem, although significant effects of both age and dominance were found in solving the problem. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Two experiments were conducted to explore the interaction of the two cerebral hemispheres in motor control, by examining hand, space and attentional asymmetries in goal-directed aiming. In Experiment 1, right-handed subjects moved to targets more quickly with their right hand than their left hand. In addition, each hand was faster when moving in its own hemispace. Although in a control condition, movements were initiated more quickly with the left hand, visual distractors disrupted left hand performance more than right hand performance. For contralateral aiming, ipsilateral distractors caused the greatest interference. In Experiment 2, when targets and distractors were all presented at the midline, a right hand advantage was found for movement time along with a left hand advantage for reaction time, independent of target and distractor location. Our findings are discussed in terms of a right hemisphere role in movement preparation and the allocation of attention in space, and greater left hemisphere involvement in movement execution.  相似文献   

12.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the cortical motor areas activated in relation to unilateral complex hand movements of either hand, and the motor area related to motor skill learning. Regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) was measured in eight right-handed healthy male volunteers using positron emission tomography during a two-ball-rotation task using the right hand, the same task using the left hand and two control tasks. In the two-ball-rotation tasks, subjects were required to rotate the same two iron balls either with the right or left hand. In the control task, they were required to hold two balls in each hand without movement. The primary motor area, premotor area and cerebellum were activated bilaterally with each unilateral hand movement. In contrast, the supplementary motor area proper was activated only by contralateral hand movements. In addition, we found a positive correlation between the rCBF to the premotor area and the degree of improvement in skill during motor task training. The results indicate that complex hand movements are organized bilaterally in the primary motor areas, premotor areas and cerebellum, that functional asymmetry in the motor cortices is not evident during complex finger movements, and that the premotor area may play an important role in motor skill learning.  相似文献   

13.
Limb preference during conversational gestures may be a reflection of functional hemispheric asymmetries. In right-handers, speech and praxis are usually mediated by the left hemisphere and in conversation, right-handers gesture more with their right than left hand. However, patients with left hemisphere brain damage, who are aphasic and apraxic but not hemiplegic, may use their right hemisphere to compensate for their left. Therefore, we investigated spontaneous lateralized gesture production during conversation in a group of left hemisphere-damaged stroke patients, who were aphasic and apraxic but not hemiparetic, and compared their performance to a group of matched controls. Whereas the control group had a strong right-hand preference, the nonparetic but apraxic and aphasic stroke patients were as likely to produce gestures with the right, left, or both hands.  相似文献   

14.
Studied the effects of age and handedness on the ability of blind children to discriminate Braille characters unimanually and to perform unimanual motor tasks. Human subjects: 48 male and female school-age children and adolescents (aged 6–14 yrs) (blindness). Each S was submitted to 2 series of unimanual tasks designed to measure the ability to (1) discriminate and recognize Braille characters unimanually (haptic efficiency) and (2) perform motor tasks involving tapping ability and grip-strength (motor efficiency). The time spent and number of errors in performing each task were evaluated according to the Ss' age, use of right or left hand, or use of both hands. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) and other statistical tests were used. (English abstract) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Examined whether young children show any evidence of right hemispheric specialization for tactual processing. 72 1-, 2-, and 3-yr-old right-handed children were each administered 6 cross-modal tasks in which they palpated a shape with either their left or right hand for 25 sec and then viewed the familiar and a novel shape in a 10-sec test of visual recognition. Although Ss of all 3 ages showed significantly more visual fixation to novel shapes, regardless of which hand had been used for palpation, scores were enhanced among 2- and 3-yr-olds following palpation with the left as compared with the right hand. This left-hand (right hemisphere) superiority was not evident in 1-yr-olds. These results are the first to demonstrate a left-hand superiority for information processing in children as young as 2 yrs and to suggest that this adultlike pattern is developed by at least the 2nd year of life. Left-hand superiority may be due to asymmetries in tactual sensitivity, motor control, and/or information processing. (40 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
An extensive literature credits the right hemisphere with dominance for processing emotion. Conflicting literature finds left hemisphere dominance for positive emotions. This conflict may be resolved by attending to processing stage. A divided output (bimanual) reaction time paradigm in which response hand was varied for emotion (angry; happy) in Experiments 1 and 2 and for gender (male; female) in Experiment 3 focused on response to emotion rather than perception. In Experiments 1 and 2, reaction time was shorter when right-hand responses indicated a happy face and left-hand responses an angry face, as compared to reversed assignment. This dissociation did not obtain with incidental emotion (Experiment 3). Results support the view that response preparation to positive emotional stimuli is left lateralized. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
A 75-year-old right-handed woman, after a probable cerebral infarct, developed an irregular constriction of the visual fields, a left-sided agraphia, and an anomia for objects in the left hand. Subsequent testing demonstrated an inability to name, though ability to recognize, letters and objects flashed in the homonymous left visual field. An inter-hemispheric disconnexion syndrome was inferred from these findings. The present publication concerns mainly the visual aspects of this disconnexion syndrome. Tasks were devised to test the abilities of the major and minor hemisphere: (a) the left hemisphere demonstrated a complete dominance for language expression and an incomplete dominance for written language comprehension; (b) the right hemisphere appeared to be dominant for some visuo-spatial tasks including number comprehension; (c) when the hemispheres were given contradictory visual informations on a non-verbal task (chimeric stimuli) there was a predominance of the right hemisphere. The right hemisphere appeared able to process complex information. Specialization of functional activities in each hemisphere is briefly discussed.  相似文献   

18.
12 male and 12 female right-handed undergraduates were videotaped while they assembled blocks to perform a series of verbal and nonverbal tasks and a neutral (nonlateralized) task. Analysis of the videotapes revealed that the frequency of movement of one hand relative to the other changed systematically with the cognitive nature of the task, but only for movements playing a functional role in task performance. For the majority of such movements, verbal tasks elicited a greater proportion of right-hand use than did a neutral task, while nonverbal tasks elicited a greater proportion of left-hand use than did a neutral task. These shifts may have reflected the engagement of lateralized problem-solving systems within the 2 hemispheres. (French abstract) (33 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
64 1st-, 3rd, and 5th-grade children and adults (16 in each group) performed a tactile shape-discrimination task. On each of 40 trials, after exploring a randomly generated nonsense form using only finger motion, Ss judged a tactually presented comparison form as either the same as or different from the 1st stimulus. In Condition 1, a single stimulus form and the comparison form were successively presented to the same hand. In Condition 2, stimulus forms were simultaneously presented to both hands; however, a comparison form was presented to only one hand. Results indicate that the left hand (right hemisphere) was more accurate than the right hand (left hemisphere) for 5th-grade Ss and adults, but no significant differences between hands were found for 1st- and 3rd-grade Ss. Overall, Condition 2 was more difficult than Condition 1, but similar laterality effects were found in both groups. Sex differences were found only in the adult group in which males made somewhat fewer errors than females. These results suggest that the right hemisphere becomes progressively more specialized for tactile spatial ability with increasing age. (29 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
We report a 42-year-old left-handed woman with congenital right hemiparesis and bilateral mirror movements in the hands. She had a porencephaly of the left hemisphere and the brain MRI demonstrated cortical and subcortical defect of the left hemisphere from Brodmann's area 6 to 40 including the left motor cortex. By electrical stimulation of the left median nerve at the wrist, N20 of the somatosensory evoked potential was recorded in the right postcentral gyrus by using the dipole tracing method. Long-loop reflexes from the bilateral thenar muscles were recorded and their latencies were almost the same. The stimulation of the right median nerve did not evoke N20, nor long-loop reflex. These electrophysiological findings suggest that the reorganization of the motor system made the right motor cortex to innervate bilateral hands, and caused bilateral mirror movements. In other words, the mirror movements managed to relieve the paralysis of the right hand though the damage of the left motor cortex was present. In the previous literature we are able to find hypotheses regarding the mechanism of mirror movements in congenital hemiparesis. Here we discussed about the reorganization of the motor system in the damaged brain.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号