首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 10 毫秒
1.
Examined the involvement of presumed gustatory thalamocortical projections in conditioned taste aversion (CTA) learning, using 108 male Long-Evans rats in 4 experiments. Neuroanatomical and neurobehavioral manipulations in the ventrolateral neostriatum were used. Findings demonstrate that projections from posterior ventromedial thalamic nuclei, parvicellular division (VPMpc), and thalamus to the anterior insular gustatory neocortex (AIGN) were essential for normal CTA learning. Because both VPMpc thalamus and the AIGN each have been implicated as functional substrates of CTA learning, the present results suggest that the gustatory thalamocortical relay per se is necessary for normal taste-illness learning. (40 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Examined the involvement of the central (CE), lateral (LA), and basolateral (BL) amygdaloid nuclei and the temporal neocortices (area 20) in conditioned taste-aversion (CTA) learning. 40 adult male Long-Evans hooded rats received bilateral electrolytic lesion placements in the CE, LA, BL, or the temporal neocortices. 16 controls received scalp and meningeal incisions only. Following recovery, Ss were habituated to a restricted drinking schedule with distilled water. Animals then received CTA conditioning, with LiCl used both as the CS and as the UCS. Anterograde degeneration histologies were performed on all brain tissue to evaluate relations between CTA learning deficits and axonal pathology induced by lesion placements. Results indicate that destruction of the CE, LA, or temporal neocortex impaired CTA acquisition, but damage induced to the BL amygdaloid nucleus did not. Anatomical observations indicated that degeneration of amygdalofugal and/or corticofugal projections to the convolutions of the olfactory tubercle (medial), subthalamic nucleus, and the parabrachial complex was correlated with CTA learning deficits. Findings suggest that destruction of the dorsolateral amygdaloid nuclei and/or the temporal neocortices may produce CTA learning deficits by affecting olfactory, gustatory, and/or gastrointestinal processing in various portions of the forebrain. (51 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
The functional relation between restricted damage to ventral primary somatosensory neocortex and the ability of rats to acquire conditioned taste aversions (CTA) was examined by a combination of behavioral and neurohistological techniques. Ss were 84 male Long-Evans hooded rats. Lesions confined exclusively to the established gustatory neocortex (GN) did not disrupt CTA acquisition, nor did lesions confined to suprarhinal cortical areas ventral to the GN. Lesions that encroached on dorsal prepiriform and insular cortices produced CTA acquisition deficits and damaged a large proportion of efferent projections to the prefrontal and precentral neocortex. Lesions of dorsal prepiriform and insular cortices did not modify taste preference–aversion thresholds to any of the 4 taste modalities. It is concluded that ventral somatosensory neocortical fields, including the established GN, do not mediate CTA acquisition and that rhinal cortices ventral and posterior to the GN are preferentially involved in associative learning for tastes and illness. (51 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Determined whether the presentation of a LiCl unconditioned stimulus/stimuli (UCS) disrupts within-compound learning in a taste aversion preparation, using 30 male and 32 female rats in 3 experiments. In Exp I, Ss showed stronger associations between 2 solutions presented in a compound when the compound was followed by LiCl. Exp II showed that an immediate LiCl injection produced stronger flavor–flavor association than a delayed injection. Exp III provided a comparison with Ss that did not receive the treatment to enhance consumption of salty solutions. Results indicate that the effects of Exp II depended on the treatment that altered consumption of 1 component. (French abstract) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Lesions in the gustatory zone of the parabrachial nuclei (PBN) severely impair acquisition of a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in rats. To test whether this deficit has a memorial basis, 15 intact rats and 10 rats with PBN lesions (PBNX) received 7 intraoral taste stimulus infusions (30 sec, 0.5 ml) distributed over a 30.5-min period after either LiCl or NaCl injection. This task measures the rapid formation of a CTA and has minimum demands on memory. LiCl-injected intact rats progressively changed their oromotor response profiles from one of ingestion to one of aversion. NaCl-injected intact rats did not change their ingestive pattern of responding. In contrast, there was no difference between LiCl- and NaCl-injected PBNX rats. These same PBNX rats failed to avoid licking the taste stimulus when tested in a different paradigm. A simple impairment in a memorial process is not likely the basis for the CTA deficit. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Sprague-Dawley rats sustaining ablations of gustatory neocortex (GN) at 2, 10, 20, or 60 days of age were compared with control-lesion and anesthetized controls (N?=?151) in the acquisition and extinction of a learned taste aversion. Ss were also tested for taste preference across 5 concentrations of NaCl solution (.04, .08, .15, .3, and .6 M). Results indicate that GN ablation disrupted aversion acquisition and extinction regardless of age at surgery. Taste-response functions for solutions shown by all GN Ss mirrored those of controls: preference (relative to water baseline) for middle concentrations and rejection of the strongest concentration. It is suggested that the 20- and 60-day-old GN Ss were hyperresponsive to the suprathreshold concentrations of NaCl (excepting the .6-M concentration). The increased response to NaCl in 20- and 60-day-old GN Ss may have been related to the significant decreases in water consumption relative to that of controls. Water consumption of controls and GN Ss in 2- and 10-day-olds was essentially equal. It is concluded that infant ablation of the GN does not spare normal taste aversion learning and that rats with GN ablations, regardless of age at surgery, respond in a normal manner to the hedonic aspects of NaCl solutions. (22 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
In 5 experiments, 110 normal male Long-Evans hooded rats and 125 Ss with lesions of the gustatory neocortex (GN) were compared for their ability to learn aversions to taste cues paired with toxicosis. When the taste presentation was followed immediately by toxicosis, normal Ss and 8 Ss with lesions of the posterior (visual) neocortex learned aversions to sucrose, sodium chloride, quinine hydrochloride, and hydrochloric acid solutions. Ss with GN lesions learned aversions to all solutions except sucrose. In preference tests, all solutions were shown to be discriminable from water by both normal and GN-lesioned Ss. Under conditions in which a 6-hr delay separated taste presentation and toxicosis, normals again learned specific aversions to all 4 solutions, but Ss with GN lesions failed to learn specific aversions to sucrose, sodium chloride, and hydrochloric acid solutions. It was shown that the ability of Ss with GN lesions to learn aversions to sucrose and quinine depended on stimulus concentration. It is proposed that the data can be accounted for by postulating a change in the threshold for taste illness associations following GN lesions. (30 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
Rats with bilateral, electrophysiologically guided, ibotenic acid lesions of the gustatory thalamus (THLX) were tested for their ability to perform a variety of taste-guided behaviors. First, in daily 30-min sessions, the rats were given repeated 10-s access periods to a range of concentrations of sucrose, NaCl, or QHCl, plus water. Both the control and the THLX rats exhibited similar concentration-response functions, regardless of hydrational state. Next, on 3 trials, the rats were given 15 min access to 0.3 M l-alanine and then injected with LiCl (0.15 M, 1.33 ml/100 g body weight ip). All rats learned a taste aversion following 1 pairing with LiCl. Finally, on 3 separate occasions, the rats were injected with furosemide, and Na(+)-appetite was evaluated 24 hr later. All rats expressed an equivalent sodium appetite after the first furosemide injection, but only the control rats increased intake of 0.51 M NaCl with repeated sodium depletions. These observations reinforce prior data implying that an intact gustatory thalamus is not necessary for the expression of some taste-guided behaviors.  相似文献   

9.
Temporal parameters of taste aversion learning are known to differ markedly from other learning paradigms in that acquisition occurs despite lengthy delays between exposure to conditioned (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US). Far less consideration has been paid to very brief CS-US intervals and the possibility that this learning may also be distinguished by an ineffectiveness of close temporal contiguity between CS and US. The effectiveness of a very brief CS-US interval (10 s) was compared with that of 2 lengthier intervals (15 and 30 min). Temporal control of CS delivery (0.15% saccharin solution) into the oral cavity and US delivery (7.5 mg/kg apomorphine hydrochloride) into circulation involved infusion pumps and indwelling catheters. Using a 1-trial learning paradigm, CS-US delays of 15 and 30 min led to significant aversions whereas the 10-s CS-US interval did not, suggesting that close temporal contiguity between CS and US is neither necessary nor sufficient for conditioned taste aversion acquisition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Conducted 3 experiments using 611 Sprague-Dawley rats to define and analyze an age-related phenomenon of conditioned taste aversion. When consumption of sucrose solution was followed by LiCl-induced illness in Ss' home, acquisition of the aversion to sucrose solution was retarded in preweanling (18-day-old) Ss. This effect was not found in adults or in 21-day-old Ss. Place of testing had no effect in the younger 2 age-groups, but in adults manifestation of the acquired aversion was retarded when they were tested in the home. There was no interaction between place of conditioning and testing for any age. The locus of the environmental influence on conditioning in preweanling Ss was found to be the place of tasting rather than place of illness, retention interval, or testing. Also, the effect was found to be invariant under minor variations in familiarization of Ss with the nonhome environment. The principle emerging from these data and others is that the home environment can have a significant influence on learning and conditioning in the immature rat. (29 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Examined the relative contributions of the amygdaloid basolateral complex (ABL) and central nucleus (CN) to taste-potentiated odor aversion (TPOA) learning, an associative learning task that is dependent on information processing in 2 sensory modalities. In Exp 1, rats with neurotoxic lesions of these systems were trained on the TPOA task by presenting a compound taste–odor conditioned stimulus (CS), which was followed by LiCl administration. Results showed that ABL damage caused an impairment in potentiated odor aversion learning but no deficit in the conditioned taste aversion. In contrast, rats with CN damage learned both tasks. Exp 2 examined the effects of ABL damage on TPOA and odor discrimination learning. The odor discrimination procedure used a place preference task to demonstrate normal processing of olfactory information. Results indicated that although ABL-lesioned animals were impaired on TPOA, there was no deficit in odor discrimination learning. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Here the authors evaluated the effect of the method of conditioning (bottle or intraoral [IO] infusion) on the strength of a flavor-drug association when measured in a standard 1-bottle consumption test or when measured by IO infusion in a taste reactivity test. When tested with the bottle test in Experiment 1, rats conditioned by bottle displayed stronger taste avoidance than those conditioned by IO infusion. When tested for rejection reactions with the taste reactivity test in Experiment 2, rats conditioned by infusion displayed a stronger aversion than did rats conditioned by bottle. The results suggest that when the contextual cues of conditioning are similar at conditioning and testing, a stronger association is evident regardless of the individual specifics of each method. These results may shed light on recent reports that different neural mechanisms are involved in conditioning by active consumption and passive infusion. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
Conditioned taste aversions (CTA) based on lithium chloride (Experiment 1), amphetamine (Experiment 2), and wheel running (Experiment 3) were examined using the analysis of the microstructure of licking to measure the palatability of the taste serving as the conditioned stimulus (CS). Pairing saccharin with amphetamine reduced saccharin intake without reducing the size of licking clusters, initial lick rate, or the distribution of inter-lick intervals (ILIs) within a cluster. By contrast, pairing saccharin with lithium or wheel-running reduced saccharin intake as well as lick cluster size, initial lick rate, and the distribution of ILIs within a cluster. As lick cluster size, initial lick rate, and ILI distribution can be used as indices of stimulus palatability, the current results indicate that taste aversions based on either lithium or activity reduced the palatability of the CS. This suggests that aversions based on both lithium and wheel running involve conditioned nausea to the CS taste. The absence of similar changes in licking microstructure with amphetamine-based CTA is consistent with other evidence indicating this does not involve nausea. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

14.
The aim of the present experiments was to study the effect of nonselective electrolytic lesions of the rat dorsal hippocampus on 2 learning phenomena: the L. J. Kamin (1969) blocking effect and latent inhibition of taste aversion learning. Bilateral dorsal hippocampal lesions selectively impaired blocking induced by 1 saccharin-lithium chloride pairing previous to 1 serial compound (saccharin–cider vinegar)–lithium pairing, but lesions had no effect on latent inhibition of a saline aversion, induced by 6 saline preexposures, in the same group of animals. Moreover, dorsal hippocampal lesions did not affect latent inhibition of saccharin-conditioned aversion induced by 1 or 6 preexposures. It is argued that blocking and latent inhibition of taste aversion learning do not share a common neural mechanism. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
The psychophysical function in two synesthetic subjects, one with geometrically shaped taste and the other with colored hearing, is investigated. Each synesthete is compared with three control subjects. Pilot studies are used to construct appropriate response domains. Compared to controls, synesthetes give a restricted set of responses that are also asymmetric with respect to the range of choices available in various subsets of the response domain. A combination of absolute and relative effects is also distinctive of synesthetic percepts. It is concluded that synesthesia is a distinct phenomenon unlike ordinary mediated associations, and occupies an intermediate position in the range of simple to complex brain mechanisms.  相似文献   

16.
The cholinergic system is important for learning, memory, and responses to novel stimuli. Exposure to novel, but not familiar, tastes increases extracellular acetylcholine (ACh) levels in insular cortex (IC). To further examine whether cholinergic activation is a critical signal of taste novelty, in these studies carbachol, a direct cholinergic agonist, was infused into IC before conditioned taste aversion (CTA) training with a familiar taste. By mimicking the cholinergic activation generated by novel taste exposure, it was hypothesized that a familiar taste would be treated as novel and therefore a salient target for aversion learning. As predicted, rats infused with the agonist were able to acquire CTAs to familiar saccharin. Effects of carbachol infusion on patterns of neuronal activation during conditioned stimulus–unconditioned stimulus pairing were assessed using Fos-like immunoreactivity (FLI). Familiar taste–illness pairing following carbachol, but not vehicle, induced significant elevations of FLI in amygdala, a region with reciprocal connections to IC that is also important for CTA learning. These results support the view that IC ACh activity provides a critical signal of taste novelty that facilitates CTA acquisition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
Metastatic lesions of the bone marrow (BM) in cancer patients are instrumental in making prognosis and selecting optimal treatment modality. Recent evidence has pointed to insufficient sensitivity of morphological, X-ray and osteoscintigraphic diagnostic procedures to deal with focal alterations in BM. Vistas in application of low-field magnetic resonance tomography for detection and differential diagnosis of localized lesions in BM are discussed.  相似文献   

18.
Heat was found to be effective as a conditional stimulus in the aversion failure procedure (S. Revusky et al; see record 1980-27581-001) and was also found to be effective as an unconditional stimulus using a taste aversion procedure in which rats exposed to high ambient temperature following saccharin consumption showed robust saccharin aversions relative to unpaired and unheated controls. The antisickness and taste aversion conditioning evidence force reexamination of the view that toxic heat effects are referred to the external environment. Together with other recent evidence from this laboratory, these data support the hypothetical antisickness mechanism of aversion failure, which requires that toxic heat serve as an internal stimulus. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
In 3 experiments, a total of 151 female Wistar rats were allowed to consume either sucrose or saline prior to being made ill by injection of either insulin or formalin, or by exposure to X rays. A 2-bottle preference test between sucrose and saline revealed that formalin was an effective agent in conditioned aversions to sucrose but not to saline. Similarly, injections of insulin were effective in producing conditioned aversions to saline but not to sucrose. X-irradiation produced strong aversions to either solution. Results are discussed with regard to the specific need states that insulin and formalin produce. (20 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Used a series of experiments to test the ability of 219 adult Peromyscus mice to form learned taste aversions. It was found that (a) the mice acquired a strong aversion after a single flavor/toxicosis pairing, (b) naive mice drinking a LiCl solution apparently began to experience toxic effects within 90 sec after the beginning of consumption, (c) the mice acquired a total aversion after a single flavor/delayed illness pairing when high doses of toxin were employed, and (d) the aversion produced by a single flavor/delayed-illness pairing was specific to the flavor paired with illness and was dependent on the contingency between the flavor and illness. Although these responses are qualitatively similar to those reported for domestic rats, the mice formed considerably weaker aversions than those previously reported for laboratory rats tested with the same weight-specific doses of LiCl. (33 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号