首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2541篇
  免费   207篇
  国内免费   2篇
电工技术   12篇
综合类   4篇
化学工业   734篇
金属工艺   7篇
机械仪表   40篇
建筑科学   97篇
矿业工程   1篇
能源动力   46篇
轻工业   532篇
水利工程   22篇
石油天然气   13篇
无线电   98篇
一般工业技术   296篇
冶金工业   605篇
原子能技术   12篇
自动化技术   231篇
  2024年   10篇
  2023年   25篇
  2022年   90篇
  2021年   143篇
  2020年   54篇
  2019年   65篇
  2018年   102篇
  2017年   94篇
  2016年   110篇
  2015年   73篇
  2014年   113篇
  2013年   165篇
  2012年   139篇
  2011年   167篇
  2010年   126篇
  2009年   122篇
  2008年   118篇
  2007年   103篇
  2006年   95篇
  2005年   86篇
  2004年   65篇
  2003年   56篇
  2002年   56篇
  2001年   31篇
  2000年   34篇
  1999年   28篇
  1998年   37篇
  1997年   34篇
  1996年   37篇
  1995年   25篇
  1994年   29篇
  1993年   23篇
  1992年   34篇
  1990年   23篇
  1989年   22篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   22篇
  1986年   17篇
  1985年   15篇
  1984年   22篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   23篇
  1980年   7篇
  1979年   15篇
  1978年   10篇
  1977年   11篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   6篇
  1972年   6篇
排序方式: 共有2750条查询结果,搜索用时 953 毫秒
971.
We evaluated the performance of 6 adult rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) on object permanence tasks. In Experiment 1, monkeys received search tasks that correspond to Stages 4, 5, and 6 of object permanence. Subjects were successful on tasks of visible displacements (Stages 4 and 5) but failed to find the object in invisible displacements (Stage 6). The monkeys adopted a search strategy of investigating a specific hiding location. In Experiment 2, monkeys were given a second opportunity to find the object if they investigated a location that was part of the displacement on their first search. Subjects relied on the same search strategy identified in Experiment 1. In Experiment 3, the experimenter hid the object in her hand rather than a container. The monkeys failed to recover the object, and individual differences were found in the strategies used. These results suggest that the upper limit of object permanence in rhesus monkeys is Stage 5. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
972.
Lipoproteins, including intermediate density lipoproteins and lipoprotein(a), and apolipoproteins A-I, B, C-II, C-III and E, were studied in 13 newly-diagnosed type I diabetic patients with severe insulinopenia without dehydration or acidosis. At baseline, the main finding was a significant increase in serum triglycerides due to raised triglyceride concentrations in all lipoproteins, particularly triglyceride-rich lipoproteins. Cholesterol concentrations were slightly increased in lipoproteins and led to a significant increase in serum cholesterol. Two days after the start of insulin therapy, lipoprotein profiles had normalized except for the LDL triglyceride contents, which remained significantly increased on the fifth day of treatment. No significant modifications were observed in lipoprotein(a), apolipoproteins A-I and E concentrations throughout the study. However, serum apolipoproteins B, C-II and C-III were increased at baseline and fell to normal levels 2 days after the start of insulin therapy. On the other hand, apolipoprotein C-II/C-III ratios in high and very low density lipoprotein, showed no significant differences at baseline compared with controls, suggesting that an apolipoprotein C-II deficiency or apolipoproteins Cs imbalance can be ruled out. In conclusion, significant lipoprotein abnormalities were observed in the insulin-deficient state of type I diabetes mellitus; insulin therapy normalizes the lipoprotein profile in two days, except for low density lipoprotein triglyceride contents which remain increased at the fifth day.  相似文献   
973.
The role of regulation as a mediator of the relations between maternal emotional expressivity and children's adjustment and social competence was examined when children (N=208) were 4.5 to just 8 years old (Time 1, T1) and 2 years later (Time 2, T2). At T2, as at T1, regulation mediated the relation between positive maternal emotional expressivity and children's functioning. When T1 relations and the stability of variables over time were controlled for in a structural equation model, T2 relations generally were nonsignificant, although parents' dominant negative expressivity predicted high regulation. In contrast, in regressions, the findings for parent positive expressivity, but not negative expressivity, held at T2 when T1 variables were controlled. Thus, relations for negative expressivity, but not positive expressivity, changed with age. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
974.
975.
Measured the incidence of stressful life events among 119 adolescents who had an alcohol abusing parent, were in treatment for alcohol/drug abuse, or had no personal or family history of alcohol abuse. Group scores were compared for 7 types of life events based on the Life Event Checklist (LEC) of M. D. Newcomb et al (see record 1982-20103-001). Adolescents in treatment experienced more deviance and emotional distress events whereas adolescents with an alcoholic parent reported significantly more family-related problems. Results provide evidence for the usefulness of the LEC in clinical populations of adolescents. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
976.
The Cocurrent Downflow Contactor (CDC) has been developed as a mass transfer and reactor device, with and without addition of tangential (swirl) flow, giving gas hold-up (Eg) values of 0.5–0.75, interfacial areas in the range 1000–6000 m2m?3 liquid and kLa values in the range of 0.15–1.55 s?1 for absorption using the O2/H2O system. It has been studied as a catalytic slurry reactor for the hydrogenation of (i) itaconic acid and (ii) triglycerides catalysed by Pd and Ni catalysts. The reactions were observed to be largely surface-reaction rate controlled, due to the very efficient mass transfer (kLa up to 11.75 s?1 under reaction conditions) and application of swirl flow-enhanced reaction rates. The CDC has recently been found to be capable of operating as a fixed bed reactor, thus eliminating a downstream catalyst separation problem (therefore more cost effective), and is superior in its mass transfer characteristics to other known devices. Scale-up can be undertaken without loss of performance efficiency.  相似文献   
977.
Tested 104 infants (aged 7–11 mo) for their detection of a frequency relational change of 1 semitone in a 5-note melody. In Exp I, Ss were able to discriminate contrasting melodies that differed from the background melodies by 1 semitone. In Exp II, Ss detected a semitone difference more easily when the major triad was background and a relatively uncommon triad, the augmented triad, was the contrast. In Exp III, Ss discriminated the major and minor backgrounds used in Exps I and II from contrasting variations, called inversions, that did not differ in triad quality. Results indicate that infants can respond to precise relations between the component tones of a melody based on familiar or stable structures. Findings also imply that sets of tones that are unfamiliar or unstable may present encoding or memory difficulties for infants, as has been found for children and adults. (French abstract) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
978.
People have a more differentiated cognitive representation of in-groups than of out-groups. This has led to the prediction that memory should be better for in-group information than for out-group information. However, past research has provided equivocal support for that prediction. This article advances a differential processing hypothesis that offers a solution to this paradox. The hypothesis suggests that whereas in-group information is organized by person categories, out-group information is organized through attribute categories. In-group membership alters the categorical basis of memory for person information, but these categories are not necessarily superior to the attribute categories that are used to organize out-group information. That is, both person and attribute categories elicit equal amounts of recall for the in-group and the out-group. Three experiments are reported that support the differential processing hypothesis. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
979.
Nanocomposite materials are prepared by drying a mixed colloidal suspension of film‐forming latex and silica. The dispersion state of the silica particles in the dry film controls the properties. We describe, with the help of some examples, how the colloidal stability of the particles in the mixed suspension dictates the final morphology of the dry film. Next, some recent work aimed at controlling the morphology by means of formulation additives is described. Such additives are either surfactants or polymer dispersants (block copolymers) which stabilize the particles (latex and/or silica) in the drying suspension. As a result of the mixing‐and‐drying preparation process, suitable additives are necessarily different from the conventional dispersants used in the formulation of composites prepared by means of melt‐processing. Copyright © 2004 Society of Chemical Industry  相似文献   
980.
To evaluate the potential use of mesquite (Prosopis chilensis (Mol) Stuntz) seed gum, the behaviour of the gum was studied using two extraction methods (alkaline and acid), different pH values, two concentrations (0.2 and 0.4% w/v) and different temperatures. The capacity of the gum to stabilise food foams was also evaluated. The alkaline extraction yield (24.9%) was higher than the acid extraction yield (17.7%). Owing molecular to hydrolysis caused by the acid, acid extraction resulted in a lower protein content. Gum from acid extraction had a higher viscosity than that from alkaline extraction at all temperatures investigated (10, 20, 30, 40, 50 and 60 °C). There were no significant differences between the viscosities of mucilage dispersions at the different values of pH studied (3.0, 4.0 and 5.0). The addition of extracted mesquite gum (obtained by either method and at either concentration studied) to egg white foam provided a higher stability and decreased the liquid drainage and collapse of the foam. Copyright © 2004 Society of Chemical Industry  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号