首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   880篇
  免费   37篇
电工技术   2篇
化学工业   169篇
金属工艺   11篇
机械仪表   8篇
建筑科学   36篇
矿业工程   9篇
能源动力   27篇
轻工业   146篇
水利工程   10篇
石油天然气   7篇
无线电   45篇
一般工业技术   113篇
冶金工业   255篇
原子能技术   4篇
自动化技术   75篇
  2023年   4篇
  2022年   12篇
  2021年   23篇
  2020年   13篇
  2019年   20篇
  2018年   11篇
  2017年   18篇
  2016年   20篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   27篇
  2013年   44篇
  2012年   32篇
  2011年   49篇
  2010年   35篇
  2009年   57篇
  2008年   47篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   28篇
  2005年   34篇
  2004年   31篇
  2003年   39篇
  2002年   27篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   19篇
  1999年   13篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   20篇
  1996年   12篇
  1995年   14篇
  1994年   14篇
  1993年   20篇
  1992年   16篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   15篇
  1988年   13篇
  1987年   11篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   10篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   9篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   9篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   5篇
  1974年   3篇
  1964年   3篇
排序方式: 共有917条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
81.
82.
Replies to comments by 7 authors (see records 2001-18772-008, 2001-18772-009, 2001-18772-010, 2001-18772-011, and 2001-18772-012) in reference to the F. Rothbaum et al's original article (see record 2000-02781-001), which highlighted evidence of cultural variations in, and noted ways in which attachment theory is laden with Western values and meaning in comparisons of the US and Japan. Here, Rothbaum et al defend their central thesis and note areas of their agreement and disagreement with the aforementioned comments and criticisms. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
83.
Administered the SCL-90-R to 78 adult patients with chronic pain, 52 adult patients with spinal cord injury, and a control group of 145 college students. Estimates of internal consistency, represented by coefficient alpha, were .69 for the Somatic Depression subscales and .91 for the Cognitive Depression subscales. Data suggestive of construct validity included differential correlations of r?=?.78 between the experimental Cognitive Depression scale and the SCL-90-R Obsessive Compulsive scale, and r?=?.66 between the experimental Somatic Depression scale and the SCL-90-R Somatization scale. Similar evidence of divergent validity was not clearly demonstrated with the remaining SCL-90-R subscales. multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) revealed that previously found item response patterning was replicated with the chronic pain and spinal cord injured Ss. Refinement of the research subscales and implications for clinical use among medical/rehabilitation samples are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
84.
Studied the development of lipsmacking and grimacing, facial expressions associated with friendliness and fear, respectively, in 26 rhesus monkeys raised under 3 conditions that included visual exposure to (a) monkeys and people, (b) one monkey, (c) neither monkeys nor people. Ss were tested through Life Weeks 1–22 for responses to their mirror image or a human face. Age, stimulus configuration, and experience interacted in the development of the 2 responses. Lipsmacking generally occurred earlier than grimacing and was most frequently elicited by the face. Frequency of lipsmacking increased initially, then declined; in contrast, frequency of grimacing increased progressively throughout testing. Rearing conditions significantly affected age of first response, level of responsiveness, and stimulus differentiation. The most restricted group was oldest at first response, least responsive, and showed the weakest differentiation of stimuli; the most experienced group was at the opposite extreme on these comparisons. (24 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
85.
Positive- and negative-limited lead-acid cells with conventional and polyaniline (PANI)-coated negative plates were assembled and tested at varying discharge rates. The cells with PANI-coated negative plates exhibit lower impedance in relation to conventional cells and sustain higher discharge-rates with lesser loss in capacity during prolonged charge–discharge cycling as compared to conventional cells. It is suggested that PANI-coated negative plates are beneficial in designing lead-acid batteries operating at partial-state-of-charge.  相似文献   
86.
The copulatory performance of male rats, tested in a large seminaturalistic environment, was assessed to determine the relation between 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations and a range of sociosexual behaviors. The 7 sexually experienced Charles River male rats were tested until sexual exhaustion. Ultrasonic signals were shown to occur in a wider range of sociosexual circumstances than previously reported; for example, the calls occurred in particular social circumstances during the preejaculatory period as well as during the postejaculatory interval. There was no consistent evidence that the emission of this call during the postejaculatory period consistently functions to keep the female away from the male. The nature and occurrence of postejaculatory ultrasonic signals showed increasing variability in successive ejaculatory series. The results of this and previous studies are interpreted within a semiotic theory of communication. The 22-kHz call is described as a message that makes available the information that the sender is in a socially depressed and withdrawn state. (14 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
87.
The fluid flow generated during the course of tapping operations from Basic Oxygen Furnaces was analyzed. Mathematical models of the filling process were developed, and their predictions checked with experimental values obtained using a one-tenth scale model of the ladle. Reasonable agreement was achieved. Based on the full scale predictions, it is suggested that the magnitude of flow velocities are technologically significant in terms of their effect on ferro-alloy immersion times and recoveries.  相似文献   
88.
Thoughts conveyed through gesture often differ from thoughts conveyed through speech. A model of the sources and consequences of such gesture–speech mismatches and their role during transitional periods in the acquisition of concepts is proposed. The model makes 2 major claims: (1) The transitional state is the source of gesture–speech mismatch. In gesture–speech mismatch, 2 beliefs are simultaneously expressed on the same problem, one in gesture and another in speech. This simultaneous activation of multiple beliefs characterizes the transitional knowledge state and creates gesture–speech mismatch. (2) Gesture–speech mismatch signals to the social world that a child is in a transitional state and is ready to learn. The child's spontaneous gestures index the zone of proximal development, thus providing a mechanism by which adults can calibrate their input to that child's level of understanding. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
89.
Examined the development of text search strategies in 144 4th, 6th, and 8th graders who were assigned to reading booklets with or without headings embedded in the reading material. Both younger and older Ss demonstrated evidence of efficient use of headings as locational aids when explicitly instructed on how to use them, and spontaneous use of headings as a search strategy was observed in half of the 4th and 6th graders and in most of the 8th graders. Ss at all levels read faster when asked to search for a specific fact than when asked to read the story line by line, indicating their awareness of when to skim. Findings suggest that by the 4th grade, children may have the cognitive skills to benefit from headings but may use these skills only in structured situations. (French abstract) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
90.
The ability of persons faking posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or closed-head injury (CHI) to respond consistently across serial testings on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory—2 (MMPI-2; J. Butcher, W. Dahlstrom, J. Graham, A. Tellegen, & B. Kaemmer, 1989) was investigated. Results showed that individuals faking PTSD obtained 2-week test–retest reliability scores comparable to individuals completing the MMPI-2 with standard instructions; individuals faking CHI obtained reliability coefficients significantly lower than individuals faking PTSD. A 3?×?2 (Response Style?×?Time) analysis of variance indicated that individuals faking a disorder obtained significantly elevated scores on validity scales sensitive to overreporting; no main effect for time was found. Results suggest that test-takers faking specific disorders can describe symptoms consistently on repeated testing and that type of disorder may affect temporal response consistency. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号