首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   399篇
  免费   2篇
电工技术   3篇
综合类   1篇
化学工业   133篇
金属工艺   5篇
机械仪表   14篇
建筑科学   9篇
能源动力   14篇
轻工业   40篇
水利工程   3篇
石油天然气   1篇
无线电   33篇
一般工业技术   47篇
冶金工业   50篇
原子能技术   2篇
自动化技术   46篇
  2023年   6篇
  2022年   36篇
  2021年   34篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   5篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   15篇
  2014年   21篇
  2013年   23篇
  2012年   21篇
  2011年   25篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   13篇
  2008年   8篇
  2007年   19篇
  2006年   18篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   7篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   7篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1972年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1961年   1篇
排序方式: 共有401条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
82.
When asked to estimate the probability of an interpretation for an event, observers may either assess whether the event can give rise to the interpretation (an inference set) or assess whether the interpretation can give rise to the event (an explanation set). These two strategies may moderate the conjunction effects (Leddo, Abelson, & Gross, 1984)—attributors' tendency to assign lower probabilities to single-reason interpretations than to their conjunctions. Our two studies showed that explanation-set instructions (e.g., "assess the probability that the interpretation could be the reason for the event") produced stronger conjunction effects than inference-set instructions (e.g., "assess the probability that the interpretation could be inferred from the event"). This Set (inference or explanation)?×?Reason (multiple or single) interaction was not affected by whether the events involved voluntary or involuntary behavior or by whether they described events that happened or failed to happen. In a third study, we found that subjects in an inference set were more likely to report that they estimated probability of the interpretation (as opposed to probability of the behavior) than were subjects in an explanation set. The extent to which the explanation set may account for conjunction effects obtained in other studies was discussed. Possible implications and determinants of the inference-explanation distinction were also considered. (19 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
83.
84.
85.
Wireless Personal Communications - Present-day wireless methods are necessary to support a variety of higher-speed data communication facilities for its subscribers such as cloud-based video...  相似文献   
86.
Zinc oxide was deposited onto calcium carbonate precipitated using a template of polysaccharides from Ulva lactuca green seaweeds (Black Sea). The resulted composite material was characterized by X-ray diffraction, scanning electron microscopy, energy-dispersive X-ray spectroscopy, UV–vis and FTIR spectroscopy. The therapeutic effect of prepared composite material was assessed in vivo as a topical application for the burns treatment and compared with that of ZnO. An increased antioxidant activity by combining zinc oxide with calcium carbonate capped with polysaccharides from green seaweeds extract in a composite material was demonstrated.  相似文献   
87.
The Zr-2.5% Nb alloy corrosion resistance in four saline solutions with and without albumin was tested through electrochemical measurements of electrochemical impedance spectroscopy (EIS), linear polarization, and cyclic voltammetry at human body temperature.

The EIS measurements were performed at open circuit potential. The impedance spectra were recorded and the equivalent electric circuits were elaborated. From the linear polarization the Tafel slopes were recorded and the corrosion parameters were calculated. From the potentiodynamic polarization curves, the breakdown potential, the protection potential, and the protection domain were observed.

The amount of ions released from the material was quantified using the ICPMS technique and the results concerning the corrosion resistance were compared with those obtained from linear and cyclic polarization.

Before and after potentiodynamic polarization the topographic measurements were performed; the roughness at the samples surface was observed using AFM. The roughness was co-related to the contact angle measurements.  相似文献   
88.
89.
Examined developmental changes in responses to consistent and discrepant video and audio nonverbal cues. A videotaped Nonverbal Discrepancy Test was administered to 121 males and 129 females aged 9–15 yrs. The discrepancy test measured (a) decoding accuracy—the extent to which Ss are able to identify affects (positivity and dominance) from visual (facial and body) cues and audio (content-filtered and random-spliced) cues—and (b) video primacy—the extent to which Ss are more influenced by video (face or body) than by audio cues. Results show that older Ss were more accurate at decoding affects than were younger Ss, particularly dominance–submission cues. Video primacy increased with age for facial cues (but not for body cues) and for cues of positivity (but not for cues of dominance). Relative to males, younger females showed more video primacy and older females showed less video primacy, particularly for cues of dominance–submission. Relative to younger Ss, older Ss showed less video primacy in decoding extremely discrepant audio and video cues than in decoding moderately discrepant ones. The development of nonverbal sensitivity to video and audio cues is discussed. (24 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
90.
Two total mixed rations (TMR) containing different proportions of roughage neutral detergent fiber (NDF) were fed to lactating cows under Israeli summer conditions, and the effects on feeding behavior and milk production were measured. Forty-two lactating cows were divided into 2 groups fed ad libitum a TMR containing either 18% NDF of roughage origin (control group) or only 12% roughage NDF, in which the corn silage component (16.5% of dry matter [DM]) was replaced with soy hulls (experiment group). This and additional adjustments in TMR were reflected in higher net energy for lactation and in vitro digestibility of the experimental TMR. Cow behavior was investigated at the feeding lane during June 2001; about 11,000 cow visits were analyzed. Feed intake per meal and average meal duration were significantly higher in the experiment group (1.51 kg of DM per meal and 12.1 min per meal, respectively) as compared with the control group (1.22 kg of DM per meal and 9.47 min per meal, respectively). However, number of meals per day recorded in the feeding lane was significantly higher in the control group (21.0 vs. 16.6 meals/d per cow). Distribution of meals and feed intake along the day depended more on management practices, such as milking and feed dispensing times, than on feed composition or weather conditions. These differences between groups were expressed in similar daily eating duration (approximately 200 min), and because the rate of feed consumption was similar for both treatments (approximately 127 g DM/min), the daily average DM intake was also similar (25.0 to 25.7 kg). However, NDF intake was higher in the experiment group. Consequently, the average milk yield was higher in the experimental group, and production of milk fat, 4% fat-corrected milk, and economically corrected milk were significantly higher in the experiment group than in the control group. Data imply that the experimental TMR containing only 12% NDF of roughage origin is more suitable for cows under hot climate conditions compared with the control TMR.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号