首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Male rats, neonatally treated with ATD (1,4,6-androstatriene-3,17-dione), which blocks the aromatization of testosterone into estradiol (E?), were tested for adult partner preference behavior (PPB; estrous female vs active male). Castration caused a decrease in preference for the female partner in all males, with ATD males showing lower preference for the female partner than controls. Long-term castrated males did not show preference for either partner. Precastration levels of PPB in control males occurred after treatment with E? or dihydrotestosterone (DHT) plus E?. DHT alone had no effect on PPB. With E? alone, the ATD males clearly preferred the male partner. When DHT was added, these ATD males showed no preference for either partner or a low preference for the female partner. In conclusion, adult PPB in male rats is activated by endogenous testosterone or by both its metabolites (DHT and E?) or by E? alone. ATD males showed a much lower preference for the female. There was a differential effect of DHT and E?: DHT had no effect, but E? clearly caused ATD to prefer the male partner and control males to prefer the female partner. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

2.
The role of classical conditioning in the copulatory preferences of male Long-Evans rats (Rattus norvegicus) was examined by pairing a neutral olfactory stimulus (almond odor) with female reproductive status. During training trials, the males were given access to scented or unscented females that were either sexually receptive or unreceptive. Subsequently, copulatory preferences were tested in males given simultaneous access to 2 receptive females, 1 scented and 1 not. Males trained with scented-receptive females displayed an ejaculatory preference for the scented female. Males trained with scented-unreceptive females or with unscented-receptive females displayed an ejaculatory preference for the unscented female. Males displayed no preference when scent and reproductive status were paired randomly. These results demonstrate that classical conditioning produces an ejaculatory preference. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

3.
Paced copulation induces conditioned place preference in female rats. The authors examined whether associating almond-scented males with paced copulation induces conditioned partner preference. The paired group received 4 paced copulations with almond-scented males and 4 nonpaced copulations with unscented males sequentially at 4-day intervals. The unpaired group received the opposite order of association, whereas the randomly paired group received random associations. A 4th group received a single pairing. On the final test, females were placed into an open field with 2 males, 1 scented and 1 unscented. Females in the paired group solicited the scented male more frequently, and most chose the scented male for their 1st ejaculation. Thus, an odor paired with paced copulation elicits conditioned partner preference in female rats. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

4.
Previous studies have demonstrated that central administration of vasopressin but not oxytocin facilitates pair bonding in the monogamous male prairie vole. This study tested vasopressin and oxytocin in the formation of the female vole's preference for a particular male partner. Initial studies showed that in monogamous female prairie voles (but not in nonmonogamous congeners), mating was followed by a partner preference that endured for at least 2 weeks. Nonmating prairie vole females developed a partner preference following oxytocin infusions, but not after vasopressin or cerebrospinal fluid infusions. Females given a selective oxytocin antagonist showed normal mating behavior, yet failed to develop a partner preference. The vasopressin antagonist failed to block partner preference formation in mated females. These results suggest that oxytocin, released with mating, may be critical to formation of a partner preference in the female prairie vole; this contrasts to vasopressin, which appears to be more important for pair bonding in the male of this species. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

5.
Male rats display a conditioned preference to ejaculate with a female bearing an odor paired previously with copulation to ejaculation. The present study examined the role of endogenous opioid and dopamine systems in this preference. Male rats received saline, the opioid receptor antagonist naloxone, or the dopamine receptor antagonist flupenthixol prior to 10 conditioning trials in a pacing chamber with an almond-scented female. On the final test, all males were injected with saline and given access to 2 females, 1 scented and the other unscented, in an open field. Only males injected with naloxone during training failed to manifest a conditioned ejaculatory preference. These findings suggest that activation of opioid, but not dopamine, systems during sexual interaction are necessary for conditioned ejaculatory preference in male rats. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

6.
The effects of the dopamine receptor antagonist flupenthixol were examined on the development of conditioned partner preference induced by paced copulation in female rats. In Experiment 1, ovariectomized, hormone-primed rats were conditioned to associate scented and unscented males with paced and nonpaced copulation, respectively. Females in Experiment 2 associated albino or pigmented males with paced or nonpaced copulation. Flupenthixol or saline was administered before each conditioning trial. During a final drug-free preference test, females could choose to copulate with either a pacing-related or nonpacing-related male. Saline-trained females copulated preferentially with the pacing-related male, whereas flupenthixol disrupted odor but not strain conditioning. The role of dopamine in conditioned partner preference depends on the type of stimuli to be learned. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

7.
Male rats were tested for their sexual preference behavior at either 37, 70, 90, or 150 days of age on three different occasions; while still sexually naive, after sexual experience with a receptive female, and while sexually aroused by having initiated copulation. These tests resulted in the following findings: a) 37-day-old sexually naive males showed a preference for other males and failed to show a preference for either sex after exposure to females; b) 70- and 90-day-old males showed a statistically significant preference for the female after acquiring sexual experience; c) 150-day-old animals showed a female-oriented preference only after being sexually aroused with two intromissions preceding the preference test, and d) none of the age groups tested showed a female-oriented preference without previous exposure to females. It was then concluded that a) female-oriented behavior requires sexual experience and b) the effects of experience varies with age.  相似文献   

8.
An ethanol oral self administration paradigm showed the existence of gender differences in alcohol preference in rats: whereas males and females initiated alcohol drinking at similar rates, females maintained their preference for ethanol over a longer duration. Neonatal estrogenization of females, which effectively confers a male phenotype on a genetically female brain, resulted in patterns of drinking that were similar to those displayed by intact male rats, indicating that gender differences in alcohol drinking patterns may be, at least partially, accounted for by sexual differentiation of the brain. To test whether gonadal steroids also exert activational effects on ethanol-seeking behavior, we also examined the effects of gonadectomy alone, or in combination with gonadal steroid replacement therapy. Castration did not significantly alter ethanol consumption in males, although treatment of castrated rats with dihydrotestosterone resulted in a significant inhibition of this parameter. As compared with the situation in intact female rats, ethanol ingestion was significantly reduced in ovariectomized female rats receiving estradiol (E2) and in ovariectomized female rats receiving combined E2 and progesterone replacement therapy. However, neither ovariectomy nor progesterone replacement in ovariectomized rats resulted in ethanol drinking patterns that were different compared to those observed in intact female controls. Thus, dihydrotestosterone and E2, respectively, appear to exert modulatory influences on the male and female rats' preference for ethanol, but further investigations are necessary to determine to what extent these effects result from activational actions on the brain.  相似文献   

9.
Studied the sexual preference behavior of 32 estrous females of 3 species of hamsters of the genus Mesocricetus by introducing individual females into an arena with a pair of males from 2 different species. When 1 male of the pair was a conspecific, females of all 3 species spent significantly more time investigating the conspecific male. When neither male was a conspecific, female Turkish hamsters (M. brandti) strongly preferred male Romanian hamsters (M. neutoni) to male Syrian hamsters (M. auratus), and female Romanian hamsters preferred male Turkish hamsters to male Syrian hamsters. Female Turkish hamsters displayed significantly more presentation behavior than did Romanian females, and Syrian females rarely presented. Female Turkish hamsters also displayed a stronger degree of preference behavior to a conspecific male than did females of either of the other species. The relatively stronger sexual preferences and greater amount of presentation displayed by Turkish hamsters may relate to the greater opportunity for sympatry with closely related forms and the polymorphic state of this species in the wild. The similarity of Turkish and Romanian hamsters on several characteristics may explain the preference of these species for each other when the alternative was a Syrian hamster. (25 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

10.
The effects of the opioid antagonist naloxone were examined on the development of conditioned partner preference induced by paced copulation in female rats. In Experiment 1, ovariectomized, hormone-primed rats were conditioned to associate scented and unscented male rats with paced and nonpaced copulation, respectively. Female rats in Experiment 2 associated albino or pigmented male rats with paced or nonpaced copulation. Naloxone or saline was administered before each conditioning trial. During a final drug-free preference test, female rats could choose to copulate with either a pacing related or unrelated male. Saline-trained female rats in the paired group copulated preferentially with the pacing-related male rat, whereas naloxone-trained female rats did not show a preference. The authors concluded that opioids mediated the conditioned partner preference induced by paced copulation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

11.
Studied approach behavior toward male and female incentives in male Sprague-Dawley rats during development from prepuberty to adulthood. Both incentives were approached, but the magnitude of the response changed with age. At 70 days of age, the typical adult approach response was evident: the female was contacted for the better part of a session and the male incentive was contacted very little. The time for the appearance of this contact response was not obviously influenced by prepubertal experience in intact males or by the presence of gonadal hormones in neonatally castrated males. However, the magnitude of the preference for the female appeared to be affected by such treatment. (8 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

12.
Male rats received subcutaneously silastic capsules, containing the aromatase inhibitor 1,4,6-androstatriene-3,17-dione (ATD), shortly after birth. Control males were given silastic capsules containing cholesterol. The capsules were removed at the age of 21 days. In adulthood, blood serum was collected early and late in the dark phase of the light/dark cycle (experiment I). Testosterone and luteinizing hormone and follicle stimulating hormone (FSH) fluctuated nocturnally, both in ATD and control males, with highest levels late in the dark phase. FSH levels were significantly higher in ATD males. Nocturnal levels of inhibin, a selective suppressor of pituitary FSH secretion, also fluctuated in both ATD and control males, with lowest levels late in the dark phase. In experiment II, ATD and control males were tested for partner preference behavior in a three-compartment box (choice: sexually active male vs. estrous female) early and late in the dark phase. When gonadally intact, ATD males, but not controls, showed a clear nocturnal rhythmicity in partner preference behavior and sexual behavior. Early in the dark phase, such ATD males preferred the vicinity of and interaction with a sexually active male. Late in the dark phase, this preference for the active male shifted to a preference for the estrous female. Control males preferred the estrous female. After castration and subsequent treatment with testosterone via silastic capsules, which ensured constant blood serum levels, ATD males continued to show their nocturnal rhythms in partner preference behavior and in sexual behavior. Thus, the underlying mechanism of the nocturnal rhythmicity phenomenon is an organizational effect of neonatal ATD treatment rather than an activational effect of fluctuating serum hormone levels.  相似文献   

13.
Investigation and urine-marking responses of 112 male and female Long-Evans rats toward conspecific urine odors were recorded in pair-wise comparison tests. Each of 16 S groups was given 15 preference tests, 1 for each of the possible pairs of 5 urine odors and a no-odor control (N). The urine sources were own group (G), intact male (M), castrated male (Mc), ovariectomized female (Fo), and estrous females (F). Results were scaled by using a technique based on L. L. Thurstone's (1927) law of comparative judgment. Intact males with sexual experience ordered their odor preferences N?  相似文献   

14.
Examined the sexual behavior of 47 female Long-Evans rats with bilateral lesions in the medial preoptic area (MPOA) and 27 Long-Evans sham-operated Ss (controls) in 2 testing conditions. In the 1st condition, in which the S could not leave the vicinity of males (no-exit test), lordosis quotients (LQs) were elevated in relation to baseline levels for MPOA Ss. In the 2nd condition, in which the female could control her proximity to males (exit test), MPOA LQs were not different from control levels, and experimental Ss permitted fewer copulatory contacts, exhibited less frequent solicitational behavior, and spent less time with males than the controls did. These findings suggest that the higher LQs seen in no-exit tests as a result of MPOA damage are not due to a lesion-induced potentiation in the Ss' preference to engage in sexual contacts with males. (27 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

15.
We examined mate switching between mated pairs of monogamous convict cichlids as a function of mate quality (size). A mated pair was established in each half of a 284-litre aquarium, an opaque partition separating the two pairs. When the partition was removed, mis-assorted pairs (large males with small females competing with small males with large females) re-sorted themselves such that the larger male and the larger female paired with each other 46% of the time. In contrast, when we exposed initially assorted pairs to each other, large pairs remained intact most of the time and dominated smaller pairs. The pair containing the large male, whether re-sorted or intact, dominated over the other pair and was the only one seen to spawn. Re-sortment resulted both from a preference of males for larger females and of females for larger males, and from the ability of larger individuals to displace their smaller consexual. Small females, however, when paired with a large male, often dominated large females and prevented the large female from mating with the large male. Re-sortment was also influenced by the compatibility of large individuals in their initial pairing situation. Large individuals that had been more compatible with their initial mates were less likely to switch mates. Our results support both the better-option and the incompatibility hypotheses of mate-switching. The availability of more than one breeding site in the aquarium had no effect on the frequency of re-sortment. Copyright 1998 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Copyright 1998 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.  相似文献   

16.
Sulfotransferases (SULTs) are Phase II drug-metabolizing enzymes that catalyze the addition of a sulfuryl moiety to both endogenous compounds, including steroids and neurotransmitters, and certain xenobiotics, including N-hydroxy-2-acetylaminoflourine and phenolic compounds, like alpha-naphthol. In contrast to certain Phase I drug-metabolizing enzymes, little is known about the regulation of the sulfotransferases. These series of studies were designed to analyze SULT mRNA expression and hormonal regulation in male and female rats. The hepatic expression of six different SULT isoforms was examined including three phenol SULTs and three hydroxysteroid SULTs. SULT mRNA expression was examined in adult and developing rats, as well as, in hypophysectomized (HX) and growth hormone-supplemented HX animals. SULT1A1 is thought to be important for the sulfation of simple phenols and its mRNA expression is about twice as high in adult male as in female rats. This difference in SULT1A1 mRNA levels is largely due to a greater decrease in mRNA levels after puberty in female than in male rats. Hypophysectomy resulted in a decrease in expression of SULT1A1 mRNA in both male and female rats. Replacement of growth hormone (GH) by either intermittent injection (male pattern) or infusion (female pattern) failed to restore SULT1A1 expression. Sulfotransferase SULT1C1 has been implicated in activation of N-hydroxyacetylaminoflourine. In contrast to SULT1A1, SULT1C1 mRNA expression is about 10-fold higher in adult males than in adult female rats. This male-dominant expression pattern emerges at 40-50 days of age and is due to an increase in SULT1C1 mRNA in males. Hypophysectomy abolished SULT1C1 expression in male rats. Interestingly, replacement of GH by injection (male pattern) restored SULT1C1 mRNA expression in males and enhanced SULT1C1 expression in female rats to levels observed in adult male rats. GH infusion (female pattern) did not affect SULT1C1 mRNA expression in either male or female rats. Estrogen sulfotransferase (SULT1E2) may play a role in estrogen homeostasis. Adult male rats express SULTIE2 mRNA at levels 10-fold higher than those observed in adult females and similar to SULT1C1, this is due to an increase in SULT1E2 mRNA occurring during puberty in the male rat. Hypophysectomy did not appreciably affect SULT1E2 expression in male rats, however in contrast to males, hypophysectomy markedly enhanced SULT1E2 expression in female rats. GH infusion suppressed SULT1E2 levels in HX male rats. The expression of hydroxysteroid sulfotransferases was also examined. The SULT-20/21 isoform was expressed in both male and female rats. Male expression of this isoform peaked at 30 days of age and then declined to approximately 30% of the level observed in adult females. SULT-20/21 mRNA expression increased sharply at 45 days of age in female rats and remained elevated. Expression of SULT-20/21 mRNA was decreased markedly by hypophysectomy in both male and female rats. GH injection did not affect SULT-20/21 mRNA expression in HX males, however this treatment resulted in a 4-fold increase in SULT-20/21 mRNA in HX females. GH infusion restored SULT-20/21 expression in HX-male rats. GH infusion did elevate SULT-20/21 mRNA expression in female-HX rats, but not to the level observed in intact females. Hydroxysteroid SULT isoform SULT-40/41 was expressed in adult female but not adult male rats. SULT-40/41 expression peaked at 15 days of age in both male and female rats and decreased thereafter. The decrease in expression was more pronounced in male rats. SULT-60 mRNA, like SULT-40/41, was expressed only in adult female rats. Male rats express SULT-60 at 30 days of age, but SULT-60 mRNA is undetectable at 60 days. SULT-60 mRNA was expressed in females only after day 30 and female SULT-60 mRNA expression remains high thereafter. SULT-40/41 and SULT-60 mRNA expression was increased by HX in male rats and decreased by HX in female rats. (ABSTRACT TRUNCATED)  相似文献   

17.
We investigated the amount of variation in mating behaviour between and within individual male and female American toads, because both sources of trait variation can influence the course of sexual selection. Males varied in all four call parameters investigated (dominant call frequency, pulse rate, call rate and call duration). Individual males lowered the dominant frequency of their call when they interacted vocally with nearby males. Dominant call frequency was more highly correlated with body size in vocally interacting males than in non-interacting males. Pulse rate of calls primarily varied with water temperature. Call rate and call duration showed the most variation of the four call properties, but this variation was unrelated to male morphology or social interactions. Females varied in three aspects of mating behaviour: two measures of pair formation and their preference for dominant frequency of male calls. The body size of paired males varied between females both in pairings initiated by either sex and in pairings initiated only by females. Males chosen by females were usually larger than average, although age and prior breeding experience of females did not affect mate choice. Playback experiments indicated that female preference for calls of low dominant frequency depended on the temporal patterning of alternative calls presented. Each of the four male vocal properties showed significant repeatability, but only one of the three aspects of female mating behaviour was repeatable. We discuss how different degrees of repeatability in sexual traits of males and females may influence the action and detection of sexual selection in this and other species. Copyright 1998 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Copyright 1998 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.  相似文献   

18.
Male rats (Rattus norvegicus) emit at least two patterns of vocalization during copulation, the mating call and the pre-ejaculatory call. Both calls promote immobility of the female during lordosis, but the pre-ejaculatory calls are more effective. We undertook, through ventral observations of the mating pair, to determine if the female failed to assume or maintain the lordosis posture when mounted by a devocalized male and also to determine if the devocalized male was providing adequate stimulation to induce receptive behavior. Females were more likely to move away from the devocalized males before assuming the full lordosis posture. Furthermore, they were more likely to move away before the males had a chance to engage in intromissive behavior. However, when the females remained immobile along enough for the males to achieve a mount or intromission, there was little difference in the behavior of either animal that resulted from the devocalization of the male. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

19.
Sexually dimorphic traits were studied in offspring of rats injected with 33 μg rat β-endorphin (β-END) 3 times daily from Day 14 to Day 21 of pregnancy. β-END males had shorter neonatal anogenital distances than did controls and were more likely to show the female lordosis pattern as adults, but they did not differ in male copulatory behavior. When given a choice between spending time with an estrous female or a male, β-END males showed a lower preference for the female than did control males. The number and somal size of neurons in the bulbocavernosus and dorsolateral nucleus of the lumbar spinal cord were unaffected by drug exposure. Elevated β-END during fetal ontogeny apparently alters the differentiation of some, but not all, sexually dimorphic traits. The data suggest that endogenous opioids may contribute to the etiology of the prenatal stress syndrome. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

20.
Hand preferences were assessed in 51 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes; 28 male and 23 female) ranging from 2 to 5 years of age. Simple reaching served as the measure of lateral bias in hand preference during 2 assessments separated by 1 year. A significant sex by hand preference interaction was found with a greater prevalence of right-handed males than females. No significant differences were found between age and either strength or direction of hand preference. A significant interaction was found between rearing and strength of hand preference. Mother-reared chimpanzees showed significantly greater strength in hand preference than nursery-reared chimpanzees. Finally, a significant positive correlation was found between tests of hand preference conducted over a 1-year interval. These data suggest that in chimpanzees hand preferences are established by 2 years of age and are stable throughout the juvenile developmental period. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号