首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   473篇
  免费   26篇
电工技术   2篇
化学工业   117篇
金属工艺   3篇
机械仪表   1篇
建筑科学   19篇
矿业工程   1篇
能源动力   9篇
轻工业   87篇
水利工程   3篇
无线电   18篇
一般工业技术   63篇
冶金工业   112篇
原子能技术   1篇
自动化技术   63篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   10篇
  2020年   7篇
  2019年   12篇
  2018年   13篇
  2017年   11篇
  2016年   8篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   14篇
  2013年   31篇
  2012年   24篇
  2011年   34篇
  2010年   20篇
  2009年   20篇
  2008年   19篇
  2007年   29篇
  2006年   15篇
  2005年   16篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   17篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   12篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   16篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   5篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   5篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   10篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   6篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1967年   2篇
排序方式: 共有499条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
61.
To define the capacity to organize verbal learning after frontal lobe injury, 32 patients with stable frontal lesions were evaluated with list learning tasks. Lesion size and site were determined from CAT or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans. The lists were contructed to assess several potential organizational processes. A mild deficit in recognition performance was observed in patients with left frontal lesions accompanied by residual minor language impairments or in patients with septal lesions. Patients with frontal lesions also had a verbal recall deficit that was related to several factors: poor higher order organization of learning, independent of lesion size or site; a selective secondary memory impairment, associated with a language deficit and size of the lesion in the left frontal region; and excess intralist repetitions in patients with right frontal lesions. Impaired list learning after frontal lesions involves several independent psychological processes. This functional heterogeneity is based on regional anatomical specialization and dissociation of task processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
62.
63.
In the domain of personality, a variety of stereotypes about old age are found. Unfortunately, the methodologies of previous studies have not dealt with a potential confound as to whether attitudes are a function of the aging process or of perceptions of generations. The present study was designed as a first step toward distinguishing between these possibilities. Young adults rated one target generation (i.e., today's 22-, 41-, or 69-year-olds) at one age period (i.e., as 22-, 41-, or 69-year-olds) on a series of personality attributes. Results demonstrated that attitudes (i.e., perceptions of personality) varied more often as a function of target generation, reflecting a general tendency for young adults to view other generations in a different fashion from the way they view their own. Overall, attitudes toward the older generation or age period were more positive than negative. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
64.
65.
Four studies with 24 19–35 yr old French-speaking adults examined the localization of perceptual form–color interference in a Stroop task with drawings. Results indicate that the time required to name the color of an incongruently colored object, such as a blue banana, was significantly longer than that for a neutral object (a red book). The same effect was found for identifying the color of the ink in which the names of these observed objects were printed. However, there did not appear to be a common semantic component in the interference observed in the 2 tasks. Extended practice on the task with the names of incongruently colored objects did not transfer to the task with the drawings of the same objects. The time to discriminate the colors of 2 drawings of incongruently colored objects was significantly longer than that for 2 drawings of neutral objects, suggesting that a perceptual inhibition occurred for incongruently colored objects (objects for which color was normally an integral part of the stimulus) that created a delay in perceiving the object's color. This contextual interference may be the complementary phenomenon to the object superiority effect. (31 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
66.
This paper presents a new family of 2D curves and its extension to 3D surfaces, respectively, calledrationconics andratioquadrics that have been designed as alternatives to the well-known superconics and superquadrics. This new model is intended as an improvement to the original one on three main points: first, it involves lower computation cost and provides better numerical robustness; second, it offers higher order continuities (C 1/G 2 orC 2/G 2 instead ofC 0/G 0); and third, it provides a greater variety of shapes for the resulting curves and surfaces. All these improvements are obtained by replacing the signed power function involved in the formulation of superconics and superquadrics by linear or quadratic rational polynomials.  相似文献   
67.
We studied how specific motivational processes are related to the salience of mastery and performance goals in actual classroom settings. One hundred seventy-six students attending a junior high/high school for academically advanced students were randomly selected from one of their classes and responded to a questionnaire on their perceptions of the classroom goal orientation, use of effective learning strategies, task choices, attitudes, and causal attributions. Students who perceived an emphasis on mastery goals in the classroom reported using more effective strategies, preferred challenging tasks, had a more positive attitude toward the class, and had a stronger belief that success follows from one's effort. Students who perceived performance goals as salient tended to focus on their ability, evaluating their ability negatively and attributing failure to lack of ability. The pattern and strength of the findings suggest that the classroom goal orientation may facilitate the maintenance of adaptive motivation patterns when mastery goals are salient and are adopted by students. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
68.
This study applied self-discrepancy theory (E. T. Higgins, R. N. Bond, R. Klein, & T. Strauman, 1986) to the study of the maternal self-concept and interrole conflict for married professional women with children. The participants, 103 married professional women with young children, completed questionnaires. Each of the 4 self-discrepancy variables was a positive predictor of positive interrole conflict and, with one exception (actual–own:ought–own self-discrepancy), a negative predictor of coping effectiveness. Both structural role redefinition and superwoman coping strategies were positively related to coping effectiveness. Use of the structural role redefinition strategy was positively associated with work support, spousal support, and social support. Work support was negatively related to anxiety and depression, but no significant relations were found between spousal support and other variables. In regression models, self-discrepancy was related to both anxiety and depression. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
69.
Negatively valued masculinity (M–) and femininity (F–) personality scales were developed to supplement the positively valued Masculinity (M+) and Femininity (F+) scales of J. T. Spence and R. L. Helmreich"s (1978) Personal Attributes Questionnaire. (M–) consisted of traits that had been judged to be (a) more typical of males than females, (b) undesirable in both sexes, and (c) agentic or instrumental in content. Two (F–) scales were developed, both containing stereotypically feminine, undesirable traits, one set of traits referring to communionlike characteristics and the other to verbal passive–aggressive qualities. In 220 male and 363 female undergraduates significant sex differences in the predicted direction were found on all scales. In both sexes, low and nonsignificant correlations were found between parallel positive and negative scales, but highly significant negative correlations were found between positive and negative cross-sex scales. Findings provide additional evidence for the multidimensionality of masculinity and femininity. Scores on a self-esteem measure were positively correlated with M+ and F+, uncorrelated with M–, and negatively correlated with the F– scales. Different patterns were associated with 2 types of problem behaviors. Neuroticism was most highly correlated (negatively) with M+, and acting out behavior was most strongly correlated (positively) with M–. (14 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
70.
Twelve-thirteen day old rats were given 1-14C linolenic acid by intraperitoneal injection. Fatty acids were isolated from the brains of animals sacrificed at the end of 8 and 48 hr and 15 and 45 days. Eight hr after the tracer, radioactivity was found neither in 18∶3 nor its endproduct, 22∶6, and palmitate was the most highly radioactive component. At longer intervals, 22∶6 seemed to retain much of the radioactivity, whereas palmitate showed a precipitous decline in radioactivity. Initial oxidation of linolenate and sparing of the linolenate complexed with polar lipids are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号