首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   276篇
  免费   1篇
电工技术   2篇
化学工业   41篇
金属工艺   3篇
机械仪表   10篇
建筑科学   4篇
能源动力   9篇
轻工业   9篇
无线电   17篇
一般工业技术   27篇
冶金工业   125篇
原子能技术   2篇
自动化技术   28篇
  2021年   2篇
  2019年   2篇
  2017年   1篇
  2016年   1篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   3篇
  2013年   12篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   5篇
  2009年   9篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   8篇
  2005年   3篇
  2003年   2篇
  2002年   5篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   39篇
  1997年   27篇
  1996年   17篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   11篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   3篇
  1974年   4篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   3篇
  1970年   3篇
  1966年   1篇
  1965年   1篇
  1958年   2篇
排序方式: 共有277条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
271.
Relations between hormone levels and aggressive behavior of adolescents in family interactions were examined in 30 boys and 30 girls, 9 to 14 years of age. The adolescents represented all five stages of pubertal development (Tanner criteria). Circulating levels of gonadotropins (luteinizing hormone and follicle-stimulating hormone), gonadal steroids (testosterone and estradiol), adrenal androgens (dehydroepiandrosterone, dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate, and androstenedione), and testosterone-estradiol binding globulin were assessed. These hormones are responsible for growth and sexual development during puberty and correlate with Tanner stage. The behavioral measures described the adolescents' expressions of anger and attempts to control or defy their parents and were based on videotaped family interactions during problem-solving tasks. On the basis of multiple regression analyses, higher estradiol and androstenedione levels were associated with higher degrees of aggressive behaviors for girls. Findings were sparse for boys. Findings are discussed in terms of (a) effects of degree of stability of hormone levels on detection of hormone-behavior relations, (b) organizing and activating influences of hormones on behavior, and (c) other mechanisms possibly underlying the relations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
272.
273.
Carbides in the Ta–C system were prepared by hot-pressing mixed powders of TaC and Ta in C/Ta atomic ratios of 1.0, 0.9, 0.8, 0.7, and 0.6 at 1800°C for 2 h. The compositions, 1.0, 0.9, and 0.8, produced single face-centered-cubic (fcc) TaC y phase with C/Ta ratios essentially the same as the starting compositions and densities exceeding 96.5% of theoretical. Grain size decreased with increasing carbon deficiency with a corresponding increase in Vickers hardness from 13.5 to 20 GPa. The flexural strength, however, decreased from 650 to 500 MPa due to a decrease in fracture toughness from 5.3 to 3.8 MPa√m. The compositions 0.7 and 0.6 produced two carbide phases, fcc TaC y and rhombohedral ζ-Ta4C3− x phase in the 0.7 composition and ζ-Ta4C3− x and hexagonal-close-packed (hcp) α-Ta2C phase in the 0.6 composition. Fracture toughness measured by single-edge-precracked-beam technique was exceptionally high (12.7±0.7 MPa√m) for the 0.6 composition with a high weight percent of the ζ-Ta4C3− x phase (∼83 wt%). The fracture surface was highly faceted with multiple cleavage crack planes and steps.  相似文献   
274.
Reports an error in Double-blind photoarray administration as a safeguard against investigator bias by Mark R. Phillips, Bradley D. McAuliff, Margaret Bull Kovera and Brian L. Cutler (Journal of Applied Psychology, 1999[Dec], Vol 84[6], 940-951). On page 947, the graphs within Figure 1 were incorrectly labeled. The corrected figure appears in this erratum. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 1999-15531-009.) This experiment examined whether a photoarray administrator's knowledge of a suspect's identity increased false identification rates. Fifty participant–administrators (PAs) presented 50 participant–witnesses (PWs) two perpetrator-absent photoarrays following a live staged crime involving two perpetrators. For one photoarray per trial, the experimenter revealed the suspect's identity to the PA. Each PA presented the photoarrays sequentially or simultaneously in the presence or absence of an observer. When the observer was present, PA knowledge of the suspect's identity had a biasing effect in sequential photoarrays only. This pattern did not emerge when the observer was absent. The experimental manipulations did not affect PAs' and PWs' ratings of photoarray fairness or PWs' ratings of pressure to make an identification. These data suggest that only administrators who are blind to the suspect's identity should present sequential photoarrays. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
275.
In 5 experiments, listeners heard words and nonwords, some cross-spliced so that they contained acoustic-phonetic mismatches. Performance was worse on mismatching than on matching items. Words cross-spliced with words and words cross-spliced with nonwords produced parallel results. However, in lexical decision and 1 of 3 phonetic decision experiments, performance on nonwords cross-spliced with words was poorer than on nonwords cross-spliced with nonwords. A gating study confirmed that there were misleading coarticulatory cues in the cross-spliced items; a sixth experiment showed that the earlier results were not due to interitem differences in the strength of these cues. Three models of phonetic decision making (the Race model, the TRACE model, and a postlexical model) did not explain the data. A new bottom-up model is outlined that accounts for the findings in terms of lexical involvement at a dedicated decision-making stage. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
276.
In the process of finding a minimal sum representation for an incompletely specified multiple-output switching function, there often occur certain types of prime implicants, referred to as useless, which can be discarded because of the presence of the don't-cares. This paper presents a correction to the definition of useless given by Tison and extends the definition to other notations. A procedure for removing useless prime implicants quickly is presented for the case when multiple-output prime implicants are derived from minterms. The deletion of useless prime implicants can, in many cases, speed up any procedure for finding a minimal sum that begins with the multiple-output prime implicants in both hand calculation and in computer implementation.This work was supported in part by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. MCS-77-09744.  相似文献   
277.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号