首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   230篇
  免费   10篇
电工技术   3篇
化学工业   93篇
金属工艺   3篇
建筑科学   5篇
矿业工程   1篇
能源动力   3篇
轻工业   25篇
水利工程   1篇
石油天然气   5篇
无线电   9篇
一般工业技术   27篇
冶金工业   50篇
原子能技术   7篇
自动化技术   8篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   1篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   1篇
  2018年   4篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   4篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   19篇
  2013年   11篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   7篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   16篇
  2007年   7篇
  2006年   14篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   6篇
  2003年   7篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   2篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   13篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   1篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   10篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   6篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   5篇
排序方式: 共有240条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
陈立新  游日安  樊卫  干峰  刘小伟 《核技术》2006,29(11):845-849
基于体素S因子计算个体化病例的三维剂量是目前核医学研究的一个重要方向.153Sm是目前骨转移治疗常用的核素之一.本文利用EGS4蒙特卡罗程序包构建了基于体素的三维吸收剂量分布模拟程序,计算了不同情况下153Sm的S因子,探讨了介质成分和物理密度对S因子的影响.结果表明:1)不同物理密度对S因子有明显影响;2)在相同物理密度的情况下,不同介质对S因子的影响相对较小.计算结果为基于体素S因子计算核素治疗的三维剂量提供了参考,明确了在不考虑介质和物理密度差异时S因子的误差.  相似文献   
12.
A model for estimating the upper to lower bainite transition has been developed for the iron–carbon–manganese–chromium–silicon alloy system by comparing the time required to decarburise a supersaturated bainitic ferrite platelet and that needed for the start of cementite precipitation in the ferrite. The problem is treated as a competition between the decarburisation time and the kinetics of cementite precipitation. Lower bainite is induced when the latter process is faster. The time for forming a volume fraction of 0.01 of the equilibrium amount of cementite is taken as the precipitation start time. The model was calibrated using experimental data from the iron–carbon system, and verified with the iron–carbon–manganese–molybdenum system and an experimental steel currently being developed for high-strength applications.  相似文献   
13.
14.
Behavioral responses of males from three New York races of European corn borer,Ostrinia nubilalis (Hübner), to various suspected sexpheromone-response antagonists were investigated with a flight tunnel protocol. Males from a bivoltine (Z) race and from a univoltine (Z) race utilizing a natural pheromone blend of 982 (Z)-(E)-11-tetradecenyl acetates and males from a bivoltine (E) race utilizing a 199 (Z)-(E)-11-tetradecenyl acetate ratio were flown to their natural blends and to blends containing 1 % of one of the following additional compounds: Z9–12OAc,E9–12OAc,Z9–14OAc, andE9–14OAc. In each race, the added components lowered significantly the number of individuals completing the behavioral sequence. The only exception was bivoltineE males flown to a blend withE9–12OAc added. The number completing the sequence in this case was statistically not significantly lower than the number that completed the sequence to the standard blend. In all three races, theZ9–14OAc produced the most dramatic reduction in completed flights, and in the univoltineZ race, this added component was significantly more effective in reducing completed flights than any other added component. The response to the Z9–14OAc is understandable given recently published findings of an electrophysiological study of single sensilla in the European corn borer.  相似文献   
15.
E. Dhooge and R. J. Hartsuiker (2010) reported experiments showing that picture naming takes longer with low- than high-frequency distractor words, replicating M. Miozzo and A. Caramazza (2003). In addition, they showed that this distractor-frequency effect disappears when distractors are masked or preexposed. These findings were taken to refute models like WEAVER++ (A. Roelofs, 2003) in which words are selected by competition. However, Dhooge and Hartsuiker do not take into account that according to this model, picture-word interference taps not only into word production but also into attentional processes. Here, the authors indicate that WEAVER++ contains an attentional mechanism that accounts for the distractor-frequency effect (A. Roelofs, 2005). Moreover, the authors demonstrate that the model accounts for the influence of masking and preexposure, and does so in a simpler way than the response exclusion through self-monitoring account advanced by Dhooge and Hartsuiker. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
16.
6-脱氢姜酚的制取与鉴定   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
为了获得高纯度的脱氢姜酚,以干姜粉制取的姜油树脂为原料,通过溶剂萃取、硅胶干柱层析和硅胶柱真空层析等分离纯化方法,获得了黄色粉状物,通过紫外可见光谱、红外光谱、核磁共振谱、质谱等测定证实所得粉状黄色粉状物为6-脱氢姜酚,证实了其烯醇互变异构体———6-脱氢姜二酮的存在。  相似文献   
17.
A study on the catalytic properties of properly activated hydrotalcite (HT) with special attention to the nature and amount of active sites present in this solid base catalyst has been undertaken. Only a small fraction (5%) of the available basic sites in the rehydrated calcined HT is active in liquid-phase aldol condensations. These sites exhibit high catalytic activity and are most likely localized at the edges of the HT-platelets. Besides a high activity, these modified HTs also show a high selectivity. No further condensation products other than diacetone alcohol (DAA) in the acetone self-condensation could be observed. Initial results with the citral–acetone condensation show that even at 273 K this reaction is catalyzed by modified HTs with a conversion of 65% and a selectivity of 90%, when the citral concentration is not too high (1 wt.%). At higher citral concentrations, no reaction is observed indicating a negative order in citral concentration.  相似文献   
18.
Controversy exists about whether dual-task interference from word planning reflects structural bottleneck or attentional control factors. Here, participants named pictures whose names could or could not be phonologically prepared, and they manually responded to arrows presented away from (Experiment 1), or superimposed onto, the pictures (Experiments 2 and 3); or they responded to tones (Experiment 4). Pictures and arrows/tones were presented at stimulus onset asynchronies of 0, 300, and 1,000 ms. Earlier research showed that vocal responding hampers auditory perception, which predicts earlier shifts of attention to the tones than to the arrows. Word planning yielded dual-task interference. Phonological preparation reduced the latencies of picture naming and gaze shifting. The preparation benefit was propagated into the latencies of the manual responses to the arrows but not to the tones. The malleability of the interference supports the attentional control account. This conclusion was corroborated by computer simulations showing that an extension of WEAVER++ (A. Roelofs, 2003) with assumptions about the attentional control of tasks quantitatively accounts for the latencies of vocal responding, gaze shifting, and manual responding. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
19.
B. Rapp and M. Goldrick (2000) claimed that the lexical and mixed error biases in picture naming by aphasic and nonaphasic speakers argue against models that assume a feedforward-only relationship between lexical items and their sounds in spoken word production. The author contests this claim by showing that a feedforward-only model like WEAVER++ (W. J. M. Levelt, A. Roelofs, & A. S. Meyer, 1999b) exhibits the error biases in word planning and self-monitoring. Furthermore, it is argued that extant feedback accounts of the error biases and relevant chronometric effects are incompatible. WEAVER++ simulations with self-monitoring revealed that this model accounts for the chronometric data, the error biases, and the influence of the impairment locus in aphasic speakers. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
20.
In order to study the promoting effect of polycations on the cobalt(II)phthalocyanine-tetrasodiumsulphonate (CoPc(NaSO3)4)-catalysed autoxidation of thiols, it is imperative to know the molecular weight dependence of the polymer. Monodisperse oligomers can often supply a lot of information. To elecidate the mechanism of the promoting effect of 2,4-ionene, a poly(quaternary ammonium)salt, on the CoPc(NaSO3)4-catalysed autoxidation of 2-mercaptoethanol, a series of monodisperse 2,4-ionene oligomers were prepared. Trimeric 2,4-ionenes, containing two quaternary ammonium groups separated by four methylene groups, showed a high co-catalytic activity, and active Co-aggregates were detected with visible light spectroscopy. The spectroscopic behaviour was in close agreement with that of high molecular 2,4-ionene/CoPc(NaSO3)4 complexes. In order to achieve this aggregation several oligomeric ionenes have to act concertedly. If the distance between the ionic sites is short the trimer acts as a simple salt in stablizing the aggregates. With respect to the catalytic activity the optimum polycation/catalyst ratio, expressed as the N+/Co ratio, decreased with increasing chain lenght and reached a constant level of 50 at a 2,4-ionene nonamer. For the trimers and pentamers this ratio is affected by the type of end-group. All synthesized 2,4-ionene oligomers exhibited excellent co-catalytic properties at the optimum N+/Co ratio, with maximum turnover frequencies of 4600 mol thiol/(mol Co s), i.e. 50 times higher than those obtained for the polymer-free system. In the case of monodisperse 2,4-ionene pentamer with bromo end-groups, double Michaelis–Menten kinetics were observed, as also was exhibited by high molecular weight ionene.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号