全文获取类型
收费全文 | 33549篇 |
免费 | 1024篇 |
国内免费 | 72篇 |
专业分类
电工技术 | 349篇 |
综合类 | 41篇 |
化学工业 | 6789篇 |
金属工艺 | 726篇 |
机械仪表 | 589篇 |
建筑科学 | 1764篇 |
矿业工程 | 135篇 |
能源动力 | 873篇 |
轻工业 | 2371篇 |
水利工程 | 412篇 |
石油天然气 | 256篇 |
武器工业 | 2篇 |
无线电 | 2290篇 |
一般工业技术 | 5652篇 |
冶金工业 | 6968篇 |
原子能技术 | 223篇 |
自动化技术 | 5205篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 283篇 |
2021年 | 461篇 |
2020年 | 362篇 |
2019年 | 442篇 |
2018年 | 554篇 |
2017年 | 510篇 |
2016年 | 677篇 |
2015年 | 533篇 |
2014年 | 842篇 |
2013年 | 2056篇 |
2012年 | 1385篇 |
2011年 | 1817篇 |
2010年 | 1427篇 |
2009年 | 1379篇 |
2008年 | 1606篇 |
2007年 | 1665篇 |
2006年 | 1369篇 |
2005年 | 1298篇 |
2004年 | 1124篇 |
2003年 | 982篇 |
2002年 | 1031篇 |
2001年 | 644篇 |
2000年 | 630篇 |
1999年 | 583篇 |
1998年 | 676篇 |
1997年 | 590篇 |
1996年 | 550篇 |
1995年 | 577篇 |
1994年 | 486篇 |
1993年 | 548篇 |
1992年 | 468篇 |
1991年 | 300篇 |
1990年 | 404篇 |
1989年 | 396篇 |
1988年 | 368篇 |
1987年 | 360篇 |
1986年 | 361篇 |
1985年 | 430篇 |
1984年 | 419篇 |
1983年 | 370篇 |
1982年 | 292篇 |
1981年 | 322篇 |
1980年 | 294篇 |
1979年 | 301篇 |
1978年 | 267篇 |
1977年 | 256篇 |
1976年 | 239篇 |
1975年 | 216篇 |
1974年 | 198篇 |
1973年 | 201篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
22.
R. James Maguire Richard J. Tkacz David L. Sartor 《Journal of Great Lakes research》1985,11(3):320-327
Water and sediment samples from 29 locations in the Detroit and St. Clair rivers were analyzed for the highly toxic tri-n-butyltin (Bu3Sn+) species and for the less toxic di-n-butyltin (Bu2Sn2+) and n-butyltin (BuSn3+) species and inorganic tin. In general, locations sampled in the St. Clair River were less contaminated with butyltin species than those in the Detroit River. Inorganic tin and BuSn3+ were detected in over 90% of all subsurface water samples, while Bu2Sn2+ and Bu3Sn+ were detected in 45 and 28% of the same samples, respectively. The highest concentration ofBu3Sn+ in subsurface water, 5.9 × 10?10 mol Sn/L, was at the mouth of the Ecorse River, a tributary of the Detroit River. The three butyltin species and inorganic tin were also detected in 23–46% of all sediment samples. The highest concentrations of Bu3Sn+ in sediment were found close to the mouths of the River Rouge, another tributary of the Detroit River, and the Ecorse River, and were 6.2 × I0–7 and 1.7 × I0–7 mol Sn/kg dry weight, respectively, for the top 2 cm of sediment. 相似文献
23.
Burgess Curt; Tanenhaus Michael K.; Seidenberg Mark S. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1989,15(4):620
To eliminate potential "backward" priming effects, S. Glucksberg et al (see record 1986-29080-001) introduced a variant of the cross-modal lexical priming task in which subjects made lexical decisions to nonword targets that were modeled on a word related to either the contextually biased or unbiased sense of an ambiguous word. Lexical decisions to nonwords were longer than controls only when the nonword was related to the contextually biased sense of the ambiguous word, leading Glucksberg et al to conclude that context does constrain lexical access and that the multiple access pattern observed in previous studies was probably an artifact of backward priming. We did not find nonword interference when the nonword targets used by Glucksberg et al were preceded by semantically related ambiguous or unambiguous word primes. However, we did replicate their sentence context results when the ambiguous words were removed from the sentences. We conclude that the interference obtained by Glucksberg et al is due to postlexical judgments of the congruence of the sentence context and the target, not to context constraining lexical access. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
24.
Previous studies have shown that bizarre and common images produce equivalent levels of recall in unmixed-list designs. Using unmixed lists, we tested the view that bizarre images would be less susceptible than common images to common sources of interference. In all experiments, subjects imaged a list of either bizarre or common sentences and then performed some kind of interfering task before recalling the initial list of sentences. Experiment 1 showed that bizarre images were better accessed than common images after imaging an intervening list of common sentences. Also, components of common images tended to be better recalled than those of bizarre images after imaging an intervening list of bizarre sentences. Experiments 2a and 2b showed that interfering tasks consisting of studying lists of common concrete nouns did not differentially affect memory for bizarre and common images. In Experiment 3, labeling and imaging an interfering list of common pictures produced higher recall of bizarre images. Generally, bizarre images appeared to be less susceptible than common images to interference from certain types of common encodings. Importantly, the superior recall of bizarre images was always due to greater image (sentence) access, whereas higher recall of common images was associated with greater recovery of the image (sentence) constituents. Explanation of the precise pattern of results requires consideration of the distinctive properties of bizarre images. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
25.
26.
Goldman Susan R.; Mertz Davis L.; Pellegrino James W. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1989,81(4):481
Practice on addition basic facts and changes in solution strategies were examined in 3rd- and 4th-grade students, practicing over 3 months. Response latencies decreased, indicating positive practice effects. Each of 3 student groups demonstrated different solution–strategy mixtures. Different mixtures led to different practice effects, indexed by shifts to more efficient counting strategies and more direct retrieval. Expectations about effects of practice and instructional strategies for optimizing practice must be sensitive to a child's actual strategy pattern. Most students had decreasing latency practice functions. However, quantitative functions fit to the practice session data indicated a high proportion of cases where the latency pattern over sessions was nonmonotonic. Trends over a small number of practice sessions may be inadequate for decisions about the utility of further practice, because such data may poorly represent long-term, cumulative effects on modifying strategy usage. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
27.
28.
James G. Strathman 《Journal of Urban Affairs》1992,14(1):79-92
ABSTRACT: A ranking of 33 urban studies and urban affairs programs in the US was developed from peer ratings obtained in a program survey and from citation activity reported in the Social Sciences Citation Index over the 1986–1989 period. A breakdown of citations by broadly defined subject areas is presented. The survey revealed a difference between Ph.D. and MUS/MA programs in the perceived relative importance of peer evaluation and citations as rating criteria. The survey results also indicate that the degree of familiarity with other programs is lower than what has been observed in most social science disciplines. 相似文献
29.
George Chryssolouris James E. Pierce Kristian Dicke 《International Journal of Flexible Manufacturing Systems》1992,4(3-4):309-330
This paper introduces a generic decision-making framework for assigning resources of a manufacturing system to production tasks. Resources are broadly defined production units, such as machines, human operators, or material handling vehicles; and tasks are activities performed by resources. In the specific context of FMS, resources correspond to individual machines; tasks correspond to operations to be performed on parts. The framework assumes a hierarchical structure of the system and calls for the execution of four consecutive steps to make a decision for the assignment of a resource to a task. These steps are 1) establishment of decision-making criteria, 2) formation of alternative assignments, 3) estimation of the consequences of the assignments, and 4) selection of the best alternative assignment. This framework has been applied to an existing FMS as an operational policy that decides what task will be executed on which resource of this FMS. Simulation runs provide some initial results of the application of this policy. It is shown that the policy provides flexibility in terms of system performance and computational effort. 相似文献
30.
Mark S. Drew 《Solar Energy》1985,35(3):291-293