首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   221篇
  免费   1篇
电工技术   2篇
化学工业   24篇
金属工艺   3篇
机械仪表   6篇
建筑科学   5篇
矿业工程   1篇
能源动力   1篇
轻工业   5篇
无线电   19篇
一般工业技术   28篇
冶金工业   120篇
自动化技术   8篇
  2020年   2篇
  2017年   1篇
  2016年   1篇
  2014年   2篇
  2013年   3篇
  2012年   2篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   3篇
  2007年   7篇
  2006年   9篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   2篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   18篇
  1997年   7篇
  1996年   17篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   5篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   8篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   8篇
  1975年   5篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   4篇
  1971年   1篇
  1948年   1篇
  1946年   1篇
排序方式: 共有222条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Previous studies have shown that the leukocyte integrin CR3 (CD11b/CD18) is physically associated with the urokinase-type plasminogen activator receptor (uPAR;CD87), a glycosyl-phosphatidylinositol (GPI)-linked protein, in resting neutrophil membranes. We now show that uPAR-to-CR3 interactions are reversible, correlating with cell shape. Neutrophils were first labeled with fluorescein conjugates of anti-CR3 F(ab')2 fragments followed by capping using a second-step F(ab')2 directed against murine F(ab')2s. Cells were then probed using rhodamine-conjugated anti-uPAR F(ab')2s. Although uPAR co-caps with CR3 on resting cells, uPAR was found to dissociate or "uncap" coincident with spontaneous cell polarization for migration. CR3 caps transformed into uropods while uPAR accumulated at lamellipodia of polarized cells. Capping was unnecessary for the observed distribution of CR3 and uPAR since the anti-CR3 and anti-uPAR F(ab')2s traffic to the uropod and lamellipodium, respectively, during polarization of uncapped cells. These receptors reassociate when cells return to a spherical morphology. In contrast to uPAR, Fc gamma RIIIB did not dissociate from CR3 caps during cell polarization. Resonance energy transfer (RET) microscopy was used to image the spatial distribution of RET and to follow the kinetics of association and dissociation. Initial levels of RET dramatically fell during cell polarization, but did not change on cells fixed with paraformaldehyde. Receptor reassociation was a biphasic process with initial reassociation about the perimeter of a cap, followed by a plateau and a slower rise in RET within a cap. We suggest that cells regulate receptor-receptor associations depending upon their physiologic activities.  相似文献   
102.
103.
Hypothesized that conditions in which an oral presentation was given before a written presentation of information would be more effective than if a written presentation preceded an oral one. Ss were 73 trainees in a program for the disadvantaged (mean age, 22.3 yrs). 4 different combinations of oral and written messages were administered to each of 4 groups. The criterion of effectiveness was a multiple choice test which measured Ss' knowledge of the information in the message. 8 a priori comparisons based on the hypothesis were in the expected direction, but only 3 were significant (p  相似文献   
104.
In order to understand how additives influence the structure and electrical properties of active layers in thin‐film devices, a compositionally identical but structurally different guest–host system based on the syn and anti isomers of triethylsilylethynyl anthradithiophene (TES ADT) is systematically explored. The mobility of organic thin‐film transistors (OTFTs) comprising anti TES ADT drops with the addition of only 0.01% of the syn isomer and is pinned at the mobility of OTFTs having pure syn isomer after the addition of only 10% of the isomer. As the syn isomer fraction increases, intermolecular repulsion increases, resulting in a decrease in the unit‐cell density and concomitant disordering of the charge‐transport pathway. This molecular disorder leads to an increase in charge trapping, causing the mobility of OTFTs to drop with increasing syn‐isomer concentration. Since charge transport is sensitive to even minute fractions of molecular disorder, this work emphasizes the importance of prioritizing structural compatibility when choosing material pairs for guest–host systems.  相似文献   
105.
The authors report on a novel method for producing the noncentrosymmetric structures necessary for the observation of the pyroelectric effect. The Langmuir-Blodgett technique has been used to fabricate ultrathin films consisting of alternate layers of 22-tricosenoic acid and 1-docosylamine. The pyroelectric coefficient has been determined using both static and dynamic detection techniques, is approximately 0.2 nC cm(-2) K(-1) (static value); this value is independent of the film thickness in the range of 11 to 99 monolayers, but increases slightly if a substrate of lower thermal expansion coefficient is used. The results indicate that there is a piezoelectrically induced secondary effect contributing to the overall pyroelectric activity.  相似文献   
106.
The present research proposes that sources in the numerical majority (vs. minority) can affect persuasion by influencing the confidence with which people hold their thoughts in response to the persuasive message. Participants received a persuasive message composed of either strong or weak arguments that was presented by a majority or a minority source. Consistent with the self-validation hypothesis, we predicted and found that the majority (vs. minority) status of the source increased the confidence with which recipients held their thoughts. As a consequence, majority (vs. minority) sources increased argument quality effects in persuasion when source status information followed message processing (Experiment 1). In contrast, when the information regarding source status preceded (rather than followed) the persuasive message, it validated the perception of the position advocated, reducing message processing. As a consequence of having more confidence in the position advocated before receiving the message, majority (vs. minority) sources reduced argument quality effects in persuasion (Experiment 2). Finally, Experiment 3 isolated the timing of the source status manipulation, revealing that sources in the numerical majority (vs. minority) can increase or decrease persuasion to strong arguments depending on whether source status is introduced before or after processing the message. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
107.
Attitude theory has long proposed a mechanism through which antecedents of message elaboration produce attitude strength consequences. However, little direct evidence exists for the intervening process. The proposed thoughtfulness heuristic holds that perceiving that more thought has taken place leads to greater attitude certainty. Two roles were established for this heuristic: first as a mediator of the impact of antecedents of elaboration on attitude certainty and second as a way to influence attitude certainty independent of actual elaboration. In Studies 1 and 2, antecedents of elaboration (need for cognition, distraction) impacted attitude certainty because they impacted the actual amount of processing, which in turn affected perceptions of the amount of processing. In Studies 3 and 4, a manipulation of perceived thought impacted certainty independent of actual thought (i.e., after thinking had already occurred). Furthermore, the thoughtfulness heuristic was shown to influence behavioral intentions, establishing perceived amount of processing as both a mediator and an independent cause of attitude strength consequences. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
108.
Gene 3 protein (pIII), a minor coat protein at one end of the filamentous bacteriophage fd, is involved in initiating the infection by the virus of Escherichia coli cells that display an F-pilus. Infection is thought to start with the adsorption of the D2 domain of pIII to the tip of the pilus, retraction of the pilus, and penetration of the E. coli cell membrane mediated by an interaction between the D1 domain of pIII and the Tol protein complex in the membrane. A subgene encoding the pIII-D1D2 di-domain was created, and the subgene was successfully overexpressed in E. coli cells. Domains D1 and D2 were separated after limited proteolysis of a modified pIII-D1D2 (designated pIII-D1D2.trp) into which two tryptic cleavage sites were introduced at appropriate points. The purified pIII-D1D2 di-domain and pIII-D2 domain were able to bind to the F-pilus, competing with the wild-type pIII and delaying infection by the intact filamentous phage. The pIII-D1 domain was unable to bind to the F-pilus by this criterion. This provides conclusive evidence that the pIII-D2 domain is responsible for the adsorption to the tip of the F-pilus and can achieve this in the absence of domain D1, opening the way to identifying the molecular basis of the interaction of pIII-D2 with the pilus.  相似文献   
109.
Three experiments tested the hypothesis that the social roles implied by specific contexts can attenuate or reverse the typical pattern of racial bias obtained on both controlled and automatic evaluation measures. Study 1 assessed evaluations of Black and Asian faces in contexts related to athlete or student roles. Study 2 compared evaluations of Black and White faces in 3 role-related contexts (prisoner, churchgoer, and factory worker). Study 3 manipulated role cues (lawyer or prisoner) within the same prison context. All 3 studies produced significant reversals of racial bias as a function of implied role on measures of both controlled and automatic evaluation. These results support the interpretation that differential evaluations based on Race X Role interactions provide one way that context can moderate both controlled and automatic racial bias. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
110.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号