首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Data were obtained for the colour appearance of unrelated colours under photopic and mesopic conditions. The effects of changes in luminance level and stimulus size were investigated. The method used was magnitude scaling of brightness, colourfulness, and hue. Two stimulus sizes (10° and 0.5°) and four starting luminance levels (60, 5, 1, and 0.1, cd/m2) were used. The results at 0.1 cd/m2 had large variations, so data were obtained for two additional stimulus sizes (1° and 2°) at this luminance level. Ten observers judged 50 unrelated colours. A total of 17,820 estimations were made. The observations were carried out in a completely darkened room, after 20 min adaptation; each test colour was presented on its own. Brightness and colourfulness were found to decrease with decreases of both luminance level and stimulus size. The CAM97u model predicted brightness more accurately than CIECAM02 but gave worse performance in predicting colorfulness. For hue, CAM97u and CIECAM02 both gave satisfactory predictions. Using the brightness correlate from CAM97u, a new colour‐appearance model based on CIECAM02 was developed specifically for unrelated colours under photopic and mesopic conditions, with parameters to allow for the effects of luminance level and stimulus size. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2011;  相似文献   

2.
Color appearance models were developed to characterize the color attributes of stimuli under different viewing conditions based on data collected through magnitude estimation or color matching experiments. Although human beings experience very high light levels under daylight and the reproduction of colors under daylight is important in the color and imaging industries, the existing color appearance models were developed based on the data that were collected under the conditions with luminance levels below 700 cd/m2 due to the lack of facilities to produce stable illumination at high light levels. A recent study investigating color preference of an artwork under a wide range of light levels from 20 to 15 000 lx suggested that CIECAM02 cannot accurately characterize the color appearance under extremely high light levels. This study was designed to directly test the performance of CIECAM02 from 100 to 3500 cd/m2. Human observers performed color match for four hues under a series pairs of adapting conditions with a haploscopic viewing condition. It was found that CIECAM02 had the best performance in characterizing the hue angles but the worse performance in characterizing the brightness with a maximum underprediction around 200% across a wide range of luminance. This was mainly due to the fact that CIECAM02 was developed based on the data collected under relatively low adapting luminance levels. The color appearance model that was proposed to use the adapting luminance levels in characterizing the cone compression in the postadaptation process was found to have a much better performance in characterizing the brightness.  相似文献   

3.
Previous research indicated that the peridot's color is dominated by the selective absorption of visible light caused by ferrous ion, the hue angle of which is in an inverse ratio of the concentration of Fe2+. This article focuses on the color effect of peridot under different standard light sources based on the CIE1976 L*a*b* color space system and round RGB diagram system and tries to find the best one for its grading and display. Based on the results of a series of experiments, including electron microprobe analysis, spectrophotometer, UV‐Vis spectrum, standard illumination box, and Munsell neutral color chips, it was suggested that the spectral power distribution and color temperature of a standard light source significantly influence the color of peridot in terms of lightness and chroma, particularly in the hue of peridot. As for color grading and displaying of peridot, standard light source A fails to fit in, and the color of peridot under a fluorescent light source has a higher chroma but a lower hue angle than that under daylight light source. The best choice for grading and displaying peridot is the standard light source D65. It is better to distinguish the hue of peridot when it is calculated by the round RGB diagram system.  相似文献   

4.
In this article, the effect of the spatial and colorimetric attributes of neighboring color on color appearance shift in bicolor striped woven fabrics is investigated. A total of 240 test/neighboring woven color combinations were constructed in four different striped paradigms. Each test color in the combinations was visually assessed by 12 observer panels with the use of the magnitude estimation method estimating the magnitude of perceptual color attributes lightness, colorfulness, and hue. The visual estimates obtained were analyzed statistically by employing correlation and simple regression methods, and, as a result, the following significant neighboring color effects were detected and individually defined: (1) neighboring color's size, lightness, colorfulness, and hue on test color's lightness, (2) neighboring color's colorfulness and hue on test color's colorfulness, and (3) neighboring color's hue on test color's hue. Furthermore, through multiple regression analysis, color appearance models by which the lightness, colorfulness, and hue of bicolor woven fabrics can be predicted were derived. The predictive performance of the models was evaluated by calculating the difference between the visually estimated and the predicted color appearances, using ΔL*, ΔC*, Δh°, and ΔECMC(2:1). Among all the derived models, the model producing the smallest mean error was chosen as a final model, and its great accuracy in color appearance predictions was verified through further statistical evaluation. It is envisaged that the findings of this research are of benefit to design textile products with bicolor striped woven fabrics to have desired color appearances. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 42, 512–521, 2017  相似文献   

5.
As light‐emitting diode (LED) light sources mature, lighting designers will be able to deliver white light with a variety of spectral power distributions and a variety of color rendering properties. This experiment examined the effects of three spectral power distributions (SPDs) that were matched in illuminance and chromaticity on three measures of color perception: one objective (performance on the Farnsworth‐Munsell 100 hue test) and two subjective (judgments of the attractiveness of one's own skin, and preferences for the saturation of printed images). The three SPDs were a quartz‐halogen (QH) lamp and two LED sources that were matched to the QH lamp in terms of both illuminance and chromaticity; the three light sources were nominally CCT = 3500 K, x = 0.40, y = 0.39 and ~ 400 lx. LED A used three channels (red, green, blue), and had very poor color rendering (Ra = 18). LED B used four channels (red, amber, cyan, white) and had very good color rendering (Ra = 96, whereas the QH had Ra = 98). Secondary hypotheses addressed the effects of age and skin and eye color on the dependent measures. As expected, LED A delivered very different color perceptions on all measures when compared to QH; LED B did not differ from QH. The results show that it is possible for LED sources to match the familiar incandescent sources. However, although it is possible to deliver what appear to be millions of colors with a three‐chip (RGB) device, there is the risk of creating a very poor luminous environment. © 2013 National Research Council Canada and Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 39, 263–274, 2014; Published Online 12 April 2013 in Wiley Online Library ( wileyonlinelibrary.com ). DOI 10.1002/col.21811  相似文献   

6.
Environmental laws mandate the protection of visibility conditions in national parks and wilderness areas from atmospheric haze, which occurs due to the emissions of anthropogenic air pollutants. To calculate the improvement in visibility that results from the reduction of these air pollutants, it is necessary to quantify the relationship of haze to the color appearance of objects being viewed through it. To this end, a field study was conducted in the Great Smoky Mountains National Park in eastern Tennessee. Color appearance of objects was quantified by color matching with a special visual colorimeter. The Hunt94 color‐appearance model was used to compare these matches with simultaneous spectral measurements. In addition to the chromaticity coordinates and luminance of the object, the Hunt94 model requires a number of parameters such as the chromaticity of the adapting light and chromatic and brightness surround induction factors, which need to be determined for the outdoor viewing conditions. This article describes how this was accomplished. Although it accounts for differences in viewing conditions between the colorimeter and daylight conditions, the Hunt94 model does not account for the perceptual transparency effect of haze on outdoor color appearance. These results challenge the manner in which current air quality–visibility models are being utilized. © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 24, 112–120, 1999  相似文献   

7.
Unique hue loci of red, yellow, green, and blue were obtained for lights of 0.5 and 2 cd. m-2 from five normal color vision subjects. Most loci curved on the chromaticity diagram. This result excludes linear equations of colormatching functions as candidates for spectral sensitivity functions of opponent color systems, (r/g)λ and (y/b)λ. The chromatic valence curves of two opponent color systems obtained by the so-called cancellation experiment were analyzed in relation to the unique hue loci and it was shown that the appearance of the valence curves can be manipulated at will by choosing the cancellation light at various points on the chromaticity diagram. A similar manipulation was also demonstrated for the additivity property of chromatic valence and it was made clear that no unique solution is obtainable.  相似文献   

8.
A technique of subjective magnitude estimation has been used to assess a set of pseudosurface colours, each having a luminance equal to half that of its adapting surround. Five observers scaled each of the colours three times in each of two adaptation conditions corresponding to daylight and tungsten light at 120 cd/m2. Loci of constant hue and saturation were derived in u', v' chromaticity space for daylight and tungsten light. These grids enable the appearance of a colour to be specified from a knowledge of its chromaticity coordinates and they enable such effects as colour constancy and colour fidelity to be investigated in terms of perceived colour appearance.  相似文献   

9.
The chromaticities of the Munsell Renotation Dataset were applied to eight color‐appearance models. Models used were: CIELAB, Hunt, Nayatani, RLAB, LLAB, CIECAM97s, ZLAB, and IPT. Models were used to predict three appearance correlates of lightness, chroma, and hue. Model output of these appearance correlates were evaluated for their uniformity, in light of the constant perceptual nature of the Munsell Renotation data. Some background is provided on the experimental derivation of the Renotation Data, including the specific tasks performed by observers to evaluate a sample hue leaf for chroma uniformity. No particular model excelled at all metrics. In general, as might be expected, models derived from the Munsell System performed well. However, this was not universally the case, and some results, such as hue spacing and linearity, show interesting similarities between all models regardless of their derivation. © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 25, 132–144, 2000  相似文献   

10.
Most color preference research focuses on colors in an object color mode. In our daily life, however, colors are perceived not only as an object color mode but also as other modes, such as unnatural object color and light source color modes. To explore the effect of the color appearance mode on color preference, we examined the relationship between color preference and the mode of color appearance. Thirty‐three color chips were chosen from the Munsell notation varying in hues and chromas. The color chips were presented in different color appearance modes by changing the subject's room illuminance and the color chip room illuminance. The experimental results showed that the brightest and most saturated colors were preferred. It was found that the subject preferred color in a light source color mode and unnatural object color mode to color in an object color mode. Moreover, we found that hue had a small effect on color preference in the light source color mode. We also investigated the relationship between color preference and the perceived color attributes (perceived chromaticness, whiteness, and blackness). In a supplementary experiment, elementary color naming was conducted. The results showed that the perceived chromaticness, perceived whiteness, and perceived blackness play a role for the determination of color preference for different color appearance modes. We, consequently, suggest that color preference is dominated not only by color attributes but also by the mode of color appearance. © 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2010  相似文献   

11.
A colorimetrically characterized computer-controlled CRT display was used to determine 24 loci of constant perceived hue for pseudo-object related stimuli, sampling the display's interior color gamut at constant lightness and the edge of its gamut at variable lightness. Nine observers performed three replications generating matching data at 132 positions. the constant hue loci were used to evaluate the correlation between perceived hue and hue angle of CIELAB, CIELUV, Hunt, and Nayatani color appearance spaces. the CIELAB, CIELUV, and Hunt spaces exhibited large errors in the region of the blue CRT primary, while the Nayatani and CIELUV spaces produced large errors in the region of the red primary for constant lightness stimuli. Along the edge of the CRT's color gamut (variable lightness stimuli), all the spaces had a similar trend, large errors in the cyan region. the differences in performance between the four spaces were not statistically significant for the constant lightness stimuli. For the variable lightness stimuli, CIELAB and CIELUV had statistically superior performance in comparison with the Nayatani space and equal performance in comparison with the Hunt space. It was concluded that for imaging applications, a new color appearance space needs to be developed that will produce small hue error artifacts when used for gamut mapping along loci of constant hue angle. © 1995 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.  相似文献   

12.
The effect of purity on hue has been reported severally (including Munsell and NCS data on constant hue loci) over 100 years but without general agreement. For example, the number of hue shift nulls in the data vary from 2 to 6. Hence, despite this effect's commonality it lacks reliable data for modeling underlying mechanisms or color appearance. The purity of a stimulus may be decreased by adding white (as in Abney's experiment), by adding black, or by adding gray such that luminance, or alternatively lightness, remains constant. This article gives new data for CRT stimuli for illuminant D65 for all four conditions but mainly for equal luminance, for 31 observers and 13 test dominant wavelengths. Further, samples were observed in two temporal conditions: either simultaneously as pairs (the contrast mode) or singly (the no‐contrast mode). Three types of samples were tested: (1) equal luminance 30 cd/m2 for all dominant wavelengths, (2) equal lightness for all dominant wavelengths, and (3) zero‐gray colors, requiring different luminances for different dominant wavelengths. In all the above conditions, the resultant hue shifts graphed a robust bimodal curve (two peaks in cyan and red, two troughs in blue and green) across the hue cycle, similar to Munsell and NCS data except the definite peak in cyan.© 2006 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 32, 25–39, 2007; Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/col.20286  相似文献   

13.
We fabricated blue polymer light‐emitting diodes (PLEDs) with indium tin oxide (ITO)/PEDOT : PSS/PVK/PFO‐poss/LiF/Al structures. All of the organic film layers were prepared by the spin‐coating method on plasma and heat‐treated ITO/glass substrates. The dependences of the optical and electrical properties of the PLEDs on the plasma and heat treatment of the ITO film and the introduction of poly(N‐vinylcarbazole) (PVK) layer were investigated. The AFM measurements indicated that the surface roughness of the ITO transparent film was improved by the plasma and heat treatment. In the emission spectra, the intensity of the excimer peaks of the PFO‐poss [polyhedral oligomeric silsesquioxane‐terminated poly(9,9‐dioctylfluorene)] emission layer were decreased for the PLED device with the PVK film layer compared with the one without the PVK layer. The maximum current density, luminance and current efficiency of the PLEDs were found to be about 470 mA/cm2, 486 cd/m2 at an input voltage of 12 V and 0.55 cd/A at 100 cd/m2 in luminance, respectively. The color coordinates (CIE chart) of the blue PLEDs were in the range of x = 0.17 ~ 0.20, y = 0.13 ~ 0.16, and the peak emission spectrum was about 430 nm, showing a good blue color. © 2008 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Appl Polym Sci, 2008  相似文献   

14.
The light‐emitting diode (LED)‐based light sources have been widely applied across numerous industries and in everyday practical uses. Recently, the LED‐based light source consisting of red, green and blue LEDs with narrow spectral bands (RGB‐LED) has been a more preferred illumination source than the common white phosphor LED and other traditional broadband light sources because the RGB‐LED can create many types of illumination color. The color rendering index of the RGB‐LED, however, is considerably lower compared to the traditional broadband light sources and the multi‐band LED light source (MB‐LED), which is composed of several LEDs and can accurately simulate daylight illuminants. Considering 3 relatively narrow spectral bands of the RGB‐LED light source, the color constancy, which is referred to as the ability of the human visual system to attenuate influences of illumination color change and hold the perception of a surface color constant, may be worse under the RGB‐LED light source than under the traditional broadband light sources or under the MB‐LED. In this study, we investigated categorical color constancy using a color naming method with real Munsell color chips under illumination changes from neutral to red, green, blue, and yellow illuminations. The neutral and 4 chromatic illuminants were produced by the RGB‐LED light source. A modified use of the color constancy index, which describes a centroid shift of each color category, was introduced to evaluate the color constancy performance. The results revealed that categorical color constancy under the 4 chromatic illuminants held relatively well, except for the red, brown, orange, and yellow color categories under the blue illumination and the orange color category under the yellow illumination. Furthermore, the categorical color constancy under red and green illuminations was better than the categorical color constancy under blue and yellow illuminations. The results indicate that a color constancy mechanism in the visual system functions in color categories when the illuminant emits an insufficient spectrum to render the colors of reflecting surfaces accurately. However, it is not recommended to use the RGB‐LED light source to produce blue and yellow illuminations because of the poor color constancy.  相似文献   

15.
Four data sets are analyzed to quantify three effects of luminance of samples on chromaticity discrimination: on ellipse area, axis dimensions (a and b), and a/b ratio. Ellipses for aperture, surface, and simulated surface colors in CIE 1931 and 1964 x, y, Y color spaces are shown to reduce axis dimensions with higher luminance by different functions for the major and minor axes. Reduction is greater for major than minor axes, thus improving ellipse circularity. The functions plot straight lines in log‐log scale as power law equations, except luminances below 3 cd/m2. We give formulae to predict a and b axes, a/b ratio, and ellipse area for almost any luminance in x, y, Y spaces. Effect of luminance is remarkable on ellipse area, which on average halves with every 3.5 times higher luminance. To illustrate the substantial effects of luminance, RIT‐DuPont ellipses are predicted for three levels of equal luminance at 42, 212, and 2120 cd/m2. In the latter, ellipses are much smaller and are nearer circular than in the former. Higher luminance is known to improve color discrimination, so reduced ellipse area is to be expected but does not occur in CIELAB and DIN99 spaces because of lack of luminance‐level dependency. We discuss our results' implications on uniform color space. Weber fraction ΔY/Y indicates brightness discrimination decreases with increasing luminance and is thus independent of chromaticity discrimination. © 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 30, 186–197, 2005; Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/col.20107  相似文献   

16.
We have measured the chromatic‐discrimination ellipses with different luminances for 66 stimuli distributed throughout the CIE‐1931 chromatic diagram. The distribution of these stimuli enabled us to analyze the influence of luminance on discrimination from clearly photopic levels to levels that could enter the mesopic range. The results show a clear influence of the luminance level on the areas of the chromatic‐discrimination ellipses. These areas remain almost constant when the luminance level is clearly photopic, and clearly increase when the luminance diminishes. However, it is not necessary that the luminance level diminish far below 2–3 cd/m2 to detect a substantial increase in the area of the ellipses. According to our data, within the photopic range appears a transition interval of luminance above which the most pronounced increase in the area of the ellipse would be appreciable. Other parameters characteristic of discrimination ellipses, such as orientation and relation to semi‐axes, vary with the luminance level, although without clearly following any rule. © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 26, 123–131, 2001  相似文献   

17.
In this article, the color appearance of a large (85°) homogeneous self‐luminous visual stimulus was studied in a psychophysical experiment. Large stimuli were displayed on a plasma display panal (PDP) monitor. The large stimuli were viewed with a fixed viewing time (2 s). They were compared with 2° and 10° stimuli presented on a grey background on a CRT monitor. The so‐called “color size effect” was found to be significant. The color stimulus was perceived to be lighter when it was large compared with the 2° and 10° situation. But we did not find the general increase of chroma claimed in previous literature. We found only small hue changes. A model of the color appearance of large‐field stimuli is presented in terms of the CIELAB L*, a*, and b* values of the corresponding 2° and 10° stimuli. © 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 33, 45–54, 2008  相似文献   

18.
Past studies investigating the unique hues only used samples with a relatively high saturation levels under standard illuminants. In this study, 10 observers selected the four samples with unique hues from 40 V6C8 (Value 6 Chroma 8) and 40 V8C4 (Value 8 Chroma 4) Munsell samples under six light sources, comprising three levels of Duv (i.e., 0, ?0.02, and ?0.04) and two levels of correlated color temperature (i.e., 2700 and 3500 K). Significant differences were found between the two chroma levels for unique blue and yellow, with the hue angles of unique yellow and blue judged using the desaturated samples being significantly different from those defined in CIECAM02. The iso‐lines of unique yellow, blue, and green did not always go through the origin of the a*‐b* or a′‐b′ planes in CIELAB and CAM02‐UCS. Thus, the problems of CIECAM02, CIELAB, and CAM02‐UCS identified in this study need further investigations.  相似文献   

19.
Experiments were carried out to investigate the effect of simultaneous contrast on colour appearance by varying the lightness, colourfulness, and hue of an induction field surrounding a test colour. A total of 814 test/surround combinations were displayed on high-resolution colour displays. Each was assessed by a panel of five to six observers using a magnitude estimation technique. the results indicate that colours presented on a computer display are affected by simultaneous contrast in a similar way to surface colours. All three colour appearance parameters studied (i.e., lightness, colourfulness, and hue) are affected and these effects are summarized. In general, the results support and add to the findings of the other studies. the Hunt colour appearance model was tested and gave a somewhat poor prediction to this data set. Further modifications are required to improve its performance. © 1995 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.  相似文献   

20.
Three different structures of multi-layer organic light-emitting devices, which consisted of two emitting layers separated by a carrier blocking layer, were investigated. Since the emitting layers are constructed to emit different colors, the colors emitted from the structures are mixed. It was found that the colors were directly mixed in the structures of this study due to the carrier blocking layer sandwiched by the two emissive layers. The blocking layer splits the carrier recombination zone, and with the emission color is controlled by balancing the split. For the white light the CIE coordinate of (0.30, 0.33) is obtained at an applied voltage of 14 V. The luminance is measured to be 1,000 cd/m2 at 14 V. with the power efficiency of 0.4 lm/W. For a luminance of 100 cd/m2 at 11 V., the CIE coordinate is found to be (0.31, 0.34) and the power efficiency was as high as 0.53 lm/W.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号