排序方式: 共有44条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
21.
Mariam N. Ismail Naftali D. Fraiman Dennis M. Callahan Jr. Gamze Gursoy Edward Viveiros Onnaz Ozkanat Zhaoxia Ji Ronald J. Willey Juliusz Warzywoda Albert Sacco Jr. 《Microporous and mesoporous materials》2009,120(3):454-459
Microporous vanadosilicate AM-6 has been hydrothermally synthesized for the first time in the absence of titanosilicate ETS-10 seeds. High purity (95–100% AM-6) products were crystallized under static conditions at 430–503 K using tetramethylammonium hydroxide (TMAOH) or tetramethylammonium bromide (TMABr) as a structure-directing agent in mixtures with molar compositions xNa2O:1.3K2O:ySiO2:0.5V2O5:wTMA:zH2O, where x = 4.5 or 5.6, y = 4.3–5.5, w = 2.0–3.0, and z = 206–214. The size (250 nm to 9 μm range), truncated square bipyramidal morphology (varied from square cuboid to thin (thickness <100–200 nm) tetragonal platy), and surface irregularities/faulting of AM-6 crystals (Si/V = 5.0 ± 0.5) can be controlled by varying the molar composition or initial pH of synthesis mixture, and crystallization temperature. Structure-directing agent can be removed from the pores with little or no adverse effects on crystallinity of AM-6 by treatment with gaseous ammonia at elevated temperatures. UV–vis and FTIR analyses suggest two (V4+, V5+) oxidation states of vanadium in the AM-6 framework. 相似文献
22.
Raz Naftali; Rodrigue Karen M.; Kennedy Kristen M.; Land Susan 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》2009,23(1):105
Several single nucleotide polymorphisms have been linked to neural and cognitive variation in healthy adults. We examined contribution of three polymorphisms frequently associated with individual differences in cognition (Catechol-O-Methyl-Transferase Val158Met, Brain-Derived-Neurotrophic-Factor Val66Met, and Apolipoprotein E ?4) and a vascular risk factor (hypertension) in a sample of 189 volunteers (age 18-82). Genotypes were determined from buccal culture samples, and cognitive performance was assessed in 4 age-sensitive domains?fluid intelligence, executive function (inhibition), associative memory, and processing speed. We found that younger age and COMT Met/Met genotype, associated with low COMT activity and higher prefrontal dopamine content, were independently linked to better performance in most of the tested domains. Homozygotes for Val allele of BDNF polymorphism exhibited better associative memory and faster speed of processing than the Met allele carriers, with greater effect for women and persons with hypertension. Carriers of ApoE ε4 allele evidenced steeper age-related increase in costs of Stroop color interference, but showed no negative effects on memory. The findings indicate that age-related cognitive performance is differentially affected by distinct genetic factors and their interactions with vascular health status. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
23.
A New Nonparametric Pairwise Clustering Algorithm Based on Iterative Estimation of Distance Profiles
Dubnov Shlomo El-Yaniv Ran Gdalyahu Yoram Schneidman Elad Tishby Naftali Yona Golan 《Machine Learning》2002,47(1):35-61
We present a novel pairwise clustering method. Given a proximity matrix of pairwise relations (i.e. pairwise similarity or dissimilarity estimates) between data points, our algorithm extracts the two most prominent clusters in the data set. The algorithm, which is completely nonparametric, iteratively employs a two-step transformation on the proximity matrix. The first step of the transformation represents each point by its relation to all other data points, and the second step re-estimates the pairwise distances using a statistically motivated proximity measure on these representations. Using this transformation, the algorithm iteratively partitions the data points, until it finally converges to two clusters. Although the algorithm is simple and intuitive, it generates a complex dynamics of the proximity matrices. Based on this bipartition procedure we devise a hierarchical clustering algorithm, which employs the basic bipartition algorithm in a straightforward divisive manner. The hierarchical clustering algorithm copes with the model validation problem using a general cross-validation approach, which may be combined with various hierarchical clustering methods.We further present an experimental study of this algorithm. We examine some of the algorithm's properties and performance on some synthetic and standard data sets. The experiments demonstrate the robustness of the algorithm and indicate that it generates a good clustering partition even when the data is noisy or corrupted. 相似文献
24.
Untreated hypertension negatively affects brain anatomy and cognitive functions, but the effects of medically treated hypertension are unclear. The authors compared 40 middle-age and older adults diagnosed with essential hypertension to demographically matched normotensive peers. Volumes of 7 brain regions and deep and periventricular white-matter hyperintensities (WMH) were measured on magnetic resonance imaging scans. Performance in 4 cognitive domains (perseveration, working memory, fluid reasoning, and vocabulary knowledge) was evaluated. Persons with hypertension had smaller prefrontal cortex and underlying white matter volumes and increased frontal WMH. No group differences were found in other examined brain regions. Among examined cognitive variables, hypertensive patients committed significantly more perseverative errors. Thus, even controlled hypertension may be associated with deficits in brain structure and cognition, warranting further study. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
25.
The neuroimaging literature on structural brain abnormalities in the major psychoses is quantitatively reviewed. The mean effect size for studies of lateral ventriculomegaly in schizophrenia (d?=?.70) corresponded to 43% nonoverlap between the distributions of schizophrenics and control Ss. Planimetry yielded larger effects than linear methods of ventricular size estimation. Although enlargement of the third ventricle was comparable to that of lateral ventriculomegaly (d?=?.66), it was found to be significantly greater after differences in measurement method were taken into account. The average cumulative length of hospitalization, adjusted for patients' age and duration of illness, predicted ventriculomegaly in schizophrenia. Studies on schizophrenia and affective disorder differed neither in the extent of reported ventriculomegaly nor in the amount of "cortical atrophy." (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
26.
The authors investigated age-related slowing of information processing in mental imagery tasks. Eighty-five healthy adults (ages 18 to 77) performed a visual, sensorimotor, reaction-time task; a visual-perceptual choice reaction task; and 3 mental imagery tasks that varied in apparent difficulty and involved stimuli at 2 levels of graphic complexity. Age was associated with prolongation of response time across all tasks and both levels of stimulus complexity. Accuracy of response was adversely affected by increase in stimulus complexity in all tasks, whereas it was negatively related to age only on the tasks with substantial mental imagery requirements. Slowing of information processing and reduction in accuracy were mediated by declines in working memory but not by decrease of sensorimotor speed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
27.
Raz Naftali; Gunning-Dixon Faith M.; Head Denise; Dupuis James H.; Acker James D. 《Canadian Metallurgical Quarterly》1998,12(1):95
To examine putative brain substrates of cognitive functions differentially affected by age, the authors measured the volume of cortical regions and performance on tests of executive functions, working memory, explicit memory, and priming in healthy adults (18-77 years old). The results indicate that shrinkage of the prefrontal cortex mediates age-related increases in perseveration. The volume of visual processing areas predicted performance on nonverbal working memory tasks. Contrary to the hypotheses, in the examined age range, the volume of limbic structures was unrelated to any of the cognitive functions; verbal working memory, verbal explicit memory, and verbal priming were independent of cortical volumes. Nevertheless, among the participants aged above 60, reduction in the volume of limbic structures predicted declines in explicit memory. Chronological age adversely influenced all cognitive indices, although its effects on priming were only indirect, mediated by declines in verbal working memory. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
28.
Memory for facts, source, and context: Can frontal lobe dysfunction explain age-related differences?
Age-related differences in memory for facts, source, and contextual details were examined in healthy young (aged 18–35 yrs) and old (aged 65–80 yrs) volunteers. In all tested memory functions, decline over time was greater in the elderly than in the young. A time-dependent increase in the prevalence of source amnesia errors was clearly associated with old age. Contrary to several recent reports, measures of frontal lobe functions did not predict source memory. Nevertheless, some of these putative frontal function measures were related to memory for contextual details. The number of perseverative responses on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test was inversely related to performance on both factual and contextual memory tests, but the association with contextual memory was stronger. Difficulties with response selection on a Stroop task predicted poor contextual memory in young but not in old adults. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
29.
Reports several errors in the original article by S. Briggs, N. Raz and W. Marks (Psychology & Aging, 1999 (Sep.), Vol.14(3), 427–435). None of the errors alter the conclusions of the article. (The following abstract of this article originally appeared in record 1999-11243-006). The authors investigated age-related slowing of information processing in mental imagery tasks. Eighty-five healthy adults (ages 18 to 77) performed a visual, sensorimotor, reaction-time task; a visual-perceptual choice reaction task; and 3 mental imagery tasks that varied in apparent difficulty and involved stimuli at 2 levels of graphic complexity. Age was associated with prolongation of response time across all tasks and both levels of stimulus complexity. Accuracy of response was adversely affected by increase in stimulus complexity in all tasks, whereas it was negatively related to age only on the tasks with substantial mental imagery requirements. Slowing of information processing and reduction in accuracy were mediated by declines in working memory but not by decrease of sensorimotor speed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
30.
The application of the self-calibration method as a means of increasing the accuracy of spectral response, SR, and internal quantum efficiency, Q(λ), measurements is discussed. The principle of the method is the precise calculation of Q(λm) of a test cell for light at λm≈0.8 μm, where the response is weakly dependent on the emitter and base parameters. The ratio of the calculated and measured Q(λm) values gives the corresponding factor for shifting the experimental spectral response curve. The sequence of calculations is described, and an algorithm of the necessary operations for a computer is developed. Several examples of the use of the self-calibration method for correction of SR measurements of solar cells with low shunt resistance demonstrate its very high effectiveness. The corrected Q(λ) values follow the respective actual data with very high accuracy even when the measured SR is decreased by factor 2–3 due to low shunt resistance of the solar cell. 相似文献