首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
Rats were infused intraorally with 4 concentrations of ethanol (3%, 6%, 9%, and 12%), and their subsequent oral, facial, and bodily responses were videotaped and analyzed. Naive rats did not display significant changes in ingestive-type responding over the concentrations tested. A significant increase in aversive responses was noted, with the largest number of aversive responses found with the 12% solution. Initial reactivity failed to predict subsequent consumption when rats were given free access to the same alcohol concentrations during 2-bottle tests. Reactivity testing after the period of alcohol access indicated that only the aversive responding changed significantly from the initial reactivity, with rats showing fewer aversive responses. The results indicated how the taste of alcohol is perceived by naive rats and how this perception is changed after consummatory experience with alcohol. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Investigated the ability of animals to form taste aversions following neural manipulations. In Exp 1, 10 rats received intraoral infusions of sucrose every 5 min starting immediately after the injection of LiCl. 12 controls were injected with NaCl. Oromotor and somatic taste reactivity behaviors were videotaped and analyzed. Lithium-injected Ss decreased their ingestive taste reactivity over time; aversive behavior increased. Controls maintained high levels of ingestive responding and demonstrated virtually no aversive behavior following sodium injection. Ss were tested several days later for a conditioned taste aversion (CTA). Rats previously injected with lithium demonstrated significantly more aversive behavior than controls. Exp 3 revealed that when similarly treated rats were tested for a CTA while in a lithium-induced state, difference in the ingestive behavior was observed. In Exp 2, naive rats were injected with NaCl or LiCl but did not receive their 1st sucrose infusion for 20 min. Ss also received infusions at 25 and 30 min postinjection. There were no differences in the task reactivity behavior displayed. Rats dramatically changed their oromotor responses to sucrose during the period following LiCl administration, provided the infusions started immediately after injection, a change attributable to associative processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Alterations in the motivation to ingest sucrose can be quantified by measuring the number and type of oral motor and somatic responses (i.e., taste reactivity [TR]) that are elicited by sucrose. In 2 experiments, rats had intraorally infused sucrose paired with LiCl injections for several trials, or they were injected with LiCl and had sucrose infused every 5 min during the 30-min postinjection period (data from A. C. Spector et al, 1988). In both experiments, ingestive TR responses decreased, whereas aversive TR responses increased over trials. Individual response components that comprise the ingestive and aversive categories followed the same trends of increase or decrease but changed at different rates as a function of number of trials or exposures. Overall, the array of response components could be projected onto a single unidimensional scale of palatability to capture the motivational states that ranged from acceptance to rejection. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Adding sweeteners to ethanol solutions is a common method of inducing rats to consume ethanol. However, it has usually been assumed that it is the sweet taste and/or the calories contained in the sweet solution that controls consumption. The present experiment examined the role of ethanol in controlling responding reinforced by ethanol or an ethanol/sucrose mixture compared with sucrose solutions of various concentrations. After initiation to self-administer 10% (v/v) ethanol using the sucrose-substitution method, rats were trained to respond under a concurrent VI 5" VI 5" schedule. During one condition, responding on one lever was reinforced by the presentation of 10% ethanol, and responding on a second lever was reinforced by water or one of the following sucrose solutions: 1% (w/v), 1.5%, 2%, 2.5%, 3%, and 5%. During a subsequent condition, responding reinforced by a 10% ethanol/2% sucrose mixture was compared under the concurrent schedule with responding reinforced by water, 2%, 2.5%, 3%, 5%, or 10% sucrose (w/v). The results indicated that the ethanol or ethanol/sucrose mixture maintained more responding than did sucrose solutions that were sweeter. Data support the conclusion that, after initiation, the taste and/or pharmacological effects of ethanol had become an important component of the reinforcing stimulus independent of the sweetener.  相似文献   

5.
The rapid acquisition and subsequent retention of lithium-induced conditioned changes in taste reactivity responses to sucrose were examined in rats with the area postrema (AP) either ablated or intact. On 2 conditioning days, a series of brief intraoral sucrose infusions was paired with the effects of LiCl or NaCl injections. Repeated associations of the sucrose taste with the effects of lithium significantly reduced ingestive responses and increased aversive responses only in the AP-intact group. AP-ablated rats treated with LiCl and rats injected with NaCl displayed an ingestive pattern of responses. Only the AP-intact rats, previously injected with LiCl, subsequently displayed evidence of a conditioned taste aversion. We conclude that toxin activation of the AP is required to produce the conditioned shift in taste reactivity responses and subsequent expression of a taste aversion in rats treated with lithium. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
Rats with ablations of the gustatory neocortex (Experiment 1) and rats with olfactory bulb ablations (Experiment 2) were compared with normal rats for aversion generalization to both single taste solutions (sucrose, sodium chloride, quinine hydrochloride, hydrochloric acid) and compound taste solutions (pairs of the four single tastants) following alcohol aversion training. All rats acquired equal and strong alcohol aversions. Control rats showed consistent aversion generalization to both the sucrose plus quinine and the sucrose plus hydrochloric acid solutions; no significant generalization occurred to the single tastants except a weak generalization to sucrose in Experiment 2. Rats with gustatory neocortical ablations failed to show aversion generalization to any of the taste solutions. Rats with olfactory bulbectomies displayed the same aversion generalization functions as control rats but exhibited significantly faster extinction of the alcohol aversion than did the trained control rats. Results from the present experiments suggest that during alcohol aversion learning, rats lacking gustatory neocortex use odor cues (no taste generalization), whereas rats lacking olfactory bulbs utilize taste cues (normal taste generalization). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Conditioned taste avoidances (CTAs) are an important component of behavioral regulation of ingestion. In the laboratory CTAs can be produced by pairing a novel taste stimulus with the physiological feedback produced by a toxin, such as lithium. Such toxins putatively activate a chemosensitive brainstem structure, the area postrema, which ultimately results in the production of a CTA. The present review describes a series of studies which examined conditioned changes in taste reactivity responses (TRRs) when a novel intraoral sucrose taste was paired with the effects of an intraperitoneal (IP) injection of LiCl, and the role of the area postrema in the formation of conditioned palatability shifts. It was first of all necessary to examine the effects of area postrema ablations on TRRs to a range of intraoral sucrose and quinine stimulus intensities. In the first study area postrema lesioned rats exhibited concentration dependent changes in TRRs to these taste stimuli that were very similar to those exhibited by sham lesioned rats. The second study demonstrated that 30 s intraoral infusions of sucrose (0.3 M), presented at 5 or 10 min intervals following an IP injection of LiCl (3.0 meq), resulted in conditioned changes in TRRs. These were characterized by orderly, gradual reductions in ingestive responses and increases in aversive responses. Finally, when area postrema lesioned rats (Study 3) were subjected to this conditioning procedure (brief sucrose presentations paired with the effects of LiCl) no evidence for conditioned or unconditioned changes in TRRs to sucrose were obtained. Lesioned rats injected with LiCl behaved similarly to sham lesioned rats injected with NaCl. These series of studies provide evidence indicating that the chemosensitive area postrema mediates the formation of conditioned palatability shifts induced by treatment with a toxin such as lithium.  相似文献   

8.
Determined, in 3 experiments with 10 Long-Evans rats, that cholecystokinin octapeptide (CCK-8) reduced the amount consumed or the number of licks of several concentrations of sucrose in short-term tests with Ss that were hungry, satiated, or fed ad lib. The suppressive effect of CCK-8 was generally smaller in the hungry than in the satiated or ad-lib condition. In the satiated and ad-lib conditions, there was no effect of sucrose concentration on the magnitude of the CCK-8 effect. In hungry Ss, the concentration-intake function for sucrose was lowered and flattened by both CCK and natural satiety. To determine whether a taste change is involved in this short-term reduction of sucrose intake, integrated chorda tympani responses were measured before and after iv infusions of CCK-8 or NaCl. The peak response to .3 M sucrose significantly increased after 2 5-μg infusions of CCK-8. Peak and tonic responses to sucrose and saline tended to increase after CCK-8 but not NaCl infusions. Results suggest that CCK-8 reduces sucrose intake in situations in which postingestional factors are minimal and that sucrose concentration has little effect on the size of this suppression. A change in perceived taste intensity or quality may play a role in the inhibitory effect of CCK-8 on intake. (33 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
Reports an error in "Taste reactivity responses elicited by cocaine-, phencyclidine-, and methamphetamine-paired sucrose solutions" by Linda A. Parker (Behavioral Neuroscience, 1993[Feb], Vol 107[1], 118-129). Table 1, on page 119, contains two errors. In the first section, the dose/route for the agent nicotine should read as follows: 1.2-2.0 mg/kg sc. In the second section, the dose/route for the agent morphine should read as follows: 2-80 mg/kg ip. Also, on page 121, paragraph 3, line 14, the parenthetical information after 40 mg/kg cocaine should read (40C; 2 × 20 mg/kg/3 cc). (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 1993-24959-001.) The nature of flavor–drug associations produced by a range of doses of the reinforcing agents cocaine (5, 10, 15, 20, or 40 mg/kg, sc), phencyclidine (0.5, 2, 10, or 20 mg/kg, sc), and methamphetamine (2, 5, or 10 mg/kg, ip) were assessed by the taste reactivity (TR) test and the conditioned taste avoidance (CTA) test. Even at the highest doses tested, none of the agents produced aversive TR responding. At doses that produced equivalent-strength CTA, lithium did establish aversive TR responding. Results provide evidence that drugs that serve as reinforcers in other paradigms produce conditioned flavor avoidance that is not motivated by a conditioned dislike for the flavor. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Fragile X syndrome, a form of mental retardation caused by inadequate levels of fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP), is characterized by extreme sensitivity to sensory stimuli and increased behavioral and hormonal reactivity to stressors. Fmr1 knockout mice lack FMRP and exhibit abnormal responses to auditory stimuli. This study sought to determine whether Fmr1 knockout mice on an F1 hybrid background are normal in their response to footshock. Knockout mice were also examined for signs of hyperexcitation across an extended trial range, and serum corticosterone levels were evaluated in response to various stressors. The ability to acquire conditioned taste aversion was also assessed. Knockout mice exhibited no impairment in associative aversive learning or memory, since they successfully expressed conditioned taste aversion. Footshock-sensitivity, freezing behavior, and corticosterone response to various stressors did not differ between knockout and wild-type mice. However, knockout mice exhibited significantly increased responses during the extended test. The knockout mice’s increased responsiveness to footshock in the extended test may be an indication of increased vulnerability to stress or enhanced emotional reactivity. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
In 4 groups of rats, behavioral responsiveness to sucrose was tested by allowing them to lick solutions in a computer-controlled gustometer (10-sec trials; 0.01–2.0 M). Rats with cautery lesions of the nasoincisor ducts (NID) behaved no differently from controls. After bilateral chorda tympani nerve (CT) section, which removes taste input from the anterior tongue (AT), rats demonstrated a marginal attenuation in their responsiveness to sucrose. Combining the 2 lesions, however, had the greatest effect on the concentration–response curve. By shifting the curve to the right and lowering the asymptotic licking rate, the combined lesion reduced the area under the curve by one third. The effects of the combined treatments were larger than would be predicted from the sum of either one alone. This presumably reflects the central convergence of primary afferent axons from the NID and AT. Neurophysiological data have demonstrated such convergence within the nucleus of the solitary tract. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Control rats rapidly learned to avoid drinking either a sucrose solution (Exp 1) or an NaCl solution (Exp 2) when the taste was paired with illness. These rats also produced aversive reactivity to each of these solutions in a taste reactivity test. Rats that lacked gustatory cortex (GC) learned to avoid drinking sucrose and NaCl, albeit at a slower rate than control rats. GC rats failed to display aversive reactivity to these tastes. The GC rats did show normal aversive reactivity to a strong quinine HCl solution during additional tests. It is suggested that the avoidance developed by GC rats did not entail a palatability shift of the conditional stimulus as it did in control rats. This altered learning strategy may account for the consistent learning deficits found in GC rats trained to avoid tastes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

13.
The ability of morphine to modify sucrose palatability was assessed by the taste reactivity test. In Experiment 1, rats were injected with morphine (0.0, 0.5, 2.0, and 10.0 mg/kg, subcutaneously), 30 min before receiving a 10-min intraoral infusion of 2% or 20% sucrose solution. A dose of 2.0 mg/kg morphine enhanced ingestive reactions elicited by both concentrations of sucrose solution. In Experiment 2, the interval between morphine pretreatment and the taste reactivity test was manipulated. Rats given 2.0 mg/kg morphine 30 or 120 min before testing displayed enhanced ingestive reactions elicited by 20% sucrose solution during the first 5 min of a 10-min test. The results support the hypothesis that morphine enhances the hedonic assessment of sucrose solution.  相似文献   

14.
The high comorbidity of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and alcohol dependence (AD) has been firmly established. Although laboratory studies have examined self-reported craving in response to trauma and alcohol cues, no studies have reported on alcohol-related physiological responding in response to trauma cues in PTSD-AD individuals. Using a cue reactivity paradigm, this study examined the impact of personalized trauma-image cues and in vivo alcohol cues on alcohol-related responding (e.g., salivation, craving) in individuals with PTSD and AD (n = 40). Participants displayed reactivity to both trauma and alcohol cues when compared to neutral cues, including increased self-reported craving and distress, as well as greater salivation. These findings suggest that through repeated pairings of trauma memories and alcohol consumption, salivation may become classically conditioned to trauma cues. Moreover, the fact that the trauma-alcohol cue combination elicited greater alcohol craving, salivary responding, distress, and arousal than either the trauma-neutral or neutral-alcohol cue combinations suggests that effects of the trauma and alcohol cues were additive in nature. Evidence that AD individuals with PTSD report increased alcohol craving and display greater salivation in response to trauma memories, supplements prior research indicating that PTSD-related negative emotion and trauma-related alcohol craving may play an important role in the maintenance of AD. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Examined the response of rats to nicotine (NI) solutions with the brief-exposure, taste reactivity (TR) test, and a 2-bottle, 24-hr preference test. Naive nondeprived Ss were administered intraoral infusions of distilled water and 1 μg/ml, 5 μg/ml, 10 μg/ml, 25 μg/ml, 50 μg/ml, and 100 μg/ml NI. NI solutions up to a concentration of 50 μg/ml elicited a number of ingestive TR responses similar to that by water. Ingestive responses significantly decreased and aversive TR responses significantly increased in response to 100 μg/ml nicotine. On the basis of these results, 2-bottle preferences for water vs 1 μg/ml, 5 μg/ml, and 0 μg/ml (water control group) NI were measured in 3 groups of naive Ss. Ss initially showed an equal preference for 0 μg/ml and 1 μg/ml NI. After 16 days of exposure, however, Ss developed a significant preference for 1 μg/ml NI. The preference ratio for 5 μg/ml NI significantly increased during the experiment, but the preference ratio remained significantly less than that for 1 μg/ml and 0 μg/ml NI solutions. Last, TR responses elicited by intraoral infusions of 1 μg/ml and 5 μg/ml NI were then measured in these Ss having had the 2-bottle experience. Ss showing a 2-bottle preference for the 1 μg/ml NI solution displayed significantly more ingestive TR responses to 1 μg/ml and 5 μg/ml NI than did the control Ss. Data indicate that prolonged voluntary access to NI results in an increased preference for NI and modifies the immediate oral/gustatory reactivity of the Ss to NI. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

16.
This study investigated the human eyeblink startle reflex as a measure of alcohol cue reactivity. Alcohol-dependent participants early (n?=?36) and late (n?=?34) in abstinence received presentations of alcohol and water cues. Consistent with previous research, greater salivation and higher ratings of urge to drink occurred in response to the alcohol cues. Differential salivary and urge responding to alcohol versus water cues did not vary as a function of abstinence duration. Of special interest was the finding that startle response magnitudes were relatively elevated to alcohol cues, but only in individuals early in abstinence. Affective ratings of alcohol cues suggested that alcohol cues were perceived as aversive. Methodological and theoretical implications of the findings are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
In 5 experiments, 110 normal male Long-Evans hooded rats and 125 Ss with lesions of the gustatory neocortex (GN) were compared for their ability to learn aversions to taste cues paired with toxicosis. When the taste presentation was followed immediately by toxicosis, normal Ss and 8 Ss with lesions of the posterior (visual) neocortex learned aversions to sucrose, sodium chloride, quinine hydrochloride, and hydrochloric acid solutions. Ss with GN lesions learned aversions to all solutions except sucrose. In preference tests, all solutions were shown to be discriminable from water by both normal and GN-lesioned Ss. Under conditions in which a 6-hr delay separated taste presentation and toxicosis, normals again learned specific aversions to all 4 solutions, but Ss with GN lesions failed to learn specific aversions to sucrose, sodium chloride, and hydrochloric acid solutions. It was shown that the ability of Ss with GN lesions to learn aversions to sucrose and quinine depended on stimulus concentration. It is proposed that the data can be accounted for by postulating a change in the threshold for taste illness associations following GN lesions. (30 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

18.
When rats are forced to drink a morphine solution as their only source of fluid, they eventually reverse their initial preference and drink more morphine than water in a 2-bottle preference test. Two experiments with 13 male Holtzman rats examined the cause of this shift in preference using the taste reactivity test, which involves the analysis of fixed action patterns elicited by taste solutions infused into Ss' mouths. Three morphine concentrations (0.03, 0.6, and 1.5 mg/ml) and 2 levels of motivation (drug-replete and drug-withdrawal states) were studied. A greater percentage of ingestive taste reactivity responses occurred to the oral morphine infusion in morphine-raised than in water-raised Ss. Data are inconsistent with the idea that enhanced morphine ingestion is caused by anticipation of positive consequences. Instead, they support the idea that rats come to "like" the flavor of the morphine solution; in other words, the palatability evaluation of the morphine changes, possibly through an association between the flavor and the hedonically positive effects of the morphine. (33 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Male mice (Mus musculus) from 15 standard inbred strains were exposed to a nearly constant concentration of ethanol (EtOH) vapor for 72 hr, averaging 1.59 ± 0.03 mg EtOH/mL blood at withdrawal. EtOH- and air-exposed groups were tested hourly for handling-induced convulsions for 10 hr and at Hours 24 and 25. Strains differed markedly in the severity of withdrawal (after subtraction of control values), and by design these differences were independent of strain differences in EtOH metabolism. Correlation of strain mean withdrawal severity with other responses to EtOH supported previously reported genetic relationships of high EtOH withdrawal with low drinking, high conditioned taste aversion, low tolerance to EtOH-induced hypothermia, and high stimulated activity after low-dose EtOH. Also supported were the positive genetic correlations among EtOH, barbiturate, and benzodiazepine withdrawal. Sensitivity of naive mice to several chemical convulsant-induced seizures was also correlated with EtOH withdrawal. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Behavioral correlates of changing neurophysiological taste sensitivities during development were assessed with a conditioned taste aversion procedure. Young rats (age 25–30 days) avoided 0.1M monochloride salts and 1.0M sucrose reliably less than adults (age 90–105 days), but the two groups did not differ when the conditioned stimulus/stimuli (CS) was 0.1M citric acid. Analyses of generalization gradients revealed that young rats were unable to discriminate among the tastes of NaCl, NH?Cl, and KCl, whereas adults readily made such discriminations. Both age groups had similar generalization gradients when the CS was 1.0M sucrose or 0.1M citric acid. These data indicate that quantitative and qualitative aspects of salt taste perception alter with age. Furthermore, the behavioral changes noted in the present study correspond closely with previous findings from developmental studies of neuropsychological taste responses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号