共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Escanilla Olga; Yuhas Courtney; Marzan David; Linster Christiane 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2009,123(4):828
Olfactory behavioral studies have shown that, when modulated through systemic injections, D1 and D2 receptors have opposing effects on odor discrimination learning. In the present study, cannulated male Sprague–Dawley rats were used to investigate how the modulation of these 2 types of dopaminergic receptors through direct infusion of D1/D2 agonists and antagonists into the olfactory bulb affect olfactory perception. Dopaminergic modulation was locally altered by manipulations of D1 (agonist SKF 82958: 14.6, 43.8, & 143.6 mM; antagonist SCH-23390: 13.4, 40.1, & 60.1 mM) and D2 (agonists quinpirole: 78.2, 117.3, & 156.4 mM; antagonist sulpiride: 0.3, 0.9, & 2.9 mM) receptors during a simultaneous odor discrimination task. The authors found that modulation of D2, but not D1, receptors significantly affected rats’ odor discrimination performance. A significant positive correlation between blockade of D2 receptors and discrimination performance, as well as a significant negative correlation between D2 receptor activation and discrimination performance, was observed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
2.
Chaudhury Dipesh; Manella Laura; Arellanos Adolfo; Escanilla Olga; Cleland Thomas A.; Linster Christiane 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2010,124(4):490
Habituation is a simple form of memory, yet its neurobiological mechanisms are only beginning to be understood in mammals. In the olfactory system, the neural correlates of habituation at a fast experimental timescale involving very short intertrial intervals (tens of seconds) have been shown to depend on synaptic adaptation in olfactory cortex. In contrast, behavioral habituation to odorants on a longer timescale with intertrial intervals of several minutes depends on processes in the olfactory bulb, as demonstrated by pharmacological studies. We here show that behavioral habituation to odorants on this longer timescale has a neuronal activity correlate in the olfactory bulb. Spiking responses of mitral cells in the rat olfactory bulb adapt to, and recover from, repeated odorant stimulation with 5-min intertrial intervals with a time course similar to that of behavioral habituation. Moreover, both the behavioral and neuronal effects of odor habituation require functioning N-methyl-d-aspartic acid receptors in the olfactory bulb. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
3.
Some controversy still exists as to how binary odorant mixtures are behaviorally perceived, despite many studies aimed at understanding this phenomenon. Binary mixture perception by rodents is a first step in elucidating how more complex odor blends may be perceived. Research thus far has examined how the degree of component similarity, olfactory receptor overlap, relative concentration of components, and even olfactory enrichment affect the behavioral perception of binary mixtures. These studies have aimed to categorize binary mixtures into 1 of 3 rigid categories, but often the results conflict as to which category a particular mixture belongs. In the present article, the authors used a habituation/discrimination paradigm to determine whether rats' perception of one component of a binary mixture of either perceptually similar or dissimilar components changed when the concentration of both components was varied together. The authors found that perception of a binary mixture changed with changing component concentration, such that one binary mixture could be categorized differently depending on component intensity. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
4.
Guérin Delphine; Peace Shane T.; Didier Anne; Linster Christiane; Cleland Thomas A. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2008,122(4):816
Noradrenergic projections from the locus coeruleus (LC) project to the olfactory bulb (OB), a cortical structure implicated in odor learning and perceptual differentiation among similar odorants. The authors tested the role of OB noradrenaline (NA) in short-term olfactory memory using an animal model of LC degeneration coupled with intrabulbar infusions of NA. Specifically, the authors lesioned cortical noradrenergic fibers in mice with the noradrenergic neurotoxin N-Ethyl-N-(2-chloroethyl)-2-bromobenzylamine hydrochloride (DSP4) and measured the effects on an olfactory habituation/spontaneous discrimination task. DSP4-treated mice failed to habituate to repeated odor presentations, indicating that they could not remember odors over the 5-min intertrial interval. The authors then infused NA bilaterally into the OBs of both DSP4-treated and nonlesioned control animals at two concentrations (10-3M and 10-5M, 2 μl/side). In DSP4-treated animals, NA administration at either concentration restored normal habituation and spontaneous discrimination performance, indicating that noradrenergic neuromodulation mediates these aspects of perceptual learning and that its efficacy does not require activity-dependent local regulation of NA release. Functional OB learning mechanisms may be necessary for normal odor recognition and differentiation among physically similar odorants. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
5.
Wei Catherine J.; Linster Christiane; Cleland Thomas A. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2006,120(2):393
Dopaminergic modulation affects odor detection thresholds and olfactory discrimination capabilities in rats. The authors show that dopamine D? receptor modulation affects odor discrimination capabilities in a manner similar to the modulation of stimulus intensity. Performance in a simultaneous odor discrimination task was systematically altered by manipulations of both odorant concentration and D? receptor activation (agonist quinpirole, 0.025-0.5 mg/kg; antagonist spiperone, 0.5 mg/kg). Rats' discrimination performance systematically improved at higher odor concentrations. Blockade of D? receptors improved performance equivalent to increasing odor concentration by 2 log units, whereas activation of D? receptors reduced odor discrimination performance in a dose-dependent manner. Bulbar dopamine release may serve a gain control function in the olfactory system, optimizing its sensitivity to changes in the chemosensory environment. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
6.
Yue Esther L.; Cleland Thomas A.; Pavlis Michelle; Linster Christiane 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2004,118(1):184
Dopaminergic modulation of cortical activity has been implicated in the formation of reward associations. There is abundant evidence for dopaminergic effects on olfactory processing. Using an olfactory discrimination task, the authors show that D? and D? dopamine receptors can regulate rats' olfactory discrimination capacities and that the effects of receptor activation functionally oppose one another. Injection of either the D? agonist SKF 38393 (10 mg/kg) or the D? antagonist spiperone (0.62 mg/kg) facilitated the discrimination of similar odorants but had no effect on the discrimination of dissimilar odorants, whereas both the D, antagonist SCH 23390 (0.025 mg/kg) and the D? agonist quinpirole (0.2 mg/kg) significantly impaired rats' ability to discriminate similar and dissimilar odorants. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
7.
Kay Leslie M.; Lowry Catherine A.; Jacobs Heather A. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2003,117(5):1108
Odor mixture perception can be configural (the mixture is qualitatively different from the components) or elemental (the components are recognizable). Some have argued that configural properties are dependent on chemical similarity and possible overlap at the receptor level. The authors show that a binary mixture in which both components activate the same receptor (17) has a configural odor, whereas a mixture that suppresses overlap has elemental odor properties. Rats trained to recognize mixtures of citronellal and octanal (strong 17 agonists) in many ratios rarely recognize the components, supporting configural representation of the odor mixture. However, when trained to recognize mixtures of citral (partial 17 agonist, inhibitor) and octanal, rats recognize 1 or both components over a wide range of ratios. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
8.
Linster Christiane; Garcia Patricia A.; Hasselmo Michael E.; Baxter Mark G. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2001,115(4):826
The neuromodulator acetylcholine is thought to modulate information processing in the olfactory system. The authors used 192 IgG-saporin, a lesioning agent selective for basal forebrain cholinergic neurons, to determine whether selective lesions of cholinergic neurons projecting to the olfactory bulb and cortex affect odor perception in rats. Lesioned and sham-operated rats were tested in an olfactory generalization paradigm with sets of chemically related odorants (n-aliphatic aldehydes, acids, and alcohols). Lesioned rats generalized more between chemically similar odorants but did not differ from controls in their response to chemically unrelated odorants or in acquisition of the conditioned odor. Results show that cholinergic inputs to the olfactory system influence perceptual qualities of odorants and confirm predictions made by computational models of this system. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
9.
The authors used laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus) of known relatedness and contrasting familiarity to assess the potential effect of preexperimental social experience on subsequent social recognition. The authors used the habituation-discrimination technique, which assumes that multiple exposures to a social stimulus (e.g., soiled bedding) ensure a subject discriminates between the habituation stimulus and a novel stimulus when both are introduced simultaneously. The authors observed a strong discrimination if the subjects had different amounts of preexperimental experience with the donors of the 2 stimuli but a weak discrimination if the subjects had either equal amounts of preexperimental experience or no experience with the stimuli. Preexperimental social experience does, therefore, appear to influence decision making in subsequent social discriminations. Implications for recognition and memory research are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
10.
Ss were led to believe that their preferences (among eight pairs of tunes) were similar to those of 1 of 2 other individual's preferences. Subsequently, it was discovered that S tended to show similar preference for other stimuli (nonsense syllables, pairs of girl's names) to those of the individual with whom they initially agreed. The results were interpreted in terms of a cognitive theory of identification and related to the conditions under which projection and introjection might take place. From Psyc Abstracts 36:04:4HJ50S. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
11.
Trimethylthiazoline (TMT), a component of fox feces, has been used in various studies as a natural predator stimulus to induce autonomic and behavioral signs of fear (e.g., higher levels of stress hormones, freezing, and risk assessment). The present study investigated whether 2 further behavioral signs of fear are induced in rats by TMT exposure: potentiation of the acoustic startle response and inhibition of appetitive behavior. In addition, the authors tested the rats for dose dependency of TMT-induced freezing behavior. The study confirmed that behavioral changes observed during TMT exposure are caused by TMT-induced fear and are dose dependent. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
12.
The authors tested the ability of 60 free-flying honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica) to discriminate a conditioning odor from an array of 26 simultaneously presented substances. The stimuli included 10 pairs of enantiomers and 6 essential oils. The bees (a) significantly distinguished between 98% of the 540 odor pairs tested, thus showing an excellent overall discrimination performance, and (b) were able to discriminate between the optical isomers of limonene, α-pinene, β-citronellol, menthol, and carvone but failed to distinguish between the (+)- and (–)- forms of α-terpineol, camphor, rose oxide, fenchone, and 2-butanol. The findings support the assumptions that enantioselective molecular odor receptors may exist only for some volatile enantiomers and that insects and mammals may share common principles of odor quality perception, irrespective of their completely differing repertoires of olfactory receptors. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
13.
Kareken David A.; Mosnik Diane M.; Doty Richard L.; Dzemidzic Mario; Hutchins Gary D. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2003,17(3):482
Aging of cerebral olfactory regions was studied in 5 younger and 6 older healthy adults, matched by odor discrimination and identification scores, with positron emission tomography during odor sensory stimulation, discrimination, and identification tasks. Sensory stimulation engaged bilateral piriform and orbitofrontal regions, but neither discrimination nor identification evoked added temporal or orbital activity. Discrimination involved the hippocampus, implicating its role in serial odor comparisons (olfactory working memory). Left inferior frontal activity during identification may reflect semantic associations. Older participants deactivated the left gyrus rectus/medial orbital gyrus (GR/MOG) during sensory stimulation but activated GR/MOG during discrimination and identification. Adjusting for detection threshold eliminated GR/MOG group differences during sensory stimulation. Diminished threshold may lead to reduced engagement of olfactory association areas. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
14.
Frederick Donald E.; Rojas-Líbano Daniel; Scott Meagen; Kay Leslie M. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2011,125(4):588
To elucidate the cognitive structures of animals, neuroscientists use several behavioral tasks. Therefore, it is imperative to have a firm understanding of each task's behavioral parameters in order to parse out possible task effects. We compare two operant discrimination tasks (Go/No-Go: GNG; Two-Alternative Choice: TAC) that are commonly used in olfactory research. Past research has suggested that solving the two tasks requires divergent cognitive strategies. One hypothesis is that the two tasks differ in how an animal optimizes reward rate by means of a speed–accuracy trade-off (SAT). If this is true, then changing tasks could give researchers an additional tool to understand animal cognition. However, no study has systematically analyzed the two tasks in parallel using odor stimuli. Using standardized training protocols, we test GNG and TAC in parallel. Our protocols allow us to isolate the stimulus sampling period from a general reaction time period. We find that the two tasks do not differ with regard to the stimulus sampling period and conclude that the two tasks do not differ in the amount of time it takes an animal to perform a discrimination. Instead, tasks differ in the time it takes to make an overt behavioral response, with GNG showing shorter periods than TAC. We also find no evidence of rats using either task-specific or intertrial interval-dependent SAT schema in order to optimize reward rate. We show that similarities between dependent variables, with the possible exception of response delay, appear to be under experimenter control. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
15.
The present study assessed whether benzodiazepines impair the acquisition, performance, and reversal of olfactory and auditory go/no-go discriminations in rats. Experiment 1 showed that midazolam (0.5-2 mg/kg sc) did not affect the performance of a well-learned two-odor olfactory discrimination and moderately facilitated performance of a go/no-go auditory discrimination. Experiment 2 found that midazolam (1 mg/kg) increased the number of errors made in the acquisition of a novel go/no-go olfactory discrimination task and in the reversal of a previously well-learned olfactory discrimination. However, midazolam did not affect the acquisition and reversal of an equivalent auditory discrimination task. Experiment 3 showed that diazepam (1 mg/kg) also impaired the acquisition and reversal of a novel olfactory discrimination task. Taken together, these results indicate that benzodiazepines cause a selective impairment of olfactory discrimination learning. This may reflect an effect of benzodiazepines in the glomerular circuitry of the olfactory bulb and at downstream olfactory processing sites such as the piriform cortex and orbitofrontal cortex. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
16.
Rats track self-, conspecific, and artificial odors to locate food. The orbital frontal cortex has been implicated in olfactory behavior, but whether it plays a role in a species-typical behavior, such as odor-guided navigation, has not been studied. Rats were trained to track 1 of 3 different odors deposited on a string. After rats were reliably tracking a scented string, they received a series of 2- and 3-odor discrimination tests. Next, all the rats received bilateral aspiration lesions of the orbital frontal cortex and experienced the same sequence of tasks. Rats learned to track and discriminate between different odors reliably. These results suggest that other areas of the brain mediate odor-guided navigation following damage to the orbital frontal cortex. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
17.
Youngentob Steven L.; Kent Paul F.; Sheehe Paul R.; Molina Juan C.; Spear Norman E.; Youngentob Lisa M. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2007,121(6):1293
Human fetal ethanol exposure is strongly associated with ethanol avidity during adolescence. Evidence that intrauterine olfactory experience influences chemosensory-guided postnatal behaviors suggests that an altered response to ethanol odor resulting from fetal exposure may contribute to later abuse risk. Using behavioral and neurophysiological methods, the authors tested whether ethanol exposure via the dam's diet resulted in an altered responsiveness to ethanol odor in infant and adult rats. Compared with controls, (a) fetal exposure tuned the neurophysiologic response of the olfactory epithelium to ethanol odor at some expense to its responsiveness to other odorants in infantile rats--this effect was absent in adults; (b) the neural effect in infantile rats was paralleled by an altered behavioral response to ethanol odor that was specific to this odorant--this effect was also absent in adults; and (c) a significant component of the infantile behavioral effect was attributable to ethanol's effect on the olfactory neural modality. These data provide evidence for an important relationship between prenatal ethanol experience and postnatal behavioral responsiveness to the drug that is modulated or determined by olfactory function. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
18.
Stevenson Richard J.; Mahmut Mehmet; Sundqvist Nina 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2007,43(1):253
Odor naming and recognition memory are poorer in children than in adults. This study explored whether such differences might result from poorer discriminative ability. Experiment 1 used an oddity test of discrimination with familiar odors on 6-year-olds, 11-year-olds, and adults. Six-year-olds were significantly poorer at discrimination relative to 11-year-olds and adults, who did not differ. Experiment 2 used the same procedure but with hard-to-name visual stimuli and compared only 6-year-olds with adults (as with the remaining experiments in this study). There was no difference in performance between these groups. Experiment 3 used the same procedure as Experiment 1 but with less familiar odors. Six-year-olds were significantly poorer at discrimination than adults. In Experiment 4 the researchers controlled for verbal labeling by using an articulatory suppression task, with the same basic procedure as in Experiment 1. Six-year-old performance was the same as for Experiment 1 and significantly poorer than that of adults. Impoverished olfactory discrimination may underpin performance deficits previously observed in children. These all may result from their lesser experience with odors, relative to adults. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
19.
Mandairon Nathalie; Stack Conor; Kiselycznyk Carly; Linster Christiane 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2006,120(1):173
The authors tested how prior odor enrichment affects the spontaneous discrimination of both preexposed and novel odors. Experimental rats were exposed to single odors or to pairs of similar or dissimilar odors for 1-hr periods twice daily over 20 days. Spontaneous discriminations between pairs of similar odors were tested before and after the odor exposure period using an olfactory habituation task. The authors found that (a) experimental rats did not spontaneously discriminate similar odor pairs before the exposure period, whereas they spontaneously discriminated them after the enrichment period, and (b) the improvement of performance was not selective for the odors used during enrichment. These results show that odor experience changes perception in the manner predicted based on other groups' electrophysiological experiments. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
20.
Infant rats exhibit sensitive-period odor learning characterized by olfactory bulb neural changes and odor preference acquisitions critical for survival. This sensitive period is coincident with low endogenous corticosterone (CORT) levels and stress hyporesponsivity. The authors hypothesized that low corticosterone levels modulate sensitive-period learning. They assessed the effects of manipulating CORT levels by increasing and removing CORT during (Postnatal Day 8) and after (Postnatal Day 12) the sensitive period. Results show that (a) exogenous CORT prematurely ends sensitive-period odor-shock-induced preferences; (b) adrenalectomy developmentally extends the sensitive period as indicated by odor-shock-induced odor-preference learning in older pups, whereas CORT replacement can reinstate fear learning; and (c) CORT manipulation modulates olfactory bulb correlates of sensitive-period odor learning in a manner consistent with behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献