首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4839篇
  免费   261篇
  国内免费   12篇
电工技术   36篇
综合类   5篇
化学工业   1020篇
金属工艺   96篇
机械仪表   103篇
建筑科学   244篇
矿业工程   8篇
能源动力   155篇
轻工业   462篇
水利工程   51篇
石油天然气   32篇
武器工业   2篇
无线电   422篇
一般工业技术   895篇
冶金工业   813篇
原子能技术   29篇
自动化技术   739篇
  2024年   15篇
  2023年   56篇
  2022年   89篇
  2021年   151篇
  2020年   118篇
  2019年   110篇
  2018年   141篇
  2017年   126篇
  2016年   157篇
  2015年   118篇
  2014年   158篇
  2013年   274篇
  2012年   275篇
  2011年   339篇
  2010年   240篇
  2009年   267篇
  2008年   298篇
  2007年   282篇
  2006年   228篇
  2005年   238篇
  2004年   170篇
  2003年   147篇
  2002年   151篇
  2001年   75篇
  2000年   65篇
  1999年   57篇
  1998年   62篇
  1997年   58篇
  1996年   67篇
  1995年   49篇
  1994年   48篇
  1993年   50篇
  1992年   37篇
  1991年   26篇
  1990年   38篇
  1989年   38篇
  1988年   31篇
  1987年   41篇
  1986年   38篇
  1985年   32篇
  1984年   25篇
  1983年   27篇
  1982年   25篇
  1981年   13篇
  1980年   13篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   13篇
  1976年   6篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   4篇
排序方式: 共有5112条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
[Correction Notice: An erratum for this article was reported in Vol 102(2) of Psychological Bulletin (see record 2008-10756-001). There was an error in the last line of Table 1, which appears on page 137. The corrected version is presented in the erratum.] Develops formulas for calculating the average ability of job candidates actually selected when the proportion of initial offers accepted is less than 100%. Three cases are considered: (a) those in which offers are declined at random, (b) those in which offers are declined by the highest-scoring applicants, and (c) those in which test scores are related to the probability of accepting an offer. It is shown that under realistic circumstances, utility formulas currently used could overestimate utility gains by 30–80% and that, under extreme circumstances, overestimation could be much worse. Such overestimation occurs even when tests are used only for screening rather than for rank-ordering candidates. (16 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
942.
We estimated the average reliability, stability, and validity of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), Rorschach Inkblot Test, and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) from articles published in the Journal of Personality Assessment and the Journal of Clinical Psychology between 1970 and 1981. Following standard psychometric theory, reliability values exceeded stability values, which exceeded validity values. Validity studies based on theory, prior research, or both showed greater effects than did studies lacking a theoretical or empirical rationale. In general, the reliability and stability of all three tests were acceptable and approximately equivalent. The convergent-validity estimates for the Rorschach and MMPI were not significantly different, but both these estimates were lower than the estimate for the WAIS. It appears that both the MMPI and Rorschach can be considered to have adequate psychometric properties if used for the purpose for which they were designed and validated. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
943.
Tension and vascular headache patients, initially treated with biofeedback and/or relaxation training in either a minimal therapist contact protocol (3 visits) or an intensive individual protocol (10 or 16 visits) were followed-up prospectively for 2 years. In the first study, for the first 6 months of follow-up, half of all patients continued to keep headache diaries and were seen monthly and the other half had only minimal contact. The results at 1-year follow-up, based on 4 weeks of daily headache diaries, revealed equally good maintenance from both treatment protocols and from both follow-up conditions. In Study 2, we found that patients remained improved over pretreatment baseline levels at the 2-year follow-up regardless of initial treatment intensity. Approximately three quarters of vascular patients who were initially improved at posttreatment remained improved at 2 years. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
944.
Conducted an intensive, case-by-case assessment of life-styles of a sample of religious students. We identified differing styles of religiousness and made comparisons by means of tests and interviews between subgroups whose subjects manifested differing religious life-styles. Those subjects with continuous religious development and mild religious experiences appeared to be healthier than those with discontinuous development and intense religious experiences; however, intense religious experiences tended to enhance the adjustment of those who experienced them. There was no evidence in the group as a whole for an overall negative or positive correlation between religiousness and mental health, but some modes of religious involvement appeared to be related to disturbance, whereas other modes appeared to be related to enhanced stability and resilience. Because causality in these relations remains uncertain, we generate hypotheses concerning further studies of life-styles and adjustment. We also discuss implications for student counseling and development. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
945.
The mechanical properties of a heat of HP9-4-20 steel in the as-quenched condition have been determined as a function of austenitizing temperature. The mechanical properties determined included smooth axisymmetric and plane strain tensile properties, the Charpy impact energy, and the fracture initiation toughness. Austenitizing temperatures of 840 °, 1050 °, and 1200 ° were utilized. The effects of austenitizing temperature on the characteristics of the primary and secondary particle distributions and on other microstructural features were quantified. For this material the results suggested that the increased fracture toughness resulting from higher austenitizing temperatures was due to both an increase in the primary particle spacing and to increased resistance to void sheet formation. The increased resistance to void sheet resistance is attributed to changes in the characteristics of the secondary particle distributions.  相似文献   
946.
The results of two experiments showed that an illusion of memory can be produced by unconscious perception. In a first phase of those experiments, a long list of words was presented for study. For the test of recognition memory given in the second phase of each experiment, presentation of a "context" word preceded that of most recognition test words. Ss were to judge whether or not the test words had been presented during the earlier study phase of the experiment. Effects of a context word on this recognition memory decision were opposite when Ss were aware vs. unaware of its presentation. For example, as compared to a condition in which no context word was presented, the probability of false recognition was increased when Ss were unaware but decreased when Ss were aware of the presentation of a context word that matched the recognition test word. Results are discussed in terms of unconscious influences on an attribution process. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
947.
We examined the influence of visual information on a decision task that was administered to an individual with monocular visual conversion disorder. Findings indicated that his performance was influenced by the visual information and by motivation instructions. The findings are discussed in terms of a model of hysterical blindness that recognizes the interplay of cognitive and motivational processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
948.
Two possible explanations were examined for finding that observed correlations among dimensions are sometimes greater than and sometimes less than true intercorrelations: It can be explained by (a) variation in true halo or (b) in terms of individual differences in perceived similarities among dimensions. Subjects (N?=?145) viewed videotapes that exhibited either high or low true halo, rated them, and rated similarities among dimensions. True halo level accounted for significant variance in rating intercorrelation, but perceived similarity did not. Results suggest that neither explanation is adequate to account for the finding that observed intercorrelations can either overestimate or underestimate true intercorrelations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
949.
Previous research regarding the effects of positive life events on physical health has been inconclusive. We tested the hypothesis that positive life events have a detrimental effect on health only among people with negative self-views. This prediction derives from an identity disruption model of stress, which holds that an accumulation of life events that are inconsistent with the self-concept leads to physical illness. To test the hypothesis, we conducted 2 prospective studies in which positive life events and self-esteem were used to predict the development of illness over time. In accordance with predictions, both studies showed that desirable life changes were associated with increases in illness only among Ss with low self-esteem; among Ss with high self-esteem, positive life events were linked to better health. Implications for understanding the manner in which life events affect health are considered. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
950.
An intervention involving 60 incarcerated juvenile delinquents (ages 14–18 yrs) yielded significant gains in sociomoral reasoning that were attributable to a dilemma-discussions treatment. The treatment consisted of 8 weekly small-group discussions of sociomoral dilemmas. Ss were pretested on the Sociomoral Reflection Measure (SRM) and the Dilemma Decisions Survey and were assigned to either consensus dilemma-discussion, nonconsensus dilemma-discussion, or no-discussion groups. Pretest results show that Ss initially differed in both sociomoral stage and dilemma opinion. Of the dilemma-discussions Ss, 87.5% were pretested at Modal Stage 2 on the SRM and shifted to Modal Stage 3 on the posttest, whereas only 14.3% of the Modal Stage 2 control Ss did so. Pretest Modal Stage 3 Ss remained at Stage 3 on the posttest. Across the experimental groups, no difference was found between groups where a consensus was required for the discussion and groups having no consensus requirement. Investigations of the possible in-program and postrelease behavioral ramifications of the dilemma-discussions treatment are recommended. (40 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号