首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Three experiments were conducted to ask if conditioned emotional responses (CERs) controlled by contextual cues modulate the acquisition of eyelid conditioned responses (CRs) to discrete conditioned stimuli (CSs). Experiment 1 showed that 30-s auditory stimuli that were paired with aversive shocks to one paraorbital region or the other controlled discriminated CERs, as measured by potentiation of a startle response. In Experiments 2 and 3, similarly trained 30-s stimuli served as contexts in which 1,050-ms CSs were paired with a paraorbital unconditioned stimulus (US). Reinforced contexts both impaired (Experiments 2A and 2B) and facilitated (Experiment 3B) acquisition of the eyeblink CR, depending on the locus of the USs involved. The data are consistent with the interpretation that CERs controlled by contextual cues facilitate CR acquisition, but do so in the face of blocking effects of CR tendencies also conditioned to the contextual cues. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
Electrical stimulation of area 24 and area 32 of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in rabbits elicited increases in respiration rate and decreases in heart rate (HR) and blood pressure. However, stimulation in area 25 elicited pressor responses and a biphasic HR response consisting of an initial HR increase followed by an HR decrease. Administration of an α-adrenergic receptor antagonist eliminated the pressor response and bradycardiac response produced by area 25 stimulation, but it had no effect on the bradycardia elicited by stimulation of area 24 or area 32. Lesions centered on area 32 of the mPFC greatly attenuated the conditioned bradycardiac response elicited by paired tone and paraorbital shock presentations. Lesions of area 24 produced a decrease in discrimination between a reinforced CS and a nonreinforced CS but had no effect on the magnitude of the CR. Area 25 lesions had no effect on any aspect of conditioned responding. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
This study examined the effects of lesions to different neuronal populations within the basal forebrain on reward-related learning. Rats received bilateral alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid (AMPA) or quinolinate lesions that preferentially destroy the cholinergic nucleus basalis magnocellularis (NBM) or noncholinergic ventral pallidal neurons, respectively. Both lesions enhanced conditioned approach responses to stimuli predictive of food but did not increase the locomotor stimulating effect of d-amphetamine. Although both lesions disrupted the discriminative control over behavior by a conditioned stimulus, they did not impair the subsequent acquisition of instrumental responding with conditioned reinforcement (CR). Indeed, both lesions were associated with an increased responding with CR following intra-accumbens infusions of d-amphetamine (0, 1, 3, 10, and 20 microg). Quinolinate lesions also increased responses on an inactive control lever. Neither lesion altered consummatory responses to food or sucrose. Results suggest that NBM lesions may disrupt the balance between cortical and subcortical dopamine levels, and/or produce a deficit in attentional mechanisms that is manifested as increased responding to specific stimuli.  相似文献   

4.
Dual perfusion in vivo brain microdialysis was used to monitor extracellular levels of dopamine in the medial prefrontal cortex and ventral striatum during the acquisition and extinction of a classical aversive conditioning paradigm in rats. The main finding was a dissociation in the pattern of release in the two brain areas. The first stimulus-footshock pairing elicited large increases in cortical dopamine over baseline levels that were much greater than the increases elicited by different stimuli of equivalent salience that were unpaired with footshock. In contrast, dopamine levels in ventral striatum were unchanged under these conditions. Over the next two pairings, there was a decline in the cortical response and an increase in the response in ventral striatum. The first presentation of the aversive conditioned stimulus in a separate context elicited the largest response in ventral striatum. Post-conditioning, the cortical response to the conditioned stimulus was smaller than that elicited by the initial stimulus-footshock pairing and was equivalent in magnitude to that elicited by stimuli unpaired with footshock. Over the final two conditioned stimuli presentations, in the absence of the footshock reinforcer (extinction), responses declined in both brain areas. Simultaneous monitoring of behaviour indicated that the neurochemical events were accompanied by effective aversive learning, as indexed by conditioned freezing responses. The data are discussed in terms of the hypothesis that medial prefrontal cortex is especially engaged during novel circumstances which may, potentially, require new learning, whilst ventral striatal dopamine more closely follows the expression of conditioned responding during learning and extinction.  相似文献   

5.
Four experiments demonstrated discriminated lateralized eyeblink conditioning in 38 male rabbits and showed how the phenomenon may be used to differentiate between the reflexive and emotive consequences of Pavlovian conditioning. Exps 1, 2, and 3 characterized how 2 conditioned stimuli (CSs), contemporaneously trained with left vs right paraorbital unconditioned stimuli (UCSs), can produce different CRs, each involving predominant closure of the eye ipsilateral to its UCS. Exp 4 showed how the associative tendencies controlled by additional stimuli could be evaluated by presentations in compound with such discriminanda: A 30-sec stimulus, presumed to acquire a conditioned emotional response but no eyeblink CR, equally potentiated the eyelid CRs elicited by both CSs. A 1,050-msec CS that evoked an eyeblink CR in isolation also increased the responding to both CSs but biased it toward its own lateralized CR. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
In Exp I, bilateral aspiration of the dorsal hippocampus produced a disruption of blocking of 30 New Zealand rabbits' nictitating membrane response in L. J. Kamin's (1968, 1969) 2-stage paradigm, but had no effect on the formation of a Pavlovian conditioned inhibitor in Exp II (27 Ss). Results of Exp I indicate that normal Ss and those with cortical lesions given conditioning to a light-plus-tone CS gave CRs to both light and tone during nonreinforced presentations of each (test phase). If, however, compound conditioning was preceded by tone acquisition, only the tone elicited a CR during testing; that is, blocking was observed. In Ss with hippocampal lesions, however, CRs were given to both light and tone during testing whether or not compound conditioning was preceded by tone acquisition. Data from Exp II show that Ss with hippocampal lesions could discriminate as well as normal Ss and those with cortical lesions between a light (CS+) and light plus tone (CS-). In addition, when the inhibitory tone was subsequently paired with the UCS in retardation testing, Ss in all 3 lesion conditions acquired the CR at the same rate. Thus, it appears that hippocampal lesions do not disrupt conditioned inhibition. Results are taken as support for the view that the hippocampus is responsible for "tuning out" stimuli that have no adaptive value to the organism. (27 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Eyeblink conditioned response (CR) timing was assessed in adult and infant rats. In Experiment 1, adult rats were trained with a 150-ms tone conditioned stimulus (CS) paired with a periorbital shock unconditioned stimulus (US; presented at 200- or 500-ms interstimulus intervals [ISIs]). The rats acquired CRs with 2 distinct peaks that occurred just before the US onset times. Experiments 2 and 3 examined developmental changes in CR timing in pups trained on Postnatal Days 24-26 or 32-34. Experiment 3 used a delay conditioning procedure in which the tone CS continued throughout the ISIs. Pups of both ages exhibited robust conditioning. However, there were age-related increases in the percentage of double-peaked CRs and in CR timing precision. Ontogenetic changes in eyeblink CR timing may be related to developmental changes in cerebellar cortical or hippocampal function. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

8.
The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) plays a critical role in conditioned autonomic adjustments but is not involved in classically conditioned somatomotor responses unless the training conditions include reversal or trace conditioning. The studies showing these effects have all used pretraining lesions. The present study assessed the effects of posttraining lesions on eyeblink (EB) and heart rate (HR) conditioned responses (CRs) in both delay and trace conditioning paradigms in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Posttraining lesions lowered the percentage of EB CRs during retesting compared with pretesting levels for both delay and trace conditioning. Control lesions and pretraining lesions produced no significant effects during retesting. Posttraining lesions had no effect on the HR CR. These findings suggest that a critical mechanism in the mPFC is involved in retrieval of information during EB conditioning but that the mPFC integration of autonomic and somatomotor processes is not critical to this retrieval process. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

9.
The baroreflex can be classically conditioned. In neuromuscular blocked (NMB) rats, electrical stimulation of the aortic depressor nerve (ADN) and dopamine-produced blood pressure rise were effective unconditioned stimuli (UCS) for auditory discriminative classical conditioning. The conditioned response (CR) pattern (bradycardia, vasodilatation, and hypotension > 10 torr) closely resembled that of the unconditioned baroreflex. Conditioned stimulus (CS) specificity was demonstrated by discrimination of baroreflex-associated and nonassociated auditory stimuli, and also by elaborating depressor and pressor CRs to auditory CSs, which respectively had been associated with either baro-afferent (depressor) or tail-shock (pressor) UCSs. The conditioned baroreflex-magnitude increased with trials. These findings support quantitative models in which CRs interact with and calibrate the gain and dynamic properties of natural reflexes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
Nausea and immune function were assessed in 20 cancer patients in the hospital prior to chemotherapy and compared with assessments conducted at home. Proliferative responses to T-cell mitogens were lower for cells isolated from hospital blood samples than for home samples obtained several days earlier. Patients also experienced increased nausea in the hospital. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses indicated that decreased immune function in the hospital was not related to increased anxiety. The observed anticipatory immune suppression is consistent with the hypothesis that chemotherapy patients may develop conditioned immune suppression as well as conditioned nausea after repeated pairings of hospital stimuli with the emetic and immunosuppressive effects of chemotherapy. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Rats exhibited a marked suppression of motility when they were re-placed in the same environment as that in which they had previously received an electric footshock. We examined the behavioral and neurochemical effects of (+)-N-allylnormetazocine hydrochloride ((+)-SKF-10,047) and (+)-pentazocine, putative sigma 1 receptor ligands, on this psychological-stress-induced motor suppression, defined as a conditioned fear stress. (+)-SKF-10,047 (3 and 6 mg/kg) dose-dependently attenuated the conditioned fear stress, whereas (+)-pentazocine failed to do so even at a higher dose (32 mg/kg). In rats showing the conditioned fear stress, dopamine turnover (i.e., the ratio of dopamine metabolites/dopamine contents) was decreased in the nucleus accumbens and was increased in the medial prefrontal cortex, but remained unchanged in the striatum. (+)-SKF-10,047 (3 and 6 mg/kg) dose-dependently reversed the decreased dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens without changing the increased dopamine turnover in the medial prefrontal cortex. (+)-Pentazocine (32 mg/kg) did not affect the stress-induced changes in dopamine turnover in these brain regions. Thus, the decreased dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens appears to be involved in the conditioned fear stress. These results suggest that (+)-SKF-10,047 ameliorates the conditioned fear stress by reversing the psychological stress-induced dysfunction in the mesolimbic dopaminergic systems, and that the (+)-SKF-10,047-sensitive sub-population of sigma 1 receptors may play in important role in this stress response.  相似文献   

12.
The amygdala has long been thought to be involved in emotional behaviour, and its role in anxiety and conditioned fear has been highlighted. Individual amygdaloid nuclei have been shown to project to various cortical and subcortical regions implicated in affective processing. Here we show that some of these nuclei have separate roles in distinct mechanisms underlying conditioned fear responses. Rats with lesions of the central nucleus exhibited reduction in the suppression of behaviour elicited by a conditioned fear stimulus, but were simultaneously able to direct their actions to avoid further presentations of this aversive stimulus. In contrast, animals with lesions of the basolateral amygdala were unable to avoid the conditioned aversive stimulus by their choice behaviour, but exhibited normal conditioned suppression to this stimulus. This double dissociation demonstrates that distinct neural systems involving separate amygdaloid nuclei mediate different types of conditioned fear behaviour. We suggest that theories of amygdala function should take into account the roles of discrete amygdala subsystems in controlling different components of integrated emotional responses.  相似文献   

13.
Behavioral detection of a low-frequency (40 Hz) vibratory dipole at source distances of 1.5–24 cm was measured by classically conditioned respiratory suppression in goldfish (Carassius auratus). Detection thresholds were compared across distances and before and after ablation of individual octavolateralis sensory channels. Detection thresholds, expressed in units of pressure (SPL), remained roughly constant as distance between the stimulus source and animal increased. Lateral line inactivation, using CoCl2, had no measurable effect on sensitivity, although some other results can be construed as weak evidence for a small contribution of the lateral line to dipole detection when source distances are ≤6 cm (  相似文献   

14.
Avoidance and alerting behaviors and accompanying physiological responses, including changes in heart rate (HR), are core components of negative emotion. Investigations into the neural mechanisms underlying the regulation and integration of these responses require animal models that simultaneously measure both the physiological and behavioral components of emotion. A primate model is of particular importance in view of the well developed prefrontal cortex of primates, and this region’s critical role in emotion regulation and the etiology of affective disorders. Therefore, we have developed a simple aversive conditioning paradigm to assess, simultaneously, cardiovascular and behavioral responses in the marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Validation of the paradigm was achieved by (1) comparing conditioned responses to a predictive cue with pseudoconditioned responses to a nonpredictive cue; (2) assessing the acquisition of conditioning following lesions of the amygdala, a region essential for associative learning in humans and rats; and (3) determining the contributions of the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system to the conditioned autonomic responses. Marmosets acquired conditioned HR and behavioral responses in the conditioned, but not the pseudoconditioned or amygdala lesioned groups. Conditioned HR accelerations were reduced by both parasympathetic and sympathetic blockade. Thus, a model of associative learning of mild negative emotion in the marmoset has been validated by psychological, neurological, and pharmacological investigation. Future studies will determine the role of the prefrontal cortex in the regulation of these negative emotional responses, to provide insights into the neuropathology of affective disorders. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
Two experiments assessed the effects of 1) combined subicular complex and posterior cingulate cortical lesions on training-induced neuronal activity (TIA) in the anterior ventral (AV) and medial dorsal (MD) thalamic nuclei; 2) hippocampal (Ammon's horn and dentate gyrus) lesions on TIA in cingulate cortex and in the AV and MD thalamic nuclei. The rabbits acquired a conditioned avoidance response (CR), stepping in an activity wheel upon hearing a 0.5-s tone (CS+), in order to prevent a foot-shock scheduled 5 s after tone onset. No response was required after a different, safety-predictive tone (CS-). In experiment 1 the combined subicular and cingulate cortical lesions enhanced thalamic TIA during acquisition and increased CR incidence in the first session of acquisition. These results confirmed the hypothesis that subicular and cingulate cortical efferents are not essential for thalamic TIA or for avoidance learning. Hippocampal lesions (experiment 2) also enhanced thalamic TIA. However, unlike subicular lesions, hippocampal lesions enhanced posterior cingulate cortical TIA as well, especially during extinction training. Hippocampal lesions did not affect CR performance. The results suggested that subicular excitatory efferents are responsible for incrementing cingulate cortical TIA, which is viewed as subserving associative attention. Activity from hippocampus downregulates the cue-elicited neuronal activity of the cingulo-thalamic circuits by suppressing the excitatory influence of the subiculum. The hippocampal influence reduces cingulo-thalamic cue-elicited activation in particular circumstances, such as the onset of CR extinction, when an expected reinforcer is omitted.  相似文献   

16.
Several studies in nonhuman primates have shown that neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex have activity that persists throughout the delay period in delayed matching to sample tasks, and age-related changes in the microcolumnar organization of the prefrontal cortex are significantly correlated with age-related declines in cognition. Activity that persists beyond the presentation of a stimulus could mediate working memory processes, and disruption of those processes could account for memory deficits that often accompany the aging process. These potential memory and aging mechanisms are being systematically examined with eyeblink conditioning paradigms in nonprimate mammalian animal models including the rabbit. The trace version of the conditioning paradigm is a particularly good system to explore declarative memory since humans do not acquire trace conditioning if they are unable to become cognitively aware of the association between a conditioning tone and an airpuff to the eye. This conditioning paradigm has been used to show that the hippocampus and cerebellum interact functionally since both conditioned responses and conditioned hippocampal pyramidal neuron activity are abolished following lesions of the cerebellar nuclei and since hippocampal lesions prevent or abolish trace conditioned blinks. However, because there are no direct connections between the hippocampal formation and the cerebellum, and because the hippocampus is not necessary for trace conditioning after a period of consolidation has elapsed, we and others have been examining the prefrontal cortex for its role in forebrain-dependent trace eyeblink conditioning. This review examines some of the literature which suggests that the prefrontal cortex serves to orchestrate a neuronal network that interacts with the cerebellum to mediate adaptively timed conditioned responses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

17.
The utility and performance of optical studies of tissue depends upon the contrast and the changes of contrast in health and disease and in functional activity. The contrast is determined both by the optical properties of extrinsic chromophores and scatterers but especially upon the changes evoked by physiological activity and pathological states. Here, we have focused upon absorption changes of the intrinsic probe, blood absorbance changes due to cortical hypoxia and to haematomas, giving, for particular conditions, absorbance changes of 0.15 and over 0.4 delta OD, respectively. Functional activity may give changes of blood volume of over 0.05 delta OD with some variability due to individual responses that is best expressed as histogram displays of the distribution of response among a significant population. Responses have been observed in prefrontal parietal and occipital functions (242 tests). Extrinsic probes afford signals dependent upon the dose tolerance of the subject and can readily equal or exceed the blood volume and oxygenation signals, and currently afford vascular volume and flow indications. However, contrast agents for the functional activity of cellular function are ultimately to be expected. Finally, light-scattering changes afford osmolyte-related responses and are here shown to indicate a larger signal attributed to cortical depolarization and K+ release in hypoxia/ischaemia. Thus, the optical method affords imaging of manifold contrasts that greatly enhance its specificity and sensitivity for diagnostic procedures.  相似文献   

18.
The present study examined the effects of lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) on the acquisition of a new response with conditioned reinforcement (CR) and its potentiation by intra-accumbens infusions of d-amphetamine (1, 3, 10, and 20 μg/μl). Rats were trained to associate a light-plus-noise compound stimulus with the availability of a sucrose solution before receiving both bilateral ibotenic acid lesions of the CeA and cannulas implanted above the nucleus accumbens. Lesions of the central nucleus did not impair the performance of positively reinforced discriminated approach, nor did they impair the acquisition of a new response with conditioned reinforcement. However, the potentiation of responding with CR following intra-accumbens amphetamine was blocked in lesioned animals. These results are discussed in terms of the possible interactions between associative mechanisms in the amygdala and the mesolimbic dopamine projection. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Elicitation of responses by electrical brain stimulation (EBS) was related to the synaptic distance of the target nucleus from the accessory abducens. Specifically, responses to EBS in the spinal trigeminal nucleus (TRIG) and red nucleus (RN) increased as a positive function of stimulation parameters. Responding to EBS in the interpositus nucleus (IP) was lower, and responding to EBS in the inferior olive (IO) was negligible. EBS in the TRIG, IP, and RN nuclei was then paired with a tone conditioned stimulus (CS). The CS modified responses for EBS in RN and TRIG but not IP. CS-EBS pairings yielded conditioned response (CR) acquisition, in which Groups TRIG, IP, and RN reached asymptotes of 90%, 70%, and 43% CRs, respectively. Thus, contrary to previous findings, EBS in the efferent pathway can support CR acquisition. The results are discussed with respect to the role of projections from the RN to the cerebellar cortex and the TRIG nucleus. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Ran 6 adolescent male rhesus monkeys alternately on classical conditioning and on operant heart rate training schedules. The classical unconditioned stimulus (UCS) was identical to the operant negative reinforcement. After operant training, some Ss changed their heart rate responses to the classical conditioned stimulus (CS). When both the operant and the classical schedules were in force simultaneously, all Ss changed their previous heart rate responses to the classical CS without significantly changing their blood pressure responses to this stimulus. The changes in heart rate response to the CS sometimes persisted long after the operant schedules were no longer in force. These results show that a classically conditioned response can be altered by operant reinforcement, and they suggest that the classical UCS actually may be an operant reinforcer. (29 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号