首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   10158篇
  免费   458篇
  国内免费   20篇
电工技术   101篇
综合类   4篇
化学工业   2054篇
金属工艺   146篇
机械仪表   200篇
建筑科学   636篇
矿业工程   21篇
能源动力   321篇
轻工业   894篇
水利工程   116篇
石油天然气   28篇
无线电   752篇
一般工业技术   2046篇
冶金工业   1457篇
原子能技术   37篇
自动化技术   1823篇
  2023年   75篇
  2022年   139篇
  2021年   224篇
  2020年   176篇
  2019年   219篇
  2018年   238篇
  2017年   238篇
  2016年   261篇
  2015年   228篇
  2014年   336篇
  2013年   700篇
  2012年   570篇
  2011年   816篇
  2010年   512篇
  2009年   487篇
  2008年   597篇
  2007年   525篇
  2006年   445篇
  2005年   401篇
  2004年   342篇
  2003年   318篇
  2002年   282篇
  2001年   157篇
  2000年   162篇
  1999年   143篇
  1998年   193篇
  1997年   190篇
  1996年   154篇
  1995年   128篇
  1994年   101篇
  1993年   102篇
  1992年   86篇
  1991年   65篇
  1990年   85篇
  1989年   71篇
  1988年   48篇
  1987年   69篇
  1986年   57篇
  1985年   62篇
  1984年   53篇
  1983年   74篇
  1982年   58篇
  1981年   48篇
  1980年   48篇
  1979年   40篇
  1978年   34篇
  1977年   34篇
  1976年   33篇
  1975年   28篇
  1974年   19篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
In a change detection paradigm, a target object in a natural scene either rotated in depth, was replaced by another object token, or remained the same. Change detection performance was reliably higher when a target postcue allowed participants to restrict retrieval and comparison processes to the target object (Experiment 1). Change detection performance remained excellent when the target object was not attended at change (Experiment 2) and when a concurrent verbal working memory load minimized the possibility of verbal encoding. (Experiment 3). Together, these data demonstrate that visual representations accumulate in memory from attended objects as the eyes and attention are oriented within a scene and that change blindness derives, at least in part, from retrieval and comparison failure. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
32.
Lepidium latifolium (perennial pepperweed) is a noxious Eurasian weed invading riparian and wetland areas of the western US. Effective management of Lepidium requires detailed, accurate maps of its distribution, as may be provided by remote sensing, to contain existing infestations and eradicate incipient populations. We mapped Lepidium with 3 m spatial resolution, 128-band HyMap image data in three sites of California's San Francisco Bay/Sacramento–San Joaquin Delta Estuary (Rush Ranch in Suisun Marsh and the Greater Jepson Prairie Ecosystem and the Cosumnes River Preserve in the Delta). These sites are markedly different in terms of hydrology, salinity, species composition, and structural and landscape diversity. Aggregated classification and regression tree models (CART), incorporating the results of mixture tuned matched filter (MTMF) analyses and spectral physiological indexes, were used to map Lepidium at the three sites. This approach was sufficiently flexible and robust to detect Lepidium with similar accuracies (~ 90%) at both Rush Ranch and Jepson Prairie, but was unsuccessful at Cosumnes River Preserve. Comparisons of the behavior of the MTMFs and the CARTs between sites reveal the importance of environmental context in species mapping. Rush Ranch presents the simplest conditions for mapping Lepidium: it is the wettest and least diverse site and Lepidium is spectrally distinct from co-occurring species. At Jepson Prairie, several co-occurring species closely resemble Lepidium spectrally. Nevertheless, hyperspectral data provide sufficient spectral detail to resolve Lepidium even at this challenging site, which is facilitated by phenological separation from the matrix of annual grasses. At Cosumnes River Preserve, however, Lepidium is neither spectrally nor phenologically distinct, and consequently could not be mapped successfully. Evidence suggests that the success of a remote sensing analysis declines as site complexity increases (species, structural, and landscape diversity; spectral variability; etc.), although this relationship is complex, indirect, and may be phenology-dependent.  相似文献   
33.
34.
Part 1 concerns representation: Pop-out for a target that can be distinguished from the distractors only by the relative position of its components was demonstrated, thereby showing that simple shape information is represented preattentively. Findings are discussed in terms of early- vs late-selection debate and in terms of theories of search and texture segregation. Part 2 concerns selection: Strong learning in a varied-mapping paradigm was demonstrated, and it was shown that preattentive shape information can be used for selection. Finally, an account of the learning, namely the group scale hypothesis, is suggested and a final experiment to test it is presented. Results confirm and extend the emphasis placed by J. Duncan and G. W. Humphreys's (1989) attentional engagement theory on grouping processes in visual search. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
35.
Clostridium tertium bacteremia is unusual, seen most often with gastrointestinal disease and/or neutropenia. Two cases are described. The first was a 19-yr-old female with acute leukemia, who developed gastrointestinal symptoms and C. tertium bacteremia while neutropenic. The second was a 57-yr-old female with quiescent ulcerative colitis, who presented with fever, rigors and epigastric pain. Four organisms including C. tertium were isolated from blood cultures. This patient responded to broad spectrum antimicrobial therapy, whereas the first patient required the addition of specific agents to recover. C. tertium is aerotolerant and thus can be misidentified as a Bacillus or Corynebacterium spp. Our isolates had a distinctive Gram stain morphology, were catalase negative and failed to sporulate aerobically--this aided in the recognition of this significant Gram-positive bacillus.  相似文献   
36.
37.
A New Approach to Abstract Syntax with Variable Binding   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
The permutation model of set theory with atoms (FM-sets), devised by Fraenkel and Mostowski in the 1930s, supports notions of ‘name-abstraction’ and ‘fresh name’ that provide a new way to represent, compute with, and reason about the syntax of formal systems involving variable-binding operations. Inductively defined FM-sets involving the name-abstraction set former (together with Cartesian product and disjoint union) can correctly encode syntax modulo renaming of bound variables. In this way, the standard theory of algebraic data types can be extended to encompass signatures involving binding operators. In particular, there is an associated notion of structural recursion for defining syntax-manipulating functions (such as capture avoiding substitution, set of free variables, etc.) and a notion of proof by structural induction, both of which remain pleasingly close to informal practice in computer science. Received October 2000 / Accepted in revised form April 2001  相似文献   
38.
The role of the proinflammatory cytokines interleukin-1 alpha (IL-1 alpha), interleukin-1 beta (IL-1 beta), interleukin-6 (IL-6), and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF alpha) in THC-induced catalepsy in mice was examined. Recombinant IL-1 beta (400 ng/mouse, IV) and TNF alpha (500 ng/mouse, IV) were effective in potentiating the cataleptic effect of low-dose THC (10 micrograms/mouse, IV). Recombinant IL-1 alpha and IL-6 did not potentiate catalepsy at any dose tested. Anti-IL-1 beta and anti-TNF alpha antibodies were effective in attenuating high-dose (75 micrograms/mouse) THC-induced catalepsy. Antibodies to IL-1 alpha and IL-6 had no effect on catalepsy. Early onset catalepsy (10 min postinjection) was potentiated by exogenous recombinant IL-1 beta and TNF alpha but only later catalepsy (2 h postinjection) was attenuated by antibodies to endogenous IL-1 beta or TNF alpha. This divergence of the cytokine effect suggests that these substances regulate, by different mechanisms, the early and late THC-induced cataleptic response.  相似文献   
39.
The present study characterised seven species of the Chabertiidae (Nematoda: Strongyloidea) belonging to either the subfamily Oesophagostominae (Oesophagostomum radiatum, Oesophagostomum venulosum, Oesophagostomum dentatum, Oesophagostomum quadrispinulatum, Oesophagostomum columbianum, Oesophagostomum bifurcum) or to the subfamily Chabertiinae (Chabertia ovina) by their second internal transcribed spacer rDNA sequence, assessed the extent of intraspecific variation and interspecific differences in the sequence, and inferred the phylogenetic relationship of C. ovina with respect to members of the Oesophagostominae. In both the phenetic and cladistic analyses of the sequence data, Chabertia was nested within Oesophagostomum, suggesting either that the species examined represent members of the same genus, or alternatively, that Oesophagostomum may represent more than one genus.  相似文献   
40.
Six experiments are reported that contrasted the effects of frequency and age of acquisition on written word recognition. Age of acquisition affected word-naming speed when frequency was controlled (Experiment 1), but there was no effect of frequency when age of acquisition was controlled (Experiment 2). Experiments 3 and 4 found an effect of age of acquisition upon immediate but not delayed naming speed, but no frequency effect on either immediate or delayed naming once age of acquisition was controlled. Independent effects of frequency and age of acquisition were observed in the lexical decision task (Experiments 5 and 6). Implications for theoretical accounts of word recognition and the possible roles of age of acquisition and frequency in word recognition are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号